Skip to main content
×
×
Home
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 1
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Hilkenmeier, Frederic Scharlau, Ingrid Weiß, Katharina and Olivers, Christian N. L. 2012. The dynamics of prior entry in serial visual processing. Visual Cognition, Vol. 20, Issue. 1, p. 48.

    ×
  • Print publication year: 2010
  • Online publication date: October 2010

30 - Priming and retouch in flash-lag and other phenomena of the streaming perceptual input

from Part V - Space–time and awareness
Summary

Summary

When rapidly successive objects or object replicas are presented as sensory streams, a stimulus within a stream is perceptually facilitated relative to an otherwise identical stimulus not within the stream. Experiments on perceptual latency priming and flash-lag have convincingly shown this. Unfortunately, no consensus exists on what is (are) the mechanism(s) responsible for in-stream facilitation. Here, I discuss several alternative explanations: perceptual extrapolation of change in the specific properties of continuous stimulation, time-saving for target processing due to the early microgenetic/formation stages for target being completed on pretarget in-stream items, control of focused selective attention by the onsets of stimulus input, and preparation of the nonspecific perceptual retouch by the preceding nontarget input in stream for the succeeding target input in stream. Revisions are outlined to overcome the explanatory difficulties that the retouch theory has encountered in the face of new phenomena of perceptual dissociation.

Introduction

Objects that do not occur in isolation are processed differently compared to when they appear as separate entities. If we compare the visual latency of an object presented alone with the latency of its replica that is presented after another object (which is presented nearby in space and time), we see that the object that comes after having been primed by other input achieves awareness faster (Neumann 1982; Bachmann 1989; Scharlau & Neumann 2003a & b; Scharlau 2004). In a typical experiment, a visual prime stimulus is presented, followed by another stimulus that acts as a backward mask to the prime.

Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

Space and Time in Perception and Action
  • Online ISBN: 9780511750540
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511750540
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×
References
Alais, D., & Burr, D. (2003). The “flash-lag” effect occurs in audition and cross-modally. Curr Biol 13: 59–63.
Arrighi, R., Alais, D., & Burr, D. (2005). Neural latencies do not explain the auditory and audio-visual flash-lag effect. Vision Res 45: 2917–2925.
Baars, B. (1995). Surprisingly small subcortical structures are needed for the state of waking consciousness, while cortical projection areas seem to provide perceptual contents of consciousness. Conscious Cogn 4: 159–162.
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the Theater of Consciousness: The Workspace of the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bachmann, T. (1984). The process of perceptual retouch: nonspecific afferent activation dynamics in explaining visual masking. Percept Psychophys 35: 69–84.
Bachmann, T. (1989). Microgenesis as traced by the transient paired-forms paradigm. Acta Psychologica 70: 3–17.
Bachmann, T. (1994). Psychophysiology of Visual Masking: The Fine Structure of Conscious Experience. Commack, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
Bachmann, T. (1997). Visibility of brief images: The dual-process approach. Conscious Cogn 6: 491–518.
Bachmann, T. (1999). Twelve spatiotemporal phenomena, and one explanation. In G., Aschersleben, T., Bachmann, & J., Müsseler (eds.), Cognitive Contributions to the Perception of Spatial and Temporal Events. (173–206). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bachmann, T. (2000). Microgenetic Approach to the Conscious Mind. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Bachmann, T., Luiga, I., Põder, E., & Kalev, K. (2003). Perceptual acceleration of objects in stream: evidence from flash-lag displays. Conscious Cogn 12: 279–297.
Bachmann, T., & Oja, A. (2003). Flash-lag without change in feature space is alive and well at late intervals after stream onset. Perception 32(Suppl.): 126–127.
Bachmann, T., & Põder, E. (2001). Change in feature space is not necessary for the flash-lag effect. Vision Res 41: 1103–1106.
Bachmann, T., Põder, E., & Luiga, I. (2004). Illusory reversal of temporal order: the bias to report a dimmer stimulus as the first. Vision Res 44: 241–246.
Bachmann, T., & Sikka, P. (2005). Perception of successive targets presented in invariant-item streams. Acta Psychologica 120: 19–34.
Baldo, M. V. C., & Klein, S. A. (1995). Extrapolation or attention shift? Nature 378: 565–566.
Baldo, M. V. C., Kihara, A. H., Namba, J., & Klein, S. A. (2002). Evidence for an attentional component of the perceptual misalignment between moving and flashing stimuli. Perception 31: 17–30.
Bogen, J. E. (1995). On the neurophysiology of consciousness: I. An overview. Conscious Cogn 4: 52–62.
Bonneh, Y., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D. (2001). Motion-induced blindness in normal observers. Nature 411: 798–801.
Börgers, C., Epstein, S., & Kopell, N. J. (2005). Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: a computational study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 7002–7007.
Bregman, A. S., & Rudnicky, A. J. (1975). Auditory segregation: stream or streams? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 104: 263–267.
Breitmeyer, B. G., & Öğmen, H. (2006). Visual Masking: Time Slices Through Conscious and Unconscious Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2000). Motion extrapolation is not responsible for the flash-lag effect. Vision Res 40: 1645–1648.
Brooks, B., & Jung, R. (1973). Neuronal physiology of the visual cortex. In R., Jung (ed.), Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Vol. VII/.3 (325–440). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Cai, R., & Schlag, J. (2001). A new form of illusory conjunction between color and shape [Abstract]. J Vis 1: 127a.
Carretie, L., Hinojosa, J. A., Mercado, F., & Tapia, M. (2005). Cortical response to subjectively unconscious danger. NeuroImage 24: 615–623.
Crick, F. (1984). Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 81: 4586–4590.
Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1998). Constraints on cortical and thalamic projections: the no-strong-loops hypothesis. Nature 391: 245–250.
Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci 6: 119–126.
DiLollo, V., Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. J Exp Psychol Gen 129: 481–507.
Eagleman, D. M. (2010). How does the timing of neural signals map onto the timing of perception? In R., Nijhawan & B., Khurana (eds.), Space and Time in Perception and Action (216–231). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Eagleman, D. M., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science 287: 2036–2038.
Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1972). Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays. Percep Psychophys 12: 201–204.
Herrmann, C. S., & Mecklinger, A. (2001). Gamma activity in human EEG is related to high-speed memory comparisons during object selective attention. Vis Cogn 8: 593–608.
Hochstein, S., & Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron 36: 791–804.
Hommuk, K., Bachmann, T., & Oja, A. (2008). Precuing an isolated stimulus temporarily outweighs in-stream stimulus facilitation. J Gen Psychol 135: 167–181.
Jaśkowski, P., van der Lubbe, R., Schlotterbeck, E., & Verleger, R. (2002). Traces left on visual selective attention by stimuli that are not consciously identified. Psychol Sci 13: 48–54.
John, E. R. (2005). From synchronous neuronal discharges to subjective awareness? In S., Laureys (ed.), Progress in Brain Research. 150: 143–171.
Kanwisher, N. (2001). Neural events and perceptual awareness. Cognition 79: 89–113.
Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (2004). Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia 42: 831–835.
Khurana, B., & Nijhawan, R. (1995). Extrapolation or attention shift? Nature 378: 566.
Khurana, B., Watanabe, K., & Nijhawan, R. (2000). The role of attention in motion extrapolation: are moving objects “corrected” of flashed objects attentionally delayed? Perception 29: 675–692.
Kirschfeld, K. (2006). Stopping motion and the flash-lag effect. Vision Res 46: 1547–1551.
Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 16–22.
Kreegipuu, K., & Allik, J. (2003). Perceived onset time and position of a moving stimulus. Vision Res 43: 1625–1635.
Krekelberg, B. (2001). The persistence of position. Vision Res 41: 529–539.
Krekelberg, B., & Lappe, M. (1999). Temporal recruitment along the trajectory of moving objects and the perception of position. Vision Res 39: 2669–2679.
LaBerge, D. (1997). Attention, awareness, and the triangular circuit. Conscious Cogn 6: 149–181.
Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends Cogn Sci 7: 12–18.
Lamme, V. A. F. (2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention; a case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Netw 17: 861–872.
Libet, B. (2004). Mind Time. The Temporal Factor in Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Llinás, R., Leznik, E., & Urbano, F. J. (2002). Temporal binding via cortical coincidence detection of specific and nonspecific thalamocortical inputs: a voltage-dependent dye-imaging study in mouse brain slices. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99: 449–454.
Llinás, R., & Ribary, U. (2001). Consciousness and the brain. The thalamocortical dialogue in health and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 929: 166–175.
Llinás, R., Ribary, U., Contreras, D., & Pedroarena, C. (1998). The neuronal basis for consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 353: 1841–1849.
Llinás, R., Urbano, F. J., Leznik, E., Ramirez, R. R., & van Merle, H. J. F. (2005). Rhythmic and dysrhythmic thalamocortical dynamics: GABA systems and the edge effect. Trends Neurosci 28: 325–333.
Magoun, H. W. (1958). The Waking Brain. Springfield, IL: C. C. Thomas.
McDonald, J. J., Teder-Sälejärvi, W. A., Di Russo, F., & Hillyard, S. A. (2005). Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception. Nat Neurosci 8: 1197–1202.
Melcher, D., Papathomas, T. V., & Vidnyansky, Z. (2005). Implicit attentional selection of bound visual features. Neuron 46: 723–729.
Mitroff, S. R., & Scholl, B. J. (2004). Last but not least: seeing the disappearance of unseen objects. Perception 33: 1267–1273.
Mitroff, S. R., & Scholl, B. J. (2005). Forming and updating object representations without awareness: evidence from motion-induced blindness. Vision Res 45: 961–967.
Montaser-Kouhsari, L., & Rajimehr, R. (2005). Subliminal attentional modulation in crowding condition. Vision Res 45: 839–844.
Moore, C. M., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Object updating and the flash-lag effect. Psychol Sci 15: 866–871.
Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997). Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive system. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 264: 1407–1414.
Murakami, I. (2001). A flash-lag effect in random motion. Vision Res 41: 3101–3119.
Naccache, L., Blandin, E., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychol Sci 13: 416–424.
Neumann, O. (1982). Experimente zum Fehrer-Raab-Effekt und das Wetterwart-Modell der visuellen Maskierung. Report No. 24/1982, Psychological Institute, Ruhr-University Bochum.
Newman, J. (1995). Thalamic contributions to attention and consciousness. Conscious Cogn 4: 172–193.
Nijhawan, R. (1994). Motion extrapolation in catching. Nature 370: 256–257.
Nijhawan, R. (2001). The flash-lag phenomenon: object motion and eye movements. Perception 30: 263–282.
Nijhawan, R., & Khurana, B. (2000). Conscious registration of continuous and discrete visual events. In T., Metzinger (ed.), Neural Correlates of Consciousness. (203–219). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Öğmen, H., Patel, S. S., Bedell, H. E., & Camuz, K. (2004). Differential latencies and the dynamics of the position computation process of moving targets, assessed with the flash-lag effect. Vision Res 44: 2109–2128.
Purpura, D. (1970). Operations and processes in thalamic and synaptically related neural subsystems. In F. O., Schmitt (ed.), The Neurosciences. Second Study Program (458–470). New York: Rockefeller University Press.
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 261–270.
Reichardt, W. (1961). Autocorrelation: a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system. In W. A., Rosenblith (ed.), Principles of Sensory Communication. New York: Wiley.
Ribary, U. (2005). Dynamics of thalamo-cortical network oscillations and human perception. In S., Laureys (ed.), Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 150 (127–142).
Rose, D. (2006). Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Salminen, N., Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2005). Independence of visual awareness from attention at early processing stages. Paper presented at ASSC9, June 24–27, Pasadena, CA.
Scharlau, I. (2004a). Evidence against response bias in temporal order tasks with attention manipulation by masked prime. Psychol Res 68: 224–236.
Scharlau, I. (2004b). The spatial distribution of attention in perceptual latency priming. Q J Exp Psychol 57: 1411–1436.
Scharlau, I. (2007). Temporal processes in prime-mask interaction. Advances in Cognitive Psychology 3: 241–255.
Scharlau, I., & Ansorge, U. (2003). Intention-mediated control of attention by nonconscious information (paper presented at ASSC8).
Scharlau, I., & Neumann, O. (2003a). Temporal parameters and time course of perceptual latency priming. Acta Psychologica 113: 185–203.
Scharlau, I., & Neumann, O. (2003b). Perceptual latency priming by masked and unmasked stimuli: evidence for an attentional interpretation. Psychol Res 67: 184–196.
Scheibel, A. (1980). Anatomical and physiological substrates of arousal: a view from the bridge. In J. A., Hobson & M. A. B., Brazier (eds.), The Reticular Formation Revisited: Specifying Function for a Nonspecific System (55–66). New York: Raven.
Schurger, A., Cowey, A., Cohen, J., Treisman, A., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2008). Distinct and independent correlates of awareness and attention in a hemianopic patient. Neuropsychologia 46: 2189–2197.
Sherman, S. M., & Guillery, R. W. (1998). On the actions that one nerve cell can have on another: distinguishing “drivers” from “modulators.”Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 7121–7126.
Sheth, B. R., Nijhawan, R., & Shimojo, S. (2000). Changing objects lead briefly flashed ones. Nat Neurosci 3: 489–495.
Shore, D. J., Spence, C., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry. Psychol Sci 12: 205–212.
Singer, W. (1998). Consciousness and the structure of neuronal representations. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 353: 1829–1840.
Steriade, M. (1996). Awakening the brain. Nature 383: 24–25.
Steriade, M. (2000). Corticothalamic resonance, states of vigilance and mentation. Neurosci 101: 243–276.
Steriade, M., Jones, E. G., & Llinás, R. R. (1990). Thalamic Oscillations and Signalling. New York: Wiley.
Steriade, M., Jones, E. G., & McCormick, D. A. (1997). Thalamus. Oxford: Elsevier.
Steriade, M., & McCarley, R. W. (2005). Brain Control of Wakefulness and Sleep. New York: Springer/Kluwer/Plenum.
Stone, A., & Valentine, T. (2005). Orientation of attention to nonconsciously recognised famous faces. Cogn Emot 19: 537–558.
Summerfield, C., Jack, A. I., & Burgess, A. P. (2002). Induced gamma activity is associated with conscious awareness of pattern masked nouns. Int J Psychophysiol 44: 93–100.
Taylor, K., Mandon, S., Freiwald, W. A., & Kreiter, A. K. (2005). Coherent oscillatory activity in monkey area V4 predicts successful allocation of attention. Cerebral Cortex 15: 1424–1437.
Treisman, A. (1998). Feature binding, attention and object perception. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 353: 1295–1306.
VanRullen, R., Reddy, L., & Koch, C. (2010). A motion illusion reveals the temporally discrete nature of visual awareness. In R., Nijhawan & B., Khurana (eds.), Space and Time in Perception and Action (521–535). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Vibell, J., Klinge, C., Zampini, M., Spence, C., & Nobre, A. C. (2007). Temporal order is coded temporally in the brain: early event-related potential latency shifts underlying prior entry in a cross-modal TOJ task. J Cogn Neurosci 19: 109–120.
Whitney, D., & Murakami, I. (1998). Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation. Nat Neurosci 1: 656–657.
Whitney, D., Murakami, I., & Cavanagh, P. (2000). Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli. Vision Res 40: 137–149.
Wright, R. D., & Richard, D. M. (2003). Sensory mediation of stimulus-driven attentional capture in multiple-cue displays. Percept Psychophys 65: 925–938.
Wyart, V., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2008). Neural dissociation between visual awareness and spatial attention. J Neurosci 28: 2667–2679.
Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2005). Audiovisual prior entry. Neurosci Lett 381: 217–222.