Hostname: page-component-5d59c44645-l48q4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-02-25T10:04:51.175Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The construction of emotional experience requires the integration of implicit and explicit emotional processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 May 2012

Markus Quirin
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, University of Osnabrueck, 49074 Osnabrueck, Germany. mquirin@uos.dehttp://www.motivationlab.uni-osnabrueck.de
Richard D. Lane
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85724-5002. lane@email.arizona.eduhttp://www.psychiatry.arizona.edu/faculty/richard-d-lane-md-phd

Abstract

Although we agree that a constructivist approach to emotional experience makes sense, we propose that implicit (visceromotor and somatomotor) emotional processes are dissociable from explicit (attention and reflection) emotional processes, and that the conscious experience of emotion requires an integration of the two. Assessments of implicit emotion and emotional awareness can be helpful in the neuroscientific investigation of emotion.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L. & Berntson, G. G. (1999) The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76(5):839–55.Google Scholar
Frewen, P., Lane, R. D., Neufeld, R. W. J., Densmore, M., Stevens, T. & Lanius, R. (2008) Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness during trauma script-imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine 70(1):2731.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. & McNaughton, N. (2000) The neuropsychology of anxiety. An enquiry into the functions of the Septo-Hippocampal system. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L. & Raichle, M. E. (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98(7):4259–64.Google Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. F., Mulvaney, S., Tobias, B. A. & Tobis, I. P. (2000) The emotional unconscious. In: Cognition and emotion, ed. Eich, E., Kihlstrom, J. F., Bower, G. H., Forgas, J. P. & Niedenthal, P. M., pp. 3086. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lambie, J. A. & Marcel, A. J. (2002) Consciousness and the varieties of emotional experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review 109:219–59.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D. (2008) Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes: A unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Medicine 70(2):214–31.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M.-L. & Dolan, R. J. (1997) Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses. Neuroreport 8(18):3969–72.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D. & Nadel, L. (2000) Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Quinlan, D. M., Schwartz, G. E. & Walker, P. A. (1990) The levels of emotional awareness scale: A cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. Journal of Personality Assessment 55(1–2):124–34.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Axelrod, B., Yun, L.-S., Holmes, A. & Schwartz, G. E. (1998) Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness: Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10(4):525–35.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D. & Schwartz, G. E. (1987) Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry 144:133–43. [Published erratum appears in American Journal of Psychiatry, April 1987, 144(4):542.]Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1996) The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
McRae, K., Reiman, E. M., Fort, C. L., Chen, K. & Lane, R. D. (2008) Association between trait emotional awareness and dorsal anterior cingulate activity during emotion is arousal-dependent. Neuroimage 41:648–55.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran, T. & Mackey, S. (2004a) Reflecting upon feelings: An fMRI study of neural systems supporting the attribution of emotion to self and other. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16 (10):1748–72. Available at: http://dept.psych.columbia.edu/~kochsner/pdf/Ochsner_Reflect_Feelings.pdf.Google Scholar
Phan, K. L., Wager, T. D., Taylor, S. F. & Liberzon, I. (2004) Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions. CNS Spectrums 9(4):258–66.Google Scholar
Quirin, M., Kazén, M. & Kuhl, J. (2009a) When nonsense sounds happy or helpless: The Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test (IPANAT). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97(3):500–16.Google Scholar
Quirin, M., Kazén, M., Rohrmann, S. & Kuhl, J. (2009b) Implicit but not explicit affectivity predicts circadian and reactive cortisol: Using the Implicit Positive and Negative Affect Test. Journal of Personality 77(2):401–25.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J. & Tellegen, A. (1999) The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76(5):820–38.Google Scholar
Winkielman, P. & Berridge, K. C. (2004) Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13(3):120–23.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (2000) Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In: Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition, ed. Forgas, J. P., pp. 3158. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar