Skip to main content
    • Aa
    • Aa
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 1
  • Cited by
    This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Maus, Gerrit W. Fischer, Jason and Whitney, David 2013. Motion-Dependent Representation of Space in Area MT+. Neuron, Vol. 78, Issue. 3, p. 554.


Visuomotor extrapolation

  • David Whitney (a1)
  • DOI:
  • Published online: 01 May 2008

Accurate perception of moving objects would be useful; accurate visually guided action is crucial. Visual motion across the scene influences perceived object location and the trajectory of reaching movements to objects. In this commentary, I propose that the visual system assigns the position of any object based on the predominant motion present in the scene, and that this is used to guide reaching movements to compensate for delays in visuomotor processing.

Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

B. Bridgeman (1995) A review of the role of efference copy in sensory and oculomotor control systems. Annals of Biomedical Engineering 23(4):409–22.

C. A. Buneo , M. R. Jarvis , A. P. Batista & R. A. Andersen (2002) Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching. Nature 416(6881):632–36.

J. D. Crawford , W. P. Medendorp & J. J. Marotta (2004) Spatial transformations for eye-hand coordination. Journal of Neurophysiology 92(1):1019.

R. L. De Valois & K. K. De Valois (1991) Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. Vision Research 31(9):1619–26.

S. Durant & A. Johnston (2004) Temporal dependence of local motion induced shifts in perceived position. Vision Research 44(4):357–66.

D. M. Eagleman & T. J. Sejnowski (2007) Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. Journal of Vision 7(4):3, 112.

H. Gomi , N. Abekawa & S. Nishida (2006) Spatiotemporal tuning of rapid interactions between visual-motion analysis and reaching movement. Journal of Neuroscience 26(20):5301–308.

L. Matin , K. Boff & J. Pola (1976) Vernier offset produced by rotary target motion. Perception & Psychophysics 20(2):138–42.

H. Öğmen , S. S. Patel , H. E. Bedell & K. Camuz (2004) Differential latencies and the dynamics of the position computation process for moving targets, assessed with the flash-lag effect. Vision Research 44:2109–28.

G. Purushothaman , S. S. Patel , H. E. Bedell & H. Öğmen (1998) Moving ahead through differential visual latency. Nature 396(6710):424.

V. S. Ramachandran & S. M. Anstis (1990) Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges. Perception 19(5):611–16.

D. Regan & K. I. Beverley (1984) Figure-ground segregation by motion contrast and by luminance contrast. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 1:433–42.

N. Saijo , I. Murakami , S. Nishida & H. Gomi (2005) Large-field visual motion directly induces an involuntary rapid manual following response. Journal of Neuroscience 25(20):4941–51.

R. J. Snowden (1998) Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion. Current Biology 8(24):1343–45.

K. Watanabe , R. Nijhawan & S. Shimojo (2002) Shifts in perceived position of flashed stimuli by illusory object motion. Vision Research 42(24):2645–50.

K. Watanabe , T. R. Sato & S. Shimojo (2003) Perceived shifts of flashed stimuli by visible and invisible object motion. Perception 32(5):545–59.

D. Whitney (2002) The influence of visual motion on perceived position. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(5):211–16.

D. Whitney & P. Cavanagh (2000) Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects. Nature Neuroscience 3:954–59.

D. Whitney & P. Cavanagh (2003) Motion adaptation shifts apparent position without the motion aftereffect. Perception & Psychophysics 65(7):1011–18.

D. Whitney , A. Ellison , N. J. Rice , D. Arnold , M. Goodale , V. Walsh & D. Milner (2007) Visually guided reaching depends on motion area MT+. Cerebral Cortex 17(11):2644–49.

D. Whitney & I. Murakami (1998) Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation. Nature Neuroscience 1(8):656–57.

D. Whitney , D. A. Westwood & M. A. Goodale (2003) The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object. Nature 423(6942):869–73.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *