Skip to main content
×
×
Home

Toward a second-person neuroscience 1

  • Leonhard Schilbach (a1), Bert Timmermans (a2), Vasudevi Reddy (a3), Alan Costall (a4), Gary Bente (a5), Tobias Schlicht (a6) and Kai Vogeley (a7)...
Abstract

In spite of the remarkable progress made in the burgeoning field of social neuroscience, the neural mechanisms that underlie social encounters are only beginning to be studied and could – paradoxically – be seen as representing the “dark matter” of social neuroscience. Recent conceptual and empirical developments consistently indicate the need for investigations that allow the study of real-time social encounters in a truly interactive manner. This suggestion is based on the premise that social cognition is fundamentally different when we are in interaction with others rather than merely observing them. In this article, we outline the theoretical conception of a second-person approach to other minds and review evidence from neuroimaging, psychophysiological studies, and related fields to argue for the development of a second-person neuroscience, which will help neuroscience to really “go social”; this may also be relevant for our understanding of psychiatric disorders construed as disorders of social cognition.

Copyright
Footnotes
Hide All
1.

Authors Leonhard Schilbach and Bert Timmermans have contributed equally to this article.

Footnotes
References
Hide All
Allen, M. & Williams, G. (2011) Consciousness, plasticity, and connectomics: The role of intersubjectivity in human cognition. Frontiers in Psychology 2:20. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00020.
Allport, F. H. (1924) Social psychology. Houghton Mifflin.
Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. & Richeson, J. (2000) Towards a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 32:201–72. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80006-4.
Amodio, D. M. & Frith, C. D. (2006) Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7(4):268–77. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884.
Anders, S., Heinzle, J., Weiskopf, N., Ethofer, T. & Haynes, J. D. (2011) Flow of affective information between communicating brains. NeuroImage 54(1):439–46. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.004.
Anderson, M. L. (2010) Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(4):245–66. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000853.
Asch, S. (1952) Social psychology. Prentice-Hall.
Auvray, M., Lenay, C. & Stewart, J. (2009) Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New Ideas in Psychology 27:3247. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002.
Bacharach, M. (2006) Beyond individual choice. Princeton University Press.
Baess, P., Zhdanov, A., Mandel, A., Parkkonen, L., Hirvenkari, L., Mäkelä, J. P., Jousmäki, V. & Hari, R. (2012) MEG dual scanning: A procedure to study real-time auditory interaction between two persons. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:83. (Electronic Journal).
Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G. & Frith, C. D. (2010) Optimally interacting minds. Science 329(5995):1081–85. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1185718.
Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C. & Loomis, J. M. (2003) Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29(7):819–33. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029007002.
Bar, M. (2007) The proactive brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(7):280–89. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.005.
Barresi, J. & Moore, C. (1996) Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19(1):107–22. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041790.
Becchio, C., Pierno, A., Mari, M., Lusher, D. & Castiello, U. (2007) Motor contagion from gaze: The case of autism. Brain 130(9):2401–11. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm171.
Becchio, C., Sartori, L. & Castiello, U. (2010) Toward you: The social side of actions. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19(3):183–88. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721410370131.
Behrens, T. E. J., Hunt, L. T. & Rushworth, M. F. (2009) The computation of social behavior. Science 324(5931):1160–64. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1169694.
Bente, G. (1989) Facilities for the graphical computer simulation of head and body movements. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 21(4):455–62. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03202817.
Bente, G., Eschenburg, F. & Aelker, L. (2007a) Effects of simulated gaze on social presence, person perception and personality attribution in avatar-mediated communication. In: Presence 2007: Proceedings of the 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence, October 25-27, 2007, Barcelona, Spain, ed. Moreno, L., pp. 207–14. Starlab Barcelona, S.L. Available at: http://www.temple.edu/ispr/prev_conferences/proceedings/2007/Bente, Eschenburg, and Aelker.pdf.
Bente, G., Feist, A. & Elder, S. (1996) Person perception effects of computer-simulated male and female head movement. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 20(4):213–28. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02248674.
Bente, G., Krämer, N. C. & Eschenburg, F. (2008a) Is there anybody out there? Analyzing the effects of embodiment and nonverbal behavior in avatar-mediated communication. In: Mediated interpersonal communication, ed. Konijn, E., Utz, S., Tanis, M. & Barnes, S., pp. 131–57. Erlbaum.
Bente, G., Krämer, N. C., Petersen, A. & de Ruiter, J. P. (2001) Computer animated movement and person perception: Methodological advances in nonverbal behavior research. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 25(3):151–66. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1010690525717.
Bente, G., Leuschner, H., Al Issa, A. & Blascovich, J. J. (2010) The others: Universals and cultural specificities in the perception of status and dominance from nonverbal behavior. Consciousness and Cognition 19(3):762–77. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.006.
Bente, G., Rüggenberg, S., Krämer, N. C. & Eschenburg, F. (2008b) Avatar-mediated networking: Increasing social presence and interpersonal trust in net-based collaborations. Human Communication Research 34(2):287318. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00322.x.
Bigelow, A. & Rochat, P. (2006) Two-month-old infants' sensitivity to social contingency in mother-infant and stranger–infant interaction. Infancy 9(3):313–25. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0903_3.
Biocca, F., Harms, C. & Burgoon, J. (2003) Toward a more robust theory and measure of social presence: Review and suggested criteria. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 12(5):456–80. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/105474603322761270.
Blascovich, J., Loomis, J., Beall, A. C., Swinth, K. R., Hoyt, C. L. & Bailenson, J. N. (2002) Immersive virtual environment technology as a methodological tool for social psychology. Psychological Inquiry 13(2):103–24. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1302_01.
Bos, P. A., Panksepp, J., Bluthe, R. M. & Honk, J. V. (2012) Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-emotional behavior: A review of single administration studies. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33(1):1735. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.01.002.
Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S. & Gergely, G. (2007) Investigating action understanding: Inferential processes versus action simulation. Current Biology 17(24):2117–21. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.057.
Brazelton, T. B. (1986) The development of newborn behavior. In: Human growth: A comprehensive treatise, vol. 2, ed. Faulkner, F. & Tanner, J. M., pp. 519–40. Plenum Press.
Brennan, S. E. & Clark, H. H. (1996) Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22(6):1482–93. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1482.
Brennan, S. E. & Hanna, J. E. (2009) Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science 1(2):274–91. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01019.x.
Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Hikosaka, O. & Nakamura, K. (2010) Coding of task reward value in the dorsal raphne nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience 30(18):6262–72. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0015-10.2010.
Brown, J., Aczel, B., Jimenez, L., Kaufman, S. B. & Plaisted-Grant, K. (2010) Intact implicit learning in autism spectrum conditions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63(9):1789–812. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210903536910.
Broz, F., Nourbakhsh, I. & Simmons, R. (2013) Planning for human–robot interaction in situated social tasks: The impact of representing time and intention. International Journal of Social Robotics 5(2):193214.
Bruner, J. (1983) Child's talk. Norton.
Buck, R., Losow, J. I., Murphy, M. M. & Constanzo, P. (1992) Social facilitation and inhibition of emotional expression and communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63(6):962–68. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.962.
Buckner, R. L. & Carroll, D. C. (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(2):4957. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004.
Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I. & Gilbert, S. J. (2007) The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(7):290–98. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004.
Caggiano, V., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G., Thier, P. & Casile, A. (2009) Mirror neurons differentially encode the peripersonal and extrapersonal space of monkeys. Science 324(5925):403406. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1166818.
Camara, E., Rodriguez-Fornells, A. & Munte, T. F. (2008) Functional connectivity of reward processing in the brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2:19. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.019.2008.
Carletta, J., Hill, R. L., Nicol, C., Taylor, T., de Ruiter, J. P. & Bard, E. G. (2010) Eyetracking for two-person tasks with manipulation of a virtual world. Behavior Research Methods 42(1):254–65. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.1.254.
Carpendale, J. E. M. & Lewis, C. (2004) Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(1):79151.
Catani, M., Jones, D. K., Daly, E., Embiricos, N., Deeley, Q., Pugliese, L., Curran, S., Robertson, D. & Murphy, D. G. (2008) Altered cerebellar feedback projections in Asperger syndrome. NeuroImage 41(4):1184–91. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.041.
Catmur, C., Gillmeister, H., Bird, G., Liepelt, R., Brass, M. & Heyes, C. (2008) Through the looking glass: Counter-mirror activation following incompatible sensorimotor learning. European Journal of Neuroscience 28(6):1208–15. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06419.x.
Catmur, C., Walsh, V. & Heyes, C. (2007) Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology 17(17):1527–31. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.006.
Chakrabarti, B. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011) Variation in the human Cannabinoid Receptor (CNR1) gene modulates gaze duration for happy faces. Molecular Autism 2(1):10. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-2-10.
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A. (1999) The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76(6):893910. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893.
Chen, F. S. & Johnson, S. C. (2012) An oxytocin receptor gene variant predicts attachment anxiety in females and autism-spectrum traits in males. Social Psychological and Personality Science 3(1):9399. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550611410325.
Choi, V. S., Gray, H. M. & Ambady, N. (2005) The glimpsed world: Unintended communication and unintended perception. In: The new unconscious, ed. Hassin, R. R., Uleman, J. S. & Bargh, J. A., pp. 309–33. Oxford University Press.
Chomsky, N. (1979) Language and responsibility. Harvester Press.
Chura, L. R., Lombardo, M. V., Ashwin, E., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T. & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010) Organizational effects of fetal testosterone on human corpus callosum size and asymmetry. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35(1):122–32. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.009.
Clark, H. H. (1996) Using language. Cambridge University Press.
Cleeremans, A. (2008) Consciousness: The radical plasticity thesis. Progress in Brain Research 168:1933. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68003-0.
Cleeremans, A. (2011) The radical plasticity thesis: How the brain learns to be conscious. Frontiers in Psychology 2:86. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00086.
Cleeremans, A., Timmermans, B. & Pasquali, A. (2007) Consciousness and metarepresentation: A computational sketch. Neural Networks 20(9):1032–39. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2007.09.011.
Coan, J. A. (2008) Toward a neuroscience of attachment. In: Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications, 2nd edition, ed. Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R., pp. 241–65. Guilford.
Cohn, J. F. & Tronick, E. Z. (1989) Specificity of infants' response to mothers' affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 28:242–8. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004583-198903000-00016.
Cooper, J. C., Dunne, S., Furey, T. & O'Doherty, J. P. (2012) Human dorsal striatum encodes prediction errors during observational learning of instrumental actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24(1):106–18. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00114.
Corbetta, M., Patel, G. H. & Shulman, G. L. (2008) The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58:306–24. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017.
Costall, A. (1995) Socializing affordances. Theory and Psychology 5(4):467–81.
Costall, A. (2006) Introspectionism and the mythical origins of modern scientific psychology. Consciousness and Cognition 15(4):634–54. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.008.
Costall, A., Leudar, I. & Reddy, V. (2006) Failing to see the irony in “mind-reading.” Theory and Psychology 16(2):163–67. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959354306062533.
Crespi, B. & Badcock, C. (2008) Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(3):241–61. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X08004214.
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4):148–53. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005.
Damasio, A. (2010) Self comes to mind. Pantheon.
Daunizeau, J., den Ouden, H. E., Pessiglione, M., Kiebel, S. J., Friston, K. J. & Stephan, K. E. (2010a) Observing the observer (II): Deciding when to decide. PLoS ONE 5(12):e15555. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015555.
Daunizeau, J., den Ouden, H. E., Pessiglione, M., Kiebel, S. J., Stephan, K. E. & Friston, K. J. (2010b) Observing the observer (I): Meta-Bayesian models of learning and decision-making. PLoS ONE 5(12):e15554. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015554.
De Jaegher, H. & Di Paolo, E. (2007) Participatory sensemaking. An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 6(4):485507. Available at: http://www.dx.doi/10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9.
De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E. & Gallagher, S. (2010) Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(10):441–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009.
de Lange, F. P., Spronk, M., Willems, R. M., Toni, I. & Bekkering, H. (2008) Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Current Biology 18(6):454–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.057.
Dewey, J. (1950) Reconstruction in philosophy. The New American Library.
Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J. & Garnero, L. (2010) Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS ONE 5(8):e12166. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012166.
Engemann, D. A., Bzdok, D., Eickhoff, S. B., Vogeley, K. & Schilbach, L. (2012) Games people play – toward an enactive view of cooperation in social neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:148. Epub June 1, 2012.
Fairhurst, M. T., Janata, P. & Keller, P. E. (2012) Being and feeling in sync with an adaptive virtual partner: Brain mechanisms underlying dynamic cooperativity. Cereb Cortex 2012 Aug 28. [Epub ahead of print]
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Johnson, M. & Simion, F. (2002) Eye contact detection at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99(14):9602–605.
Fiebich, A. & Gallagher, S. (2012) Joint attention in joint action. Philosophical Psychology. DOI:10.1080/09515089.2012.690176. Published online May 28, 2012.
Fiske, S. T. & Depret, E. (1996) Control, interdependence, and power: Understanding social cognition in its social context. European Review of Social Psychology 7(1):3161. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14792779443000094.
Fogel, A. (1993) Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self and culture. Harvester Weatsheaf.
Frijda, N. H. (1986) The emotions. Cambridge University Press.
Friston, K. (2008) Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology 4(11): e1000211. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000211.
Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. (2006) The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron 50(4):531–34. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001.
Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. (2008) Implicit and explicit processes in social cognition. Neuron 60(3):503–10. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.032.
Frith, U. & Frith, C. D. (2010) The social brain: Allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 365(1537):165–76. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0160.
Froese, T. & Di Paolo, E. A. (2010) Modeling social interaction as perceptual crossing: An investigation into the dynamics of the interaction process. Connection Science 22(1):4368. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540090903197928.
Fuchs, T. (2009) Das Gehirn – ein Beziehungsorgan. Eine phänomenologisch-ökologische Konzeption. Kohlhammer.
Fuchs, T. & De Jaegher, H. (2009) Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 8(4):465–86. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11097-009-9136-4.
Gallagher, S. (2001) The practice of mind: Theory, simulation, or interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5–7):83108. Available at: http://pegasus.cc.ucf.edu/~gallaghr/practice01.htm.
Gallagher, S. (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press.
Gallagher, S. (2007) Simulation trouble. Social Neuroscience 2(3–4):353–65. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910601183549.
Gallagher, S. (2008) Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Consciousness and Cognition 17(2):535–43. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.003.
Gamer, M. & Buchel, C. (2009) Amygdala activation predicts gaze toward fearful eyes. Journal of Neuroscience 29(28):9123–26. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1883-09.2009.
Gamer, M., Zurowski, B. & Buchel, C. (2010) Different amygdala subregions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107(20):9400–405. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1000985107.
Gangopadhyay, N. & Schilbach, L. (2012) Seeing minds: A neurophilosophical investigation of the role of perception-action coupling in social perception. Social Neuroscience 7(4):410–23. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2011.633754.
Garner, W. R. (1999) Reductionism reduced: Review of “Toward a new behaviorism: The case against perceptual reductionism” by William R. Uttal. Contemporary Psychology 44:2021.
Garrod, S. & Pickering, M. J. (2004) Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(1):811. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.016.
Gergely, G. & Watson, J. S. (1996) The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis 77(6):1181–212. Available at: http://www.pep-web.org/document.php?id=ijp.077.1181a.
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Lifflin.
Gilbert, S. J., Meuwese, J. D., Towgood, K. J., Frith, C. D. & Burgess, P. W. (2009) Abnormal functional specialization within medial prefrontal cortex in high-functioning autism: A multi-voxel similarity analysis. Brain 132(4):869–78. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn365.
Giles, H., Coupland, N. & Coupland, J. (1992) Accommodation theory: Communication, context and consequences. In: Contexts of accommodation, ed. Giles, H., Coupland, J. & Coupland, N., pp. 168. Cambridge University Press.
Goldman, A. (2006) Simulating minds. The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press.
Goldstein, M. H. & Schwade, J. A. (2008) Social feedback to infants' babblings facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science 19(5):515–23. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x.
Goldstein, M. H. & Schwade, J. (2010) From birds to words: Perception of structure in social interactions guides vocal development and language learning. In: The Oxford handbook of developmental behavioral neuroscience, ed. Blumberg, M. S., Freeman, J. H. & Robinson, S. R., pp. 708–29. Oxford University Press.
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J., Briesch, J. & Syal, S. (2010a) Learning while babbling: Prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness to learn. Infancy 15(4):362–91. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00020.x.
Goldstein, M. H., Waterfall, H. R., Lotem, A., Halpern, J. Y., Schwade, J. A., Onnis, L. & Edelman, S. (2010b) General cognitive principles for learning structure in time and space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(6):249–58. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.004.
Groen, W. B., Tesink, C., Petersson, K. M., van Berkum, J., van der Gaag, R. J., Hagoort, P. & Buitelaar, J. K. (2010) Semantic, factual, and social language comprehension in adolescents with autism: An fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex 20(8):1937–45. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp264.
Grossmann, T. & Johnson, M. H. (2010) Selective prefrontal cortex responses to joint attention in early infancy. Biology Letters 6(4):540–43. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.1069
Grossmann, T., Johnson, M., Farroni, T. & Csibra, G. (2007) Social perception in the infant brain: Gamma oscillatory activity in response to eye gaze. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2(4):284–91. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm025.
Haggard, P. (2009) The sources of human volition. Science 324(5928):731–33. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1173827.
Haggard, P. & Tsakiris, M. (2009) The experience of agency: Feelings, judgments, and responsibility. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18(4):242–24. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01644.x.
Hampton, A. N., Bossaerts, P. & O'Doherty, J. P. (2008) Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 105(18):6741–46. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0711099105.
Hari, R. & Kujala, M. V. (2009) Brain basis of human social interaction: From concepts to brain imaging. Physiological Reviews 89(2):453–79.
Harrist, A. W. & Waugh, R. M. (2002) Dyadic synchrony: Its structure and function in children's development. Developmental Review 22(4):555–92. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00500-2.
Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., Keysers, C. (2012) Brain-to-brain coupling: A mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends in Cognitive Science 16(2):114–21.
Heider, F. (1958) The psychology of interpersonal relations. Wiley.
Heider, F. & Simmel, M. (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology 57(2):243–59. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1416950.
Henckens, M. J., Hermans, E. J., Pu, Z., Joels, M. & Fernandez, G. (2009) Stressed memories: How acute stress affects memory formation in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 29(32):10111–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1184-09.2009.
Hermans, E. J., Bos, P. A., Ossewaarde, L., Ramsey, N. F., Fernandez, G. & van Honk, J. (2010) Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal BOLD response during reward anticipation in healthy women. NeuroImage 52(1):277–83. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.019.
Hertwig, R. & Erev, I. (2009) The description-experience gap in risky choice. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(12):517–23. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004.
Heyes, C. (2010) Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34(4):575–83. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007.
Heyes, C., Bird, G., Johnson, H. & Haggard, P. (2005) Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research 22(2):233–40. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.009.
Hikosaka, O. & Isoda, M. (2010) Switching from automatic to controlled behavior: Cortico-basal ganglia mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(4):154–61. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.006.
Hobson, R. P. (1991) Against the theory of “Theory of Mind”. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9(1):3351.
Hobson, R. P. (1999) Intersubjective foundations for joint attention: Co-ordinating attitudes (rather than actions). Paper presented at the Joint Attention Conference, University of Warwick, June 11–13, 1999.
Hull, C. L. (1943) Principles of behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Hutto, D. D. (2008) Folk-psychological narratives. The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. MIT Press.
Izuma, K., Saito, D. N. & Sadato, N. (2008) Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron 58(2):284–94. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.020.
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L. & Jasnow, M. D. (2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 66(2): Serial No. 265.
James, W. (1890/2007) The principles of psychology, vol. 1. Dover/Cosimo Classics. (Original work published in 1890; Cosimo Classics first edition, 2007).
Jenkins, J. J. (1986) Interview with James J. Jenkins. In: The cognitive revolution in psychology, ed. Baars, B. J., pp. 239–52. Guilford.
Jermann, P., Nüssli, M.-A. & Li, W. (2010) Using dual eye-tracking to unveil coordination and expertise in collaborative Tetris. In: BCS'10 Proceedings of the 24th BCS Interaction Specialist Group Conference [British Computer Society Conference on Human–Computer Interaction, Dundee, United Kingdom, September 6–10, 2010], pp. 36–44. British Computer Society/ACM Digital Library.
Jones, E. E. & Gerard, H. B. (1967) Foundations of social psychology. Wiley.
Jones, E. E. & Nisbett, R. E. (1971) The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. General Learning Press.
Kampe, K. K., Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. (2003) “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. Journal of Neuroscience 23(12):5258–63. Available at: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/23/12/5258.full.pdf.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. MIT Press.
Kelso, J. A. S., de Guzman, G. C., Reveley, C. & Tognoli, E. (2009) Virtual Partner Interaction (VPI): Exploring novel behaviors via coordination dynamics. PLoS ONE 4(6):e5749. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005749.
Keltner, D. & Haidt, J. (1999) Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion 13(5):505–21. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026999399379168.
Kennedy, D. P., Redcay, E. & Courchesne, E. (2006) Failing to deactivate: Resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103(21):8275–80. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0600674103.
Keysers, C. & Gazzola, V. (2007) Integrating simulation and theory of mind: From self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(5):194–96. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002.
Khan, M. A., Lawrence, G., Fourkas, A., Franks, I. M., Elliott, D. & Pembroke, S. (2003) Online versus offline processing visual feedback in the control of movement amplitude. Acta Psychologica 113(1):8397. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00156-7.
King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T., Fonagy, P. & Montague, P. R. (2008) The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science 321(5890):806–10. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1156902.
King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R. & Montague, P. R. (2005) Getting to know you: Reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange. Science 308(5718):7883. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1108062.
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. & Volkmar, F. (2003) The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 358(1430):345–60. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1202.
Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G. & Jones, W. (2009) Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature 459(7244):257–61. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature07868.
Knoblich, G. & Sebanz, N. (2008) Evolving intentions for social interaction: From entrainment to joint action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B: Biological Sciences 363(1499):2021–31. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0006.
Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V. & Dehaene, S. (2009) Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science 19;324(5934):1583–85. doi: 10.1126/science.1171599.
Kokal, I., Gazzola, V. & Keysers, C. (2009) Acting together in and beyond the mirror neuron system. NeuroImage 47(4):2046–56. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.010.
Konvalinka, I. & Roepstorff, A. (2012) The two-brain approach: How can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00215.
Konvalinka, I., Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A. & Frith, C. D. (2010) Follow you, follow me: Continuous mutual prediction and adaptation in joint tapping. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63(11):2220–30. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.497843.
Kourtis, D., Sebanz, N. & Knoblich, G. (2010) Favouritism in the motor system: Social interaction modulates action simulation. Biology Letters 6(6):758–61. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2010.0478.
Krueger, J. (2010) Extended cognition and the space of social interaction. Consciousness and Cognition 20(3):643–57. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.022.
Kugiumutzakis, G. (1998) Neonatal imitation in the intersubjective companion space. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S., pp. 6388. Cambridge University Press.
Kuhl, P. K. (2007a) Cracking the speech code: How infants learn language. Acoustical Science and Technology 28(2):7183. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1250/ast.28.71.
Kuhl, P. K. (2007b) Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science 10(1):110–20. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x.
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M. & Liu, H.-M. (2003) Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short- term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100(15):9096–101.
Kuzmanovic, B., Schilbach, L., Lehnhardt, F. G., Bente, G. & Vogeley, K. (2011) A matter of words: Impact of verbal and nonverbal information on impression formation in high-functioning autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5(1):604–13. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.07.005.
Lavelli, M. & Fogel, A. (2002) Developmental changes in mother-infant face-to-face communication: Birth to 3 months. Developmental Psychology 38(2):288305. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.288.
Leahey, T. H. (1992) The mythical revolutions of American psychology. American Psychologist 47(2):308–18. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.47.2.308.
Leekam, S. & Ramsden, C. (2006) Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36(2):185–97. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0054-1.
Legerstee, M. & Varghese, J. (2001) The role of maternal affect mirroring on social expectancies in three-month-old infants. Child Development 72(5):1301–13. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00349.
Lenay, C., Stewart, J., Rohde, M., Ali Amar, A. (2011) “You never fail to surprise me”: The hallmark of the Other: Experimental study and simulations of perceptual crossing. Interaction Studies 12(3):373–96. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/is.12.3.01len.
Leudar, I. & Costall, A. (2008) Against theory of mind. Macmillan.
Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. B. & Vishton, P. M. (1999) Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283(5398):7780. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.283.5398.77.
Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J. & Schmidt, R. C. (2009) Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science 1:320–39. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01022.x.
Masayuki, M. & Okihide, H. (2008) Representation of negative motivational value in the primate lateral habenula. Nature Neuroscience 12:7784. doi:10.1038/nn.2233.
Materna, S., Dicke, P. W. & Thier, P. (2008) Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(1):108–19. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20.1.108.
McQuaid, N., Bibok, J. & Carpendale, J. (2009) Relation between maternal contingent responsiveness and infant social expectations. Infancy 14(3):390401. doi: 10.1080/15250000902839955.
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, K. (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198(4312):7578. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.198.4312.75.
Milstein, D. M. & Dorris, M. C. (2007) The influence of expected value on saccadic preparation. Journal of Neuroscience 27(18):4810–18.
Mitchell, J. P. (2009) Social psychology as a natural kind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(6):246–51. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.008.
Mojzisch, A., Schilbach, L., Helmert, J. R., Pannasch, S., Velichkovsky, B. M. & Vogeley, K. (2006) The effects of self-involvement on attention, arousal, and facial expression during social interaction with virtual others: A psychophysiological study. Social Neuroscience 1(3–4):184–95. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910600985621.
Moll, H., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2007) Fourteen-month-olds know what others experience only in joint engagement. Developmental Science 10(6):826–35. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00615.x.
Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., Dhamala, M., Wiest, M. C., Karpov, I., King, R. D., Apple, N. & Fischer, R. E. (2002) Hyperscanning: Simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. NeuroImage 16(4):1159–64. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1150.
Mundy, P. & Newell, L. (2007) Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16(5):269–74. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00518.x.
Murray, L. & Trevarthen, C. (1985) Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In: Social perception in infants, ed. Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A., pp. 177–97. Ablex.
Muscatell, K. A., Addis, D. R. & Kensinger, E. A. (2010) Self-involvement modulates the effective connectivity of the autobiographical memory network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(1):6876. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp043.
Nadel, J. & Tremblay-Leveau, H. (1999) Early perception of social contingencies and interpersonal intentionality: Dyadic and triadic paradigms. In: Early social cognition, ed. Rochat, P., pp. 189212. Erlbaum.
Nadig, A., Vivanti, G. & Ozonoff, S. (2009) Adaptation of object descriptions to a partner under increasing communicative demands: A comparison of children with and without autism. Autism Research 2(6):334–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.102.
Nagy, E. & Molnar, P. (2004) Homo imitans or homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development 27(1):5463. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.06.004
N'Diaye, K., Sander, D. & Vuilleumier, P. (2009) Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: Gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion 9(6):798806. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017845.
Neisser, U. (1980) On “social knowing.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 6(4):601605. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616728064012.
Neisser, U. (1997) The future of cognitive science: An ecological analysis. In: The future of the cognitive revolution, ed. Johnson, D. M. & Erneling, C. E., pp. 247–60. Oxford University Press.
Newen, A. & Schlicht, T. (2009) Understanding other minds: A criticism of Goldman's Simulation Theory and an outline of the Person Model Theory. Grazer Philosophische Studien 79(1):209–42.
Newman-Norlund, R. D., Bosga, J., Meulenbroek, R. G. & Bekkering, H. (2008) Anatomical substrates of cooperative joint-action in a continuous motor task: Virtual lifting and balancing. NeuroImage 41(1):169–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.026
Newman-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijlen, A. M. J. & Bekkering, H. (2007) The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience 10(7):817–18. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1911.
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M. & Hess, U. (2010) The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(6):417–33; discussion 433–80. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000865.
Noë, A. (2009) Out of our heads. Hill & Wang.
Northoff, G. & Bermpohl, F. (2004) Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(3):102107. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004.
Oberman, L. M., Pineda, J. A. & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007) The human mirror neuron system: A link between action observation and social skills. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2(1):6266. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl022.
Ochsner, K. N. & Lieberman, M. D. (2001) The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist 56(9):717–34. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.9.717.
Oullier, O. & Basso, F. (2010) Embodied economics: How bodily information shapes the social coordination dynamics of decision-making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365(1538):291301. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0168.
Over, H. & Carpenter, M. (2009) Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Developmental Science 12(3):F1–8. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.x.
Pacherie, E. (2008) The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition 107(1):179217. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.003.
Panksepp, J., Herman, B. H., Vilberg, T., Bishop, P. & DeEskinazi, F. G. (1980) Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 4(4):473–87. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(80)90036-6.
Parise, E., Palumbo, L., Handl, A. & Frederici, A. (2008) Gaze direction influences word processing in 4- to 5-month-old infants: An ERP investigation. Poster presented at the Developmental Section Conference of the British Psychological Society, Oxford, UK, September 1–3, 2008.
Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C. & Schultz, R. T. (2004) Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron 42:163–72.
Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T. & McCarthy, G. (2003) Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of context. Neuropsychologia 41(2):156–70. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00146-X.
Penn, D. C. & Povinelli, D. J. (2008) On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a “theory of mind.” In: Social intelligence: From brain to culture, ed. Emery, N., Clayton, N. & Frith, C., pp. 415–30. Oxford University Press.
Pessoa, L. & Engelmann, J. B. (2010) Embedding reward signals into perception and cognition. Frontiers in Neuroscience 4:17. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2010.00017.
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J. & Glenwright, M. R. (2011) Processing of ironic language in children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 41(8):1097–112. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1131-7.
Pfeiffer, U. J., Timmermans, B., Bente, G., Vogeley, K. & Schilbach, L. (2011) The non-verbal Turing test: Differentiating mind from machine in gaze-based social interaction. PLoS ONE 6(11):e27591. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027591.
Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(2):169–90; discussion 91–226.
Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Milea, D. & Muri, R. M. (2004) Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Current Opinion in Neurology 17(1):1725. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00019052-200402000-00005.
Pisella, L., Gréa, H., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., Desmurget, M., Rode, G., Boisson, D. & Rossetti, Y. (2000) An “automatic pilot” for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: Toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nature Neuroscience 3(7):729–36. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/76694.
Port, R. F. & van Gelder, T. (1995) Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition. MIT Press.
Press, C., Gillmeister, H. & Heyes, C. (2007) Sensorimotor experience enhances automatic imitation of robotic action. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 274(1625):2509–14. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.0774.
Preston, S. D. & de Waal, F. B. (2002) Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(1):120. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X02000018.
Prinz, J. (2003) Emotions embodied. In: Thinking about feeling, ed. Solomon, R... pp. 4460. Oxford University Press.
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A. & Shulman, G. L. (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98(2):676–82. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.676.
Redcay, E., Dodell-Feder, D., Pearrow, M. J., Mavros, P. L., Kleiner, M., Gabrieli, J. D. & Saxe, R. (2010) Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience. NeuroImage 50(4):1639–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.052.
Reddy, V. (1996) Omitting the second-person in social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19(1):140–41. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041996.
Reddy, V. (2000) Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science 3(2):186–92. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00112.
Reddy, V. (2003) On being the object of attention: Implications for self–other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(9):397402. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00191-8.
Reddy, V. (2005) Before the “third element”: Understanding attention to self. In: Joint attention: Communication and other minds, ed. Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T. & Roessler, J., pp. 85109. Clarendon.
Reddy, V. (2008) How infants know minds. Harvard University Press.
Reddy, V. (2012) A gaze at grips with me. In: Joint attention: New developments in psychology, philosophy of mind, and social neuroscience, ed. Seemann, A., pp. 137–59. MIT Press.
Reddy, V., Williams, E., Costantini, C. & Lang, B. (2010) Engaging with the self: Mirror behaviour in autism, Down syndrome and typical development. Autism 14(5):531–46. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362361310370397.
Reid, V. M., Striano, T., Kaufman, J. & Johnson, M. H. (2004) Eye gaze cueing facilitates neural processing of objects in 4-month-old infants. NeuroReport 15(16):2553–55. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200411150-00025.
Richardson, D. C., Dale, R. & Kirkham, N. Z. (2007a) The art of conversation is coordination: Common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue. Psychological Science 18(5):407–13. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01914.x.
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L. & Baron, R. M. (2007b) Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33(4):845–59. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.845.
Rietveld, E. (2008) Situated normativity: The normative aspect of embodied cognition in unreflective action. Mind 117(468):9731001. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzn050.
Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2010) The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(4):264–74. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2805.
Rochat, P. & Striano, T. (2002) Who's in the mirror? Self–other discrimination in specular images by four- and nine-month-old infants. Child Development 73(1):3546. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00390.
Rossetti, Y., Pisella, L. & Vighetto, A. (2003) Optic ataxia revisited: Visually guided action versus immediate visuomotor control. Experimental Brain Research 153(2):171–79. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1590-6.
Runeson, S. & Frykholm, G. (1983) Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 112(4):585615. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.112.4.585.
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A. & Barrueco, S. (1997) Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science 8(2):101105. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00690.x.
Saito, D. N., Tanabe, H. C., Izuma, K., Hayashi, M. J., Morito, Y., Komeda, H., Uchiyama, H., Kosaka, H., Okazawa, H., Fujibayashi, Y. & Sadato, N. (2010) “Stay tuned”: Inter-individual neural synchronization during mutual gaze and joint attention. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 4:127. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00127.
Santos, N. S., Kuzmanovic, B., David, N., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Eickhoff, S. B., Shah, J. N., Fink, G. R., Bente, G. & Vogeley, K. (2010) Animated brain: A functional neuroimaging study on the parametric induction of animacy experience. NeuroImage 53(1):291302. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.080.
Sarrazin, J.-C., Cleeremans, A. & Haggard, P. (2008) How do we know what we are doing? Time, intention, and awareness of action. Consciousness and Cognition 17(3):602–15. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.007.
Sartori, L., Becchio, C., Bulgheroni, M. & Castiello, U. (2009) Modulation of the action control system by social intention: Unexpected social requests override preplanned action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35(5):14901500. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015777.
Sato, A. & Yasuda, A. (2005) Illusion of sense of self-agency: Discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of actions modulates the sense of self-agency, but not the sense of self-ownership. Cognition 94(3):241–55. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.04.003.
Schilbach, L. (2010) A second-person approach to other minds. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(6):449.
Schilbach, L., Bzdok, D., Timmermans, B., Fox, P. T., Laird, A. R., Vogeley, K. & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012a) Minds at rest revisited: Using ALE meta-analyses to investigate commonalities in the neural correlates of socio-emotional processing and unconstrained cognition. PLoS One. 7(2):e30920.
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Cieslik, E. C., Kuzmanovic, B. & Vogeley, K. (2012b) Shall we do this together? Social gaze influences action control in a comparison group, but not in individuals with high-functioning autism. Autism: International Journal of Research and Practice 16(2):151–62. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1362361311409258.
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Cieslik, E., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R. & Vogeley, K. (2010a) Eyes on me: An fMRI study of the effects of social gaze on action control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6(4):393403. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq067.
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Mojzisch, A. & Vogeley, K. (2008a) What's in a smile? Neural correlates of facial embodiment during social interaction. Social Neuroscience 3(1):3750. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910701563228.
Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Fink, G. R. & Vogeley, K. (2008b) Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Consciousness and Cognition 17(2):457–67. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013.
Schilbach, L., Wilms, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Romanzetti, S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R. & Vogeley, K. (2010b) Minds made for sharing: Initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(12):2702–15. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21401.
Schilbach, L., Wohlschlaeger, A. M., Kraemer, N. C., Newen, A., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R. & Vogeley, K. (2006) Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction. Neuropsychologia 44(5):718–30. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.017.
Schippers, M. B. & Keysers, C. (2011) Mapping the flow of information within the putative mirror neuron system during gesture observation. NeuroImage 57(1):3744. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.018.
Schippers, M. B., Roebroeck, A., Renken, R., Nanetti, L. & Keysers, C. (2010) Mapping the information flow from one brain to another during gestural communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107(20):9388–93. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001791107.
Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C. & Turvey, M. T. (1990) Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16(2):227–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.16.2.227.
Schnell, K., Bluschke, S., Konradt, B. & Walter, H. (2011) Functional relations of empathy and mentalizing: An fMRI study on the neural basis of cognitive empathy. NeuroImage 54(2):1743–54. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.024.
Schonberg, T., Fox, C. R. & Poldrack, R. A. (2011) Mind the gap: Bridging economic and naturalistic risk-taking with cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(1):1119. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.002.
Schultz, R. T. (2005) Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 23:125.
Schutz, A. (1972) The phenomenology of the social world, trans. Walsh, G. & Lehnert, F.. Heinemann Educational Books.
Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H. & Knoblich, G. (2006) Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(2):7076. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009.
Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., Stumpf, L. & Prinz, W. (2005) Far from action-blind: Representation of others' actions in individuals with autism. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22(3–4):433–54. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000121.
Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S. & Frith, U. (2009) Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science 325(5942):883–85. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1176170.
Shockley, K., Santana, M. V. & Fowler, C. A. (2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29(2):326–32. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.326.
Short, J., Williams, E. & Christie, B (1976) The social psychology of telecommunications. Wiley.
Southgate, V. & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2008) Unbroken mirrors: Challenging a theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(6):225–29. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.005.
Sporns, O., Chialvo, D. R., Kaiser, M. & Hilgetag, C. C. (2004) Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(9):418–25. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.008.
Stel, M., van Dijk, E. & Olivier, E. (2009) You want to know the truth? Then don't mimic! Psychological Science 20(6):693–99. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02350.x.
Stern, D. (1985) The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic Books.
Striano, T., Kopp, C., Grossman, T. & Reid, V. (2006) Eye contact influences neural processing of emotional expressions in 4-month-old infants. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1(2):8794. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl008.
Suda, M., Takei, Y., Aoyama, Y., Narita, K., Sakurai, N., Fukuda, M. & Mikuni, M. (2011) Autistic traits and brain activation during face-to-face conversations in typically developed adults. PLoS ONE 6(5):e20021. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020021.
Suda, M., Takei, Y., Aoyama, Y., Narita, K., Sato, T., Fukuda, M. & Mikuni, M. (2010) Frontopolar activation during face-to-face conversation: An in situ study using near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuropsychologia 48(2):441–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.036.
Tamietto, M. & de Gelder, B. (2010) Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(10):697709. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2889.
Teufel, C., Fletcher, P. C. & Davis, G. (2010) Seeing other minds: Attributed mental states influence perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(8):376–82. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.005.
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press.
Thinès, G., Costall, A. & Butterworth, G. E. (1991) Michotte's experimental phenomenology of perception. Erlbaum.
Thompson, E. (2007) Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Harvard University Press.
Timmermans, B., Schilbach, L., Pasquali, A. & Cleeremans, A. (2012) Higher order thoughts in action: consciousness as an unconscious re-description process. Philosoptical Transactions of the Royal Society London B: Biological Science 19;367(1594):1412–23.
Tognoli, E., Lagarde, J., DeGuzman, G. C. & Kelso, J. A. (2007) The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(19):8190–95. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0611453104.
Tomasello, M. (1995) Joint attention as social cognition. In: Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, ed. Moore, C. & Dunham, P., pp. 103–30. Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M. (2009) Why we cooperate. MIT Press.
Tomasello, M. & Carpenter, M. (2007) Shared intentionality. Developmental Science 10(1):121–25. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x.
Tost, H., Braus, D. F., Hakimi, S., Ruf, M., Vollmert, C., Hohn, F. & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2010) Acute D2 receptor blockade induces rapid, reversible remodeling in human cortical–striatal circuits. Nature Neuroscience 13(8):920–22. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2572.
Trevarthen, C. (1977) Descriptive analyses of infant communication behavior. In: Studies in mother-infant interaction: The Loch Lomond Symposium, ed. Schaffer, H. R., pp. 227–70. Academic Press.
Trevarthen, C. (1980) The foundations of intersubjectivity: Development of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. In: The social foundations of language and thought: Essays in honor of J. S. Bruner, ed. Olson, D., pp. 316–42. Norton.
Triesch, J., Jasso, H. & Deak, G. O. (2007) Emergence of mirror neurons in a model of gaze following. Adaptive Behavior 15(2):149–65. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1059712307078654.
Triesch, J., Teuscher, C., Deak, G. O. & Carlson, E. (2006) Gaze following: Why (not) learn it? Developmental Science 9(2):125–47. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00470.x.
Triplett, N. (1898) The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. The American Journal of Psychology 9(4):507–33. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1412188.
Tylén, K., Weed, E., Wallentin, M., Roepstorff, A. & Frith, C. (2010) Language as a tool for interacting minds. Mind and Language 25(1):329. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01379.x.
Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C. & Keenan, J. P. (2007) The self and social cognition: The role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(4):153–57. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.01.001.
Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K. & van Knippenberg, A. (2004) Mimicry and pro-social behavior. Psychological Science 15(1):7177. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.x.
Van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., De Bouter, C. & Van Knippenberg, A. (2003) It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84(5):1093–102. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1093.
Vogeley, K. & Bente, G. (2010) “Artificial humans”: Psychology and neuroscience perspectives on embodiment and nonverbal communication. Neural Networks 23(8–9):1077–90. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.06.003.
Vogeley, K. & Newen, A. (2009) Consciousness of oneself and others in relation to mental disorders. In: The neuropsychology of mental illness, ed. Wood, S., Allen, N., Pantelis, C., pp. 408–13. Cambridge University Press. Available at: http://ebooks.cambridge.org/chapter.jsf?bid=CBO9780511642197&cid=CBO9780511642197A042.
Volman, I., Toni, I., Verhagen, L. & Roelofs, K. (2011) Endogenous testosterone modulates prefrontal-amygdala connectivity during social emotional behavior. Cerebral Cortex 21(10):2282–90. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr001.
Wang, Y., Ramsey, R. & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011) The control of mimicry by eye contact is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience 31(33):12001–10. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0845-11.2011.
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. & Thomas, M. (2007) Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science 10(1):7583. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x.
Wheatley, T., Milleville, S. C. & Martin, A. (2007) Understanding animate agents: Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychological Science 18(6):469–74. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01923.x.
Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Perra, O., Perrett, D. I. & Whiten, A. (2005) An fMRI study of joint attention experience. NeuroImage 25(1):133–40. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.047.
Wilms, M., Schilbach, L., Pfeiffer, U., Bente, G., Fink, G. R. & Vogeley, K. (2010) It's in your eyes – using gaze-contingent stimuli to create truly interactive paradigms for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(1):98107. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq024.
Wolff, P. (1987) The development of behavioral states and the expression of the emotions in early infancy. University of Chicago Press.
Worringham, C. J. & Messick, D. M. (1983) Social facilitation of running: An unobtrusive study. The Journal of Social Psychology 121(1):2329. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1983.9924462.
Yoshida, W., Dolan, R. J. & Friston, K. J. (2008) Game theory of mind. PLoS Computational Biology 4(12):e1000254. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000254.
Zahavi, D. (2005) Subjectivity and selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. The MIT Press.
Zahavi, D. & Parnas, J. (2003) Conceptual problems in infantile autism research: Why cognitive science needs phenomenology. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10(9–19):53571(19). Available at: http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/2003/00000010/F0020009/art00005.
Zajonc, R. B. (1965) Social facilitation. Science 149(3681):269–74.
Zaki, J. & Ochsner, K. (2009) The need for a cognitive neuroscience of naturalistic social cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1167:1630.
Zwickel, J. & Vo, M. L. (2010) How the presence of persons biases eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17(2):257–62. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.2.257.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed