Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T10:31:53.751Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Theory And Problems in Psycholinguistics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2008

Extract

A major problem for linguistic theory is that of specifying how the sounds of language relate to meaning. Although this problem is an exceedingly complex one, considerable progress has been made in formalizing grammars which, while incomplete, have provided a reasonable first account of the structure of natural language. These developments in linguistic theory, and in particular generative–transformational grammars, have had a profound effect on the psychological study of language, shifting the emphasis from the behavioristic studies of words to the analysis of sentence comprehension and production. More recently, following parallel developments in lingistics, psychologists have also turned their attention to units larger than the sentence, with a particular focus on text comprehension.

Type
Psycholinguistics
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

UNANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY

Anderson, R. C. and Pichert, J. W.. 1978. Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective. Journal of verball learning and verbal behavior. 17.112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antos, S. J. 1979. Processing facilitation in a lexical decision task. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance. 5.527545.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. 1932. Remembering: A Study of experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Black, M. 1937. Vagueness. Philosophy of science. 4.427455.Google Scholar
Blank, M. A. and Foss, D. J.. 1978. Semantic facilittation and lexical access during sentence processing. Memory and cognition. 6.644652.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R., and Franks, J. J.. 1972. Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive psychology. 3.193209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bransford, J. D., and Franks, J. J.. 1971. The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Cognitive psychology. 2.331350.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., and Johnson, M. K.. 1973. Consideration of some problems of comprehension. In Chase, W. G. (ed.) Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D. and Johnson, M. K.. 1972. Contextual prerequisties for understanding: some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 11.717726.Google Scholar
Caplan, D. 1972. Clause boundaries and recognition latencies for words in sentences. Perception and psychophysics. 12.7376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A. and Grober, E.. (1976). Polysemy and the structure of the subjective lexicon. In Rameh, C. (ed.) Georgetown University round table on language and linguistics. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, M. M., Tanenhaus, M. K. and Bever, T. G.. 1978. The perception of relations: Functional and contextual factors in the segmentation of speech. In Levelt, W. J. and Flores d'Arcais, G. B. (eds.) Studies in the Perception of language. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. and Clark, H. H.. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language. 55. 767811.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. 1973. Space, time, and the child. In Morre, T. E. (ed.) Cognitive development and the acquistion of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cole, R. A. 1973. Listening for mispronunciation: A measure of what we hear during speech. Perception and psychophysics. 11.153156.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M. and Loftus, E. F.. 1975. A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological review. 82.407428.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M. and Quillian, M. R.. 1969. Retrival time from semantic memory. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 8.240248.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M. and Quillian, M. R.. 1970. Does category size affect categorization time? Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 9.432438.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M. and Quillian, M. R.. 1972. How to make a language user. In Tulving, E. and Donaldson, W. (eds.) Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Crothers, E. J. 1978. Inference and coherence. Discourse Processes. 1.5171.Google Scholar
Crothers, E. J.. 1979. Paragraph structure inference. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Danks, J. A. and Glucksberg, S.. 1980. Experimental psycholinguistics. Annual review of Psychology. 31.391417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dooling, D. J. and Lachman, R.. 1971. Effects of comprehension on retention of prose. Journal of experimental psychology. 88.216222.Google Scholar
Fillenbaum, S. and Rapoport, A.. 1971. Structures in the subjective lexicon. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Flores, d'Arcais G. B. 1978. The perception of complex sentences. In Levelt, W. J. M. and Flores d'Arcais, G. B. (eds.) Studies in the perception of language. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G., and Garrett, M. F.. 1974. The psychology of language. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. 1976. Accessing the mentall lexicon. In Walker, E. C. T. and Wales, R. J. (eds.) New approaches to language mechanisms. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. 1979. On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Frederiksen, C. H. 1975. Representing logical and semantic structure of knowledge acquired from discourse. Cognitive psychology. 7.371458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gellatly, A. R. H. and Gregg, V. H.. 1975. The effects of negative relatedness upon word-picture and word-word comparisons and subsequent recall. British Journal of psychology. 66.311323.Google Scholar
Gellatly, A. R. H. and Gregg, V. H.. 1977. Intercategory distance and categorization time: Effects of negative-probe relatedness. Journal of verbal learnig and verbal behavior. 16.505518.Google Scholar
Glass, A. L., Holyoak, K. J., and Kiger, J. I.. 1979. Role of antonym relations in semantic judgments. Journal of experimental psychology: Human learning and memory. 5.598606.Google Scholar
Glass, A. L., and Meany, P. J.. 1978. Evidence for two kinds of low-typical intances in a categorization task. Memory and cognition. 6.622628.Google Scholar
Glenn, C. G. 1978. The role of episodic structure and of story length in children's recall of simple stories. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 17.229247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, S. E. and Clark, H. H.. 1974. What's new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 13.512521.Google Scholar
Hersch, H. M. and Caramazza, A.. 1976. A fuzzy set approach to modifiers and vagueness in natural language. Journal of experimental psychology. General. 105.254276.Google Scholar
Holyoak, K. J. and Glass, A. L.. 1978. Recognition confusions among quantifiers. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 17.249264.Google Scholar
Jarvella, R. 1970. Effects of syntax on running memory span for connected discourse. Psychonomic science. 19.235236.Google Scholar
Jarvella, R.. 1971. Syntactic processing of connected speech. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 10.409416.Google Scholar
Jarvella, R. and Herman, S. J.. 1972. Clause structure of sentences and speech processing. perception and psychophysics. 11.381384.Google Scholar
Johnson, R. 1970 Recall of prose as a function of the sructural importance of the linguistic units. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 9.1220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, R. and Scheidt, B. 1977. Organization encodings in the serial learing of prose. Journal of verbal learning and behavior. 16.575578.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1978. The meaning of modality. Cognitive science. 2.1726.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. 1974. The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., Mandel, T. S., and Kozminsky, E.. 1977. Summarizing scrambled stories. Memory and cognition. 5.547552.Google Scholar
Kozminsky, E. 1977. Altering comprehension: the effect of biasing titles on text comprehension. Memory and cognition. 5.482490.Google Scholar
Landauer, T. K. and Meyer, D. E.. 1972. Category size and semantic memory retrieval. Journal of verbal learnig and verbal behavior. 11.539549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F. Jr., 1978. The role of two types of semantic information in the processing of false sentences. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 17.523537.Google Scholar
McCloskey, M. 1980. The stimulus familiarity problem in semantic memory research. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 19.485502.Google Scholar
McCloskey, M. and Glucksberg, S.. 1978. Natural categories: Well defined or fuzzy sets? Memory and cognition. 6.462472.Google Scholar
McKoon, G. 1977. Organization of information in text memory. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 16.247260.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. 1978. A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval. Discourse processes. 1.1435.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. 1975. Sentence perception as an interactive parallel process. Science. 189.226228.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. 1973. Linguistics structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies. Nature. 244.522523.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Tyler, L. K., and Seidenberg, M.. 1978. Sentence processing and the clause boundary. In Levelt, W. J. M. and Flores d'Arcais, G. B. (eds.) Studies in the perception of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Meyer, B. J. F. 1975. The organization of prose and its effect on memory. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Meyer, B. J. F. and McConkie, G. W.. 1973. What is recalled after hearing a passage? Journal of educational psychology. 65.109117.Google Scholar
Meyer, D. E. and Schvaneveldt, R. W.. 1971. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of experimental psychology. 90.227234.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. 1972. English verbs of motion: A case study of semantics and lexical memory. In Melton, A. W. and Martin, E. (eds.) Coding processes in human memory. Washington, D.C.: V. H. Winston and sons.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. and Johnson-Laird, P. N.. 1976. Language and perception. Cambridge. MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. 1975. A framework for representing knowledge. In Winston, P. (ed.) The psychology of computer vision. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Morton, J. 1969. The interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological review. 76.165178.Google Scholar
Morton, J. 1970. A fuctional model for memory. In Norman, D. A. (ed.) Models of human memory. New york: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Morton, J. and Long, J.. 1976. Effect of word transitional probability on phoneme identification. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 15.4351.Google Scholar
Neely, J. H. 1976. Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Evidence for facilitory and inhibitory processes. Memory and cognition. 4.648654.Google Scholar
Neely, J. H. 1977. Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: The roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and capacity attention. Journal of experimental psychology. General. 106.166.Google Scholar
Oden, G. C. 1977. Fuzziness in semantic memory: Choosing exemplars of subjective categories. Memory and cognition. 5.198204.Google Scholar
Ortony, A. (ed.) 1979. Metaphor and thought. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J. 1980. How big is big? Relative and absolute properties in memory. Cognition. 8.145174.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J., Shoben, E. J., and Smith, E. E.. 1973. Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 12.120.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. 1973. On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Moore, T. E. (ed.) Cognitive development and acquisition of language. New york: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. and Mervis, C. B.. 1975. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive psychology. 7.573605.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S. S., and Rubenstein, M. A.. 1971. Evidence for phonemic recording in visual word recognition. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 10.645657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. 1975. Notes on a schema for stories. In Bobrow, D. C. and Collins, A. (eds.) Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Remelhart, D. E. 1977. Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In Laberge, A. A., Samuels, S. J., and Clark, H. (eds.) Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Russell, B. 1921. The analysis of mind. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L. and Glenn, C. G.. 1979. An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. O. (ed.) Advances in discourse processing: New directions in discourse processing. Vol. 2. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L. and Nezworski, T.. 1978. The effects of organization and instructional set on story memory. Discourse processes. 1.177193.Google Scholar
Swinney, D. A. and Hakes, D. T.. 1976. Effects of prior context upon lexical access during sentence comprehension. Jorunal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 15.681689.Google Scholar
Thorndyke, P. W. 1976. The role of inferences in discourse comprehension. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 15.437446.Google Scholar
Thorndyke, P. W. 1977. Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive psychology. 9.77110.Google Scholar
Townsend, D. J., Ottaviano, D., and Bever, T. G.. 1979. Immediate memory for words from main and subordinate clauses at different age levels. Journal of psycholinguistic research. 8.83101.Google Scholar
Tweedy, J. R., Lapinski, R. H., and Schvaneveldt, R. W.. 1977. Semantic context effects of word recognition: Influence of varying the proportion of items presented in an appropriate context. Memory and cognition. 5.8489.Google Scholar
Tyler, L. K. and Marslen-Wilson, W. D.. 1977. The one-line effects of semantic context on syntactic processing. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 16.683692.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. T. 1971. Conjoint frequency, category size, and categorization time. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior. 10.382385.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical investigations. Tr. Anscombe, G. E. M.. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar