Skip to main content
    • Aa
    • Aa
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 322
  • Cited by
    This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Abou-Ismail, Usama A. and Mendl, Michael T. 2016. The effects of enrichment novelty versus complexity in cages of group-housed rats (Rattus norvegicus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Vol. 180, p. 130.

    Doughty, Amanda K. Ferguson, Drewe Matthews, Lindsay R. and Hinch, Geoff N. 2016. Assessing feeding motivation in sheep using different behavioural demand models and measures. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Vol. 180, p. 43.

    Greenway, Eleanor Jones, Katherine S. and Cooke, Gavan M. 2016. Environmental enrichment in captive juvenile thornback rays, Raja clavata (Linnaeus 1758). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Vol. 182, p. 86.

    Kroshko, Jeanette Clubb, Ros Harper, Laura Mellor, Emma Moehrenschlager, Axel and Mason, Georgia 2016. Stereotypic route tracing in captive Carnivora is predicted by species-typical home range sizes and hunting styles. Animal Behaviour, Vol. 117, p. 197.

    Mäekivi, Nelly 2016. Modelling Ex Situ Animal Behaviour and Communication. Biosemiotics,

    Protopopova, Alexandra 2016. Effects of sheltering on physiology, immune function, behavior, and the welfare of dogs. Physiology & Behavior, Vol. 159, p. 95.

    Reijgwart, Marsinah L. Vinke, Claudia M. Hendriksen, Coenraad F.M. van der Meer, Miriam Schoemaker, Nico J. and van Zeeland, Yvonne R.A. 2016. Ferrets’ (Mustela putorius furo) enrichment priorities and preferences as determined in a seven-chamber consumer demand study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Vol. 180, p. 114.

    Tolkamp, Bert J. and D’Eath, Rick B. 2016. Nutrition and the Welfare of Farm Animals.

    Acklin, Casey J and Gault, Ruth A 2015. Effects of natural enrichment materials on stress, memory and exploratory behavior in mice. Lab Animal, Vol. 44, Issue. 7, p. 262.

    Bayne, Kathryn A.L. Beaver, Bonnie V. Mench, Joy A. and Winnicker, Christina 2015. Laboratory Animal Medicine.

    Bethell, Emily J. and Koyama, Nicola F. 2015. Happy hamsters? Enrichment induces positive judgement bias for mildly (but not truly) ambiguous cues to reward and punishment inMesocricetus auratus. Royal Society Open Science, Vol. 2, Issue. 7, p. 140399.

    Bethell, Emily J. 2015. A “How-To” Guide for Designing Judgment Bias Studies to Assess Captive Animal Welfare. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, Vol. 18, Issue. sup1, p. S18.

    Blidaru, Ligia and Opre, Adrian 2015. The Moralization of Eating Behavior. Gendered Cognitive and Behavioral Strategies. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 187, p. 547.

    Brown, Marilyn J. and Winnicker, Christina 2015. Laboratory Animal Medicine.

    Dawkins, Marian 2015.

    de Oliveira, Adriana Terçariol, César and Genaro, Gelson 2015. The Use of Refuges by Communally Housed Cats. Animals, Vol. 5, Issue. 2, p. 245.

    Hasenjager, Matthew J. and Bergl, Richard A. 2015. Environmental conditions associated with repetitive behavior in a group of African elephants. Zoo Biology, Vol. 34, Issue. 3, p. 201.

    Hernandez, Carlos E. Hinch, Geoff Lea, Jim Ferguson, Drewe and Lee, Caroline 2015. Acute stress enhances sensitivity to a highly attractive food reward without affecting judgement bias in laying hens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Vol. 163, p. 135.

    Marchant-Forde, Jeremy N. 2015. The Science of Animal Behavior and Welfare: Challenges, Opportunities, and Global Perspective. Frontiers in Veterinary Science, Vol. 2,

    Mehrkam, Lindsay R. and Dorey, Nicole R. 2015. Preference assessments in the zoo: Keeper and staff predictions of enrichment preferences across species. Zoo Biology, Vol. 34, Issue. 5, p. 418.


From an animal's point of view: Motivation, fitness, and animal welfare

  • Marian Stamp Dawkins (a1)
  • DOI:
  • Published online: 01 May 2011

To study animal welfare empirically we need an objective basis for deciding when an animal is suffering. Suffering includes a wide range ofunpleasant emotional states such as fear, boredom, pain, and hunger. Suffering has evolved as a mechanism for avoiding sources ofdanger and threats to fitness. Captive animals often suffer in situations in which they are prevented from doing something that they are highly motivated to do. The “price” an animal is prepared to pay to attain or to escape a situation is an index ofhow the animal “feels” about that situation. Withholding conditions or commodities for which an animal shows “inelastic demand” (i.e., for which it continues to work despite increasing costs) is very likely to cause suffering. In designing environments for animals in zoos, farms, and laboratories, priority should be given to features for which animals show inelastic demand. The care ofanimals can thereby be based on an objective, animal-centered assessment of their needs.

Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

Animal Behavior Society/Association for the Study of Animal Behavior (1986) Guidelines for the use of animals in research. Animal Behaviour 34: 315–18. [arMSD]

(1988) Sacrificial symbolism in animal experimentation: Object or pet? Anthrozoos 2: 98117. [AA]

N. H. Azrin (1961) Timeout from positive reinforcement. Science 133: 382–83. [JST]

B. A. Baldwin , & D. B. Stephens (1973) The effects of conditione d behaviour and environmental factors on plasma corticosteroid levels in pigs. Physiology and Behavior 10: 267–74. [aMSD]

J. D. Baldwin , & J. I. Baldwin (1976) Effects of food ecology on social play: A laboratory simulation. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 40: 114. [FW]

R. Bogdan & S. Taylor (1989) Relationships with severely disabled people: The social construction of humanness. Social Problems 36: 135–48. [AA]

D. A. Booth , D. Lovett & G. M. McSherry (1972) Postingestive modulation of the sweetness preference gradient in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 78: 485512. [aMSD]

D. M. Broom (1983) Sterotypes as animal welfare indicators. In: Indicators relevant to farm animal welfare, ed. D. Smidt . Martinus Nijhoff. [aMSD]

(1986a) Indicators of poor welfare. British Veterinary Journal 142: 524–26. [aMSD, DMB]

(1988b) The scientific assessment of animal welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 20: 5–19. [DMB, TW]

P. L. Brown , & H. M. Jenkins (1968) Autoshaping of the pigeon’s key-peck. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11: 18. [RD]

M. Cabanac (1979) Sensory pleasure. Quarterly Review of Biology 54: 129. [aMSD]

(1985) Influence of food and water deprivation on the behavior of the white rat foraging in a hostile environment. Physiology and Behavior 35: 701–9. [aMSD]

B. Carder & K. Berkowitz (1970) Rats’ preference for earned in comparison with free food. Science 167: 1273–74. [aMSD]

C. R. Chapman , K. L. Casey , R. Dubner , R. Foley , K. M. Gracely & A. E. Reading (1985) Pain measurement: An overview. Pain 22: 132. [aMSD]

G. H. Collier , E. Hirsch & P. Hamlin (1972) The ecological determinant s of reinforcement in the rat. Physiology and Behavior 9: 705–16. [aMSD]

R. L. Conner , J. Vernikos-Danellis & SS Levine . (1971) Stress, fighting and neuroendocrine function. Nature 234: 564–66. [FT]

R. Dantzer & B. A. Baldwin (1974) Changes in heart rat e during suppression of operant responding in pigs. Physiology and Behavior 12: 385–89. [aMSD]

C. Darwin (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. Modern Library. [BER]

M. S. Dawkins (1977) Do hens suffer in battery cages? Environmental preference and welfare. Animal Behaviour 25: 1034–46. [MM]

(1983a) Battery hens name their price: Consumer demand theory and th e measurement of ethological “needs.” Animal Behaviour 31: 11951205. [arMSD]

(1989) Time budgets in Red Junglefowl as a baselin e for the assessment of welfare in domestic fowl. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 24: 7780. [aMSD]

M. S. Dawkins , & T. Beardsley (1986) Reinforcing properties of access to litter in hens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 351–64. [aMSD, TW]

J. M. Diamond , W. H. Karasov , D. Phan & F. L. Carpenter (1986) Digestive physiology is a determinant of foraging bout frequency in hummingbirds. Nature, London 320: 6263. [aMSD]

J. W. Driscoll , & P. Bateson (1988) Animals in behavioural research. Animal Behaviour 36: 1569–74. [AEM]

(1981) Animal rights-animal welfare: A scientist’s assessment. Poultry Science 60: 489–99. [TW]

I. J. H. Duncan , C. J. Savory & D. G. M. Wood-Gush (1978) Observations on the reproductive behaviou r of domestic fowl in the wild. Applied Animal Ethology 4: 2941. [aMSD]

C. H. Eisemann , W. K. Jorgensen , D. J. Merrit , M. J. Rice , B. W. Cribb , P. D. Webb & M. P. Azlucki (1984) Do insects feel pain? A biological view. Experientia 40: 164–67. [ANR]

J.-M. Faure (1986) Operant determination of the cage and feeder size preference of the laying hen. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 325–36. [aMSD]

A. F. Fraser (1984) The behaviour of suffering in animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 13: 15. [AFF]

(1989) Animal welfare practice: Primary factors and objectives. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 22: 159–76. [AFF]

A. F. Fraser , & J. P. Quine (1989) Veterinary examination of suffering as a behaviour-linked condition. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 23: 353–64. [AFF]

G. G. Gallup (1970) Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science 167: 8687. [RWB]

(1982) Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology 2: 237–48. [EAS]

J-G. J. Godin , & S. A. Smith (1988) A fitness cost of foraging in the guppy. Nature (London) 333: 6971. [AEM]

S. J. Gould , & R. C. Lewontin (1978) The spandrels of San Marcos and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 205: 581–98. [DM, rMSD]

J. P. Henry (1982) The relation of social to biological processes in disease. Social Science and Medicine 16: 369–80. [FT]

J. M. Herbers (1981) Time resources and laziness in animals. Oecologia 49: 252–62. [aMSD]

W. C. Holz , & N. H. Azrin (1961) Discriminative properties of punishment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 4: 225–32. [JST]

G. A. Horridge (1962) Learning of leg position by the ventral nerve cord in headless insects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 157: 3352. [SW]

A. I. Houston (1987) The control of foraging decisions. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior, vol. 6, Foraging, ed. M. L. Commons , A. Kacelnik & S. J. Shettleworth . Lawrence Erlbaum.

(1989) The value of food: Effects of open and closed economies. Animal Behaviour 37: 546–62.

A. I. Houston , & J. M. McNamara (1982) A sequential approach to risk taking. Animal Behaviour 30: 1260–61. [aMSD]

(1976) Preference decisions of domestic hens for wire or litter floors. Applied Animal Ethology 2: 155–65. [aMSD]

B. O. Hughes , I. J. H. Duncan & M. F. Brown (1989) The performance of nest-building by domestic hens: Is it more important than the construction of a nest? Animal Behaviour 37: 210–14.

S. R. Hursh (1984) Behavioral economics. Journal for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 42: 435–52.

I. R. Inglis , & N. J. K. Ferguson (1986) Starlings search for food rather than eat freely available, identical food. Animal Behaviour 34: 614–17.

B. O. Hughes , & A. J. Black (1973) The preference of domestic hens for different types of battery cage floor. British Poultry Science 14: 615–19. [aMSD]

B. O. Hughes , & I. J. H. Duncan (1988) The notion of ethological “need,” models of motivation, and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 36: 16961707. [TW, FW]

F. Huntingford (1984) Some ethical issues raised by studies of predation and aggression. Animal Behaviour 32: 210–15. [aMSD]

K. G. Johnson , & M. Cabanac (1982a) Homeostatic competition between food intake and temperatur e regulation in rats. Physiology and Behavior 28: 675–79. [aMSD]

(1982b) Homeostatic competition in rats fed at varying distances from a thermoneutral refuge. Physiology and Behavior 29: 715–20. [aMSD]

P. L. Katz (1974) A long-term approach to foraging optimization. American Naturalist 108: 758–82. [aMSD]

R. T. Kelleher , & W. H. Morse (1968) Schedules using noxious stimuli, III: Responding maintaine d with response-produced electric shocks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11: 819–38. [JST, rMSD]

(1988) Conditionnement operant et utilization de 1’espace chez la poule pondeuse en cage. Behavioural Processes 16: 4356. [aMSD]

S. E. G. Lea (1978) The psychology and economics of demand. Psychological Bulletin 85: 441–66. [aMSD, JA]

N. P. Lester (1984) The “feed-drink” decision. Behaviour 89: 200–19. [aMSD]

M. Lewis & J. Brooks-Gunn (1979) Social cognition and the acquisition of self Plenum Press. [EAS]

A. E. Magurran , & T. J. Pitcher (1987) Provenance, shoal size, and the sociobiology of predator evasion behaviour in minnow shoals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 229: 439–65. [AEM]

M. Mangel & C. W. Clark (1986) Towards a unified foraging theory. Ecology 67: 1127–38. [aMSD]

P. Martin & T. Caro (1985) On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. Advances in the Study of Behavior 15: 59103. [JA]

A. Marwine & G. Collier (1979) The rat at the waterhole. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 93: 391402. [aMSD]

L. R. Matthews , & J. Ladewig (1987) Stimulus requirements of housed pigs assessed by behavioural demand functions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 17: 365–83. [aMSD]

L. R. Matthews , & W. Temple (1979) Concurrent schedule assessment of food preferences in cows. Journal for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32: 245–54. [aMSD]

P. D. McArthur (1986) Similarity of playback songs to self-song as a determinant of response strength in song sparrows. (Melospiza melodia). Animal Behaviour 34: 199207. [EAS]

M. Milinski & R. Heller (1978) Influence of a predator on the optimal foraging behaviour of sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Nature (London) 275: 642–44. [aMSD, AEM]

N. E. Miller (1956) Effects of drugs on motivation: The value of using a variety of measures. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 65: 318–33. [aMSD]

(1985) The value of behavioral research on animals. American Psychologist 40: 423–40. [aMSD]

C. L. Morgan (1894) An introduction to comparative psychology. Walter Scott. [BER]

W. D. Morrison , & I. McMillan (1985) Operant control of the thermal environment by chicks. Poultry Science 64: 9194. [aMSD]

N. Mrosovsky & D. F. Sherry (1980) Animal anorexias. Science 207: 837–42. [SJS]

A. J. Neuringer (1969) Animals respond for food in the presence of free food. Science 166: 399401. [aMSD]

Y-K. Ng (1975) Bentham or Bergson? Finite sensibility, utility functions, and social welfare functions. Review of Economic Studies (Oct.) 42: 545–69. [Y-KN]

C. J. Nicol (1986) Non-exclusive spatial preference in the laying hen. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 337–50. [aMSD]

(1987) Behavioural responses of laying hens following a period of spatial restriction. Animal Behaviour 35: 1709–19. [aMSD]

T. Nishida (1983) Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees. Primates 24: 318–36. [RWB]

M. A. Novak , & S. J. Suomi (1988) Psychological well-being of primates in captivity. American Psychologist 43: 765–73. [MAN]

E. A. Peterson , J. S. Augenstein , D. C. Tanis & D. G. Augenstein (1981) Noise raises blood pressure without impairing auditory sensitivity. Science 211: 1450–52. [JST]

V. A. Ponteaux , G. I. Christison & W. R. Stricklin (1983) Perforated-floor preferences of weanling pigs. Applied Animal Ethology 11.1923. [aMSD]

E. O. Price (1984) Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. Quarterly Review of Biology 59: 132. [aMSD]

T. J. Roper (1983) Learning as a biological phenomenon. In: Animal behaviour, vol. 3, Genes, development, and learning, ed. T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater . Blackwell Scientific Publications. [aMSD]

(1984) Response of thirsty rate to absence of water: Frustration, disinhibition, or compensation? Animal Behaviour 32: 1225–35. [aMSD]

(1986b) Aversion of sheep for handling treatments: Paired choice studies. Applied Animal Behavior Science 16: 363–70. [aMSD]

M. Segal (1978) Serotonic innervation of the locus coeruleus from the dorsal raphe. Journal of Physiology (Cambridge) 286: 401–15. [CRC]

D. F. Sherry , N. Mrosovsky & J. A. Hogan (1980) Weight loss and anorexia during incubation in birds. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 94: 8998. [DMcF, rMSD]

(1986) Antipredator responses and the perception of danger by mosquito larvae. Ecology 67: 434–41. [AEM]

B. F. Skinner (1963) Behaviorism at fifty. Science 140: 951–58. [aMSD]

E. A. Stone (1975) Stress and catecholamines. In: Catecholamines and behavior, vol. 2, ed. A. J. Fridhoff . Plenum Press. [CRC]

(1988) Farming beyond the ability for pigs to adapt. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 20: 6371. [AFF]

(1984) Impoverished environments and welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 12: 313. [aMSD]

(1985) Ontogeny and preference in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 14: 388–89. [aMSD]

J. M. Weiss , L. A. Pohorecky , S. Salman & M. Gruenthal (1976) Attenuation of gastric lesions by psychological aspects of aggressio n in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 90: 252–59. [FT]

P. R. Wiepkema (1987) Behavioural aspects of stress. In: Biology of stress in farm animals: An integrative approach, ed. P. R. Wiepkema & P. W. M. Van Adrichem . Martinus Nijhoff. [FT]

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *