Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-xdx58 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-11T13:17:34.826Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adapting neuroeconomics for environmental and energy policy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2018

NIK SAWE*
Affiliation:
Precourt Energy Efficiency Center and Emmett Interdisciplinary Program in Environment and Resources, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Neuroimaging methods provide insight into the neural mechanisms underlying the decision process, characterizing choice at the individual level and, in a growing number of contexts, predicting national- and market-level behavior. This dual capacity to examine heterogeneity while forecasting aggregate choice is particularly beneficial to those studying environmental decision-making. To effectively reduce residential energy usage and foster other pro-environmental behaviors, policy-makers must understand the effects of information frames and behavioral nudges across individuals who hold a diverse array of attitudes toward the environment and face a broad range of barriers to action. This paper articulates the potential of neuroeconomic methods to aid environmental policy-makers interested in behavior change, especially those interested in closing the energy efficiency gap. Investigation into the roles of affect, eco-labeling and social norms will be discussed, as well as personal identity and climate change beliefs. Combining neuroimaging with behavioral economics experiments can inform the development of effective messaging, characterize the influence of individual differences on the decision process and aid in forecasting the efficacy of policy interventions at scale.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Introduction

The human decisions – great and small – that can create severe local and global environmental challenges are often motivated and justified by economic rationales. In response, ecosystem services approaches have arisen to attempt to quantify the previously intrinsic and intangible benefits of natural resources in financial terms (Daily et al., Reference Daily, Polasky, Goldstein, Kareiva, Mooney, Pejchar, Ricketts, Salzman and Shallenberger2009). Eco-labeling, such as the Energy Guide and Energy Star programs, has been put in place by the US Environmental Protection Agency and others to direct consumers' attention to the potential economic benefits of buying energy-efficient appliances (US Environmental Protection Agency, 2016). Yet valuation measures for natural resources have encountered challenges generated by inconsistent preferences and strong emotional reactions (Sawe, Reference Sawe2017), and policy-makers still face an “energy efficiency gap” caused by consumer underinvestment in energy efficiency (Wilson & Dowlatabadi, Reference Wilson and Dowlatabadi2007). In the environmental domain, as in many other contexts, human behavior seldom aligns with the economic rational actor model (Gowdy, Reference Gowdy2008).

The importance of addressing the human behavioral elements of environmental problems has historically been overlooked by scientists and policy-makers (Gardner & Stern, Reference Gardner and Stern2002), but has received increased attention as behavioral economics and choice architecture approaches to public policy design have become more prevalent (Gowdy, Reference Gowdy2008; Shogren & Taylor, Reference Shogren and Taylor2008; Venkatachalam, Reference Venkatachalam2008). Recently, neuroeconomic methods, which combine behavioral economics experiments with brain imaging, have become especially promising for their potential to assess policy-relevant decision-making at the national scale, enabling population-level behavioral prediction using small neuroimaging samples in a growing number of contexts from microlending initiatives (Genevsky & Knutson, Reference Genevsky and Knutson2015) to the efficacy of anti-smoking ad campaigns (Falk et al., Reference Falk, Berkman and Lieberman2012). The ability of neuroeconomic methods to simultaneously characterize the individual heterogeneity that influences environmental decision-making (Sawe & Knutson, Reference Sawe and Knutson2015) and scale to predict market-level choice (Karmarkar & Yoon, Reference Karmarkar and Yoon2016) permits insight into specific versus generalizable responses. This may be especially crucial for environmental behavior, where the public exhibits a high degree of heterogeneity (Sahoo & Sawe, Reference Sahoo and Sawe2015) that can potentially be leveraged to achieve more sustainable outcomes (Dolnicar & Grun, Reference Dolnicar and Grun2009).

This article will delineate the potential benefits to environmental public policy of utilizing neuroimaging in concert with behavioral economics studies of environmental decision-making in order to yield sustainable behavior change, with a focus on meaningful reductions in energy usage. Many of the core elements to environmental decision-making have been well studied in neuroeconomics – how individuals allocate scarce resources (Lee, Reference Lee2008), deal with uncertain or ambiguous risks (Hsu et al., Reference Hsu, Bhatt, Adolphs, Tranel, Camerer, Adolphus, Tranel and Camerer2005) and make trade-offs between short- and long-term benefits (McClure, Reference McClure2004) or between moral and economic values (Berns et al., Reference Berns, Bell, Capra, Prietula, Moore, Anderson, Ginges and Atran2012) – as well as more direct applications such as consumer responses to organic labels (Linder et al., Reference Linder, Uhl, Fliessbach, Trautner, Elger and Weber2010) and philanthropic decisions to protect threatened natural resources (Sawe & Knutson, Reference Sawe and Knutson2015). A brief overview of the neural correlates of affect and experimental design using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) will be followed by discussions of potential contributions to the study of energy-efficient purchases, the influence of temporal discounting on residential energy usage, the roles of personal values and social norms and the impact of climate change beliefs.

Neuroimaging and the role of affect

Neuroeconomic methods can quantify, explain and even predict how components of the decision process can motivate an individual's departures from the choices of an economically rational actor. Distinct neural regions of interest have been found to correlate with positive and negative emotional responses, the computation of subjective value and the performance of cost–benefit assessment, math processing, social cognition and moral judgment and more (Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2007; Knutson & Greer, Reference Knutson and Greer2008; Kaufmann et al., Reference Kaufmann, Wood, Rubinsten and Henik2011).Footnote 1 Neuroimaging permits us to assess the extent and timing of these different influences on choice and how a given region's activity may change as a function of factors in the decision process (e.g., increases in the cost of an energy-efficient product), qualities of the individual (e.g., environmental attitudes) or interactions between these decision- and individual-level attributes.

The measurement of the role of affect, or emotion, in decision-making is perhaps one of the most relevant to studying environmental decisions (Sawe, Reference Sawe2017). Kahneman et al. (Reference Kahneman, Ilana and Schkade1999) have described responses to surveys attempting to elicit the value of natural resources as examples of “affective valuation”: rather than expressing economic preferences, respondents' willingness-to-pay (WTP) measurements instead act as a proxy for their emotional and attitudinal reactions. The other powerful motivator for studying affect when assessing the implications of environmental messaging and policy nudges is that neural activity in affective systems, specifically the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), is key to predicting behavior at the population level in many of the neuroimaging studies that have successfully forecasted aggregate choice (Berns & Moore, Reference Berns and Moore2011; Genevsky & Knutson, Reference Genevsky and Knutson2015; Venkatraman et al., Reference Venkatraman, Dimoka, Pavlou, Vo, Hampton, Bollinger, Hershfield, Ishihara, Winer, Venkatraman, Dimoka, Pavlou, Vo and Hampton2015; Kühn et al., Reference Kühn, Strelow and Gallinat2016; Genevsky et al., Reference Genevsky, Yoon and Knutson2017; Scholz et al., Reference Scholz, Baek, Brook, Donnell, Suk, Cappella and Falk2017). Thus, affective circuitry may be central to evaluating the effectiveness of such messaging and policy interventions at scale.

Affect is often modeled on a two-axis scale by psychologists, with one axis representing valence, or how positively or negatively the individual feels toward a given stimulus, and the other representing arousal, or how intensely this response is felt (Bradley & Lang, Reference Bradley and Lang1999). Elicitation of strong positive or negative arousal can evoke a reaction to approach or avoid that stimuli (Knutson & Greer, Reference Knutson and Greer2008). These appetitive or aversive reactions, rooted in evolutionary behavior, are driven by distinct neural systems (Knutson & Greer, Reference Knutson and Greer2008).

The affect–integration–motivation (AIM) framework (Samanez-Larkin & Knutson, Reference Samanez-Larkin and Knutson2015) serves as one basis for thinking about the role of affective brain circuits in decision-making. The striatum, specifically the NAcc, exhibits activity strongly associated with positive arousal (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Katovich and Suri2014) and responds to anticipation or delivery of a wide array of rewards, from food to sex to money (Knutson & Greer, Reference Knutson and Greer2008). When examining the NAcc's associations in existing studies with meta-analytic maps on the NeuroSynth database (www.neurosynth.org), this role in reward and incentive processing, instrumental in motivation, rises to the fore.

In contrast, the anterior insula's response is associated more with negative arousal toward losses, risks and stimuli that are aversive both physiologically and morally (Sanfey, Reference Sanfey2007; Knutson & Greer, Reference Knutson and Greer2008; Preuschoff et al., Reference Preuschoff, Quartz and Bossaerts2008), though there is evidence that the anterior insula is more responsive to arousal than valence, and thus more generally scales with salience (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Katovich and Suri2014).

These affective circuits play a central role in the initial assessment and weighting of the considered stimuli – for instance, NAcc activity often scales with reward magnitude (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Taylor, Kaufman, Peterson and Glover2005; Ballard & Knutson, Reference Ballard and Knutson2009; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Samanez-Larkin, Katovich and Knutson2014) – and this information is then passed to the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), which integrates affective responses with other contextual or conceptual considerations to arrive at a decision (Karmarkar & Yoon, Reference Karmarkar and Yoon2016). The mPFC, especially in ventral subregions, is implicated in many studies in such cost–benefit assessments and the calculation and indexing of subjective value (Montague et al., Reference Montague, King-Casas and Cohen2006; Grabenhorst & Rolls, Reference Grabenhorst and Rolls2011; Wallis, Reference Wallis2012; Bartra et al., Reference Bartra, McGuire and Kable2013; Clithero & Rangel, Reference Clithero and Rangel2013).

Experimental design using functional magnetic resonance imaging

A number of methods including electroencephalography and transcranial magnetic stimulation exist to deconstruct the roles of different brain regions, but our discussion of neuroeconomic methods to inform environmental policy will center on fMRI. FMRI is particularly suited to measuring activity in structures deep in the brain like the NAcc (Karmarkar & Yoon, Reference Karmarkar and Yoon2016) and is widely used as it strikes a balance between spatial and temporal resolution when collecting neural data (Cohen, Reference Cohen2005). This resolution is still limited to the scale of millimeters and seconds, in part because fMRI measures neural activity by proxy through the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal, which changes as oxygenated blood rushes to meet the metabolic demands of brain activity. Optogenetic research in animal models, which uses specific light wavelengths to activate neurons that have been modified via viral vectors, has been paired with fMRI to confirm that the BOLD signal can serve as an appropriate index of neural activity (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Durand, Gradinaru, Zhang, Goshen, Kim, Fenno, Ramakrishnan and Deisseroth2010).

Experimenters must strictly control the timing and complexity of the presented stimuli to yield interpretable neural responses to each factor within a given decision process. This is often achieved by breaking down an fMRI decision task into discrete packets of information, frequently presented for several seconds each. For instance, in an environmental philanthropy study (Sawe & Knutson, Reference Sawe and Knutson2015) where participants were asked to donate in order to protect a park from being utilized for a new proposed land use, participants were first shown the threatened park, then the land use, then the requested donation amount and finally the option to donate or not; information was presented additively in phases of four seconds each for a total trial length of 16 seconds. This sequential design permitted researchers to analyze brain response as a function of qualities of the park, the destructiveness of the proposed land use, the magnitude of the requested donation and the interactions of these factors.

More holistic approaches without this stepwise design are still possible if experimenters are focused on output models (e.g., identifying brain regions that predict choice). However, in complex multi-attribute decisions, this may obscure the mechanisms by which each attribute drives decision-making. In the context of sound policy design, understanding how a given messaging frame or behavioral nudge is successful at inspiring environmental behavior change is key to finding solutions across a diverse array of environmental problems and barriers to action.

Yet fMRI is not a skeleton key for studying environmental decision-making. To improve the signal-to-noise ratio, the same type of decision must be made numerous times by each subject (Huettel & McCarthy, Reference Huettel and McCarthy2001). Many environmental decisions that policy-makers may wish to study are not easily adapted to these repeated measures designs because they are essentially one-shots (e.g., voting on a given piece of legislation). For charitable giving or consumer purchasing paradigms, this is less problematic as the particulars of each decision (e.g., the energy efficiency of a given appliance) can be systematically varied in a fashion that maintains both participants' engagement and a sense of realism. Real decisions can also be studied in these contexts despite the high number of trials, because one or more decisions can be chosen at random to count as binding. For instance, if a purchase or donation was made, the amount can be removed from an endowment or payment for study participation. As the participant does not know which decision will be binding, they make each independently and under the assumption that it will be implemented.

FMRI's repeated measures design can be both beneficial and a potential confound, as it raises implicit comparisons across goods or services. Studies on joint evaluation of multiple goods (Kahneman & Ritov, Reference Kahneman and Ritov1994; Ritov & Kahneman, Reference Ritov and Kahneman1997; Kahneman et al., Reference Kahneman, Ilana and Schkade1999) have shown potentially drastic effects on WTP when evaluating multiple goods together or in sequence, rather than in isolation. However, this effect appears to be large when the goods are perceived to be in different categories, but minor when they are within the same category (Bonini et al., Reference Bonini, Ritov and Graffeo2008), as would be the case in many neuroimaging tasks. Even so, repeated trials may cause individuals to anchor to characteristics of early stimuli, creating problematic reference points that influence decision-making. Randomizing the presentation of stimuli across participants can help mitigate these order effects. More complex changes in decision-making strategies over time, as individuals attempt to compare and contrast information across trials, may be more difficult to model. This could be a source of confounds that are difficult to anticipate or control for experimentally. Alternatively, this dynamic evolution of choice strategies could indicate that subjects are calibrating their evaluation criteria as they gain familiarity with a given market (or choice set), offering a better representation of informed decision-making. This market familiarity may be more realistic in some contexts with environmental impacts (e.g., comparison shopping for refrigerators) than others (e.g., valuation of the worth of ecosystem services). Indeed, lack of familiarity with (or existence of) markets for environmental public goods is one of the great challenges facing environmental economists and policy-makers (Shogren & Taylor, Reference Shogren and Taylor2008).

Energy-efficient purchases and eco-labeling

Consumers routinely underinvest in energy efficiency, behaving as if they steeply discount future energy savings (Hausman, Reference Hausman1979; Train, Reference Train1985). US consumers have been shown to inadequately consider fuel costs during vehicle purchases (Allcott, Reference Allcott2011a), with approximately 30% of consumers ignoring fuel costs entirely (Leard, Reference Leard2013). This gives rise to the “Energy Efficiency Gap” or “Energy Paradox” (Jaffe & Stavins, Reference Jaffe and Stavins1994), where favorable technologies with beneficial ratios of capital investment to payback periods are underutilized (Wilson & Dowlatabadi, Reference Wilson and Dowlatabadi2007). Such systematic underinvestment is posited to slow the diffusion of energy-efficient products into the marketplace (Gillingham & Palmer, Reference Gillingham and Palmer2014). The potential returns on energy efficiency investment are considerable and far-reaching: a consulting report by McKinsey & Co. estimated that a $520 billion investment across US businesses and residences would yield returns of $1.2 trillion, reducing end-use energy consumption in 2020 by 23% of projected demand and abating up to 1.1 gigatons of greenhouse gas emissions annually (Granade et al., Reference Granade, Creyts, Derkach, Farese, Nyguist and Ostrowski2009). As 2020 draws closer, we have fallen short of this investment target.

This underinvestment in energy efficiency may be due in part to cognitive limitations. In a bounded rationality framework (Gigerenzer & Selten, Reference Gigerenzer and Selten2002; Kahneman, Reference Kahneman2003), our finite cognitive bandwidth prevents us from sustaining the perfect attentional focus and memory capacity to retain and evaluate all the information necessary for complex decisions, and thus we fall back on “maximizing” and “satisficing” heuristics to streamline the decision process (Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Ward, Monterosso, Lyubomirsky, White and Lehman2002). In energy efficiency decisions, this may lead to inattention to energy consumption data (Sahoo & Sawe, Reference Sahoo and Sawe2015). Such inattention to energy data may in fact be rational: Sallee (Reference Sallee2014) asserts that when consumer preferences for other product attributes (e.g., brand) are sufficiently strong, considerations of energy efficiency differences are insignificant, and finds that car consumers experience only a small welfare loss when making decisions without detailed fuel cost data. However, for the policy-maker who is concerned with reductions in energy use and greenhouse gas emissions, this inattentiveness to energy consumption data remains problematic.

One natural strategy to counteract this inattention is by increasing the salience of energy consumption data through the use of detailed eco-labels (e.g., EnergyGuide in the USA). Sallee (Reference Sallee2014) counters that these labels are still imperfect and contain uncertainty due to heterogeneity in usage patterns. Thus, more simplistic attentional cues, such as the binary Energy Star certification logo, may be a preferable alternative, balancing across salience, informational complexity and outcome (Newell & Siikamäki, Reference Newell and Siikamäki2013).

However, visual salience can come at a cost, overriding preferences to determine choice, especially under cognitive load or time constraints (Denny et al., Reference Denny, Kober, Wager and Ochsner2012). Houde (Reference Houde2011) provides evidence that the Energy Star label may crowd out efforts to assess electricity costs, acting as an informational substitute and prompting many individuals who would have invested more in energy efficiency in the label's absence to view Energy Star-certified products as “good enough.” Alternatively, the Energy Star could serve to focus consumers' attention on more concrete energy consumption data. In a discrete choice experiment, Sahoo and Sawe (Reference Sahoo and Sawe2015) found that, on average, the Energy Star increases the value that consumers place on energy consumption savings. These accounts need not be at odds; the Energy Star's effect on decision-making may differ across individuals. The study identified heterogeneity in the Energy Star label's effects, decreasing the weight placed on energy consumption in 13% of a national sample of homeowners (n = 1550), while increasing it in the majority (Sahoo & Sawe, Reference Sahoo and Sawe2015).

Neuroimaging may be helpful in determining the effect of these eco-labels on attention and energy consumption valuation. Stepwise presentation of information, as discussed earlier, would permit assessment of the response to an eco-label like the Energy Star, as well as the label's subsequent influence on the processing of energy data. The Energy Star elicits a price premium (Ward et al., Reference Ward, Clark, Jensen, Yen and Russell2011) similar to that found in an fMRI study of organic food labeling (Linder et al., Reference Linder, Uhl, Fliessbach, Trautner, Elger and Weber2010). It is likely that both labels operate through similar mechanisms, increasing NAcc activation, which is often highly predictive of purchasing behavior both within laboratory populations (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Rick, Wimmer, Prelec and Loewenstein2007) and when forecasting national behavior (Berns & Moore, Reference Berns and Moore2011; Kühn et al., Reference Kühn, Strelow and Gallinat2016). We might also hypothesize that the Energy Star's presence would be correlated with changes in activity in the mPFC when energy consumption data are shown due to the region's role in cost–benefit assessment and value computation. If the Energy Star facilitates closer consideration of energy consumption, mPFC activity may increase in instances where the label is present.

While the NAcc and mPFC act as neural correlates of positive arousal and cost–benefit assessment, and also correlate with WTP (Knutson et al., Reference Knutson, Rick, Wimmer, Prelec and Loewenstein2007; Plassmann et al., Reference Plassmann, O'Doherty and Rangel2007; Linder et al., Reference Linder, Uhl, Fliessbach, Trautner, Elger and Weber2010), there is a more direct path to studying how the attentional effects of eco-labeling are related to energy information processing. FMRI has been combined with eye-tracking to study the relationships between visual attention, neural activity and consumer decisions (Lim et al., Reference Lim, O'Doherty and Rangel2011). This multimethod approach could be used to assess the degree to which attention to eco-labeling like the Energy Star enhances or detracts from the consideration of concrete energy data. Moving beyond binary labels to information-rich alternatives, it becomes even more crucial to understand attentional dynamics. The EnergyGuide's complex provision of energy data presents a multitude of numbers, including annual electricity use in kWh, cost ranges for similar models and annual operating cost (for some products, these are presented twice with different cost estimates for electric versus natural gas water heaters). These translated attributes are a form of choice architecture, serving as signposts for individuals to understand how a product's energy efficiency aligns with their values and goals (Ungemach et al., Reference Ungemach, Camilleri, Johnson, Larrick and Weber2017). Understanding the degree to which this complexity in energy labeling impacts decision-making, and whether a parsimonious optimum might be achieved between the levels of detail proffered in the Energy Star and EnergyGuide labels could be an important contribution to the design and optimization of new ecolabels.

Beyond optimization, neuroimaging's ability to forecast population-level behavior offers the greatest potential for policy-makers. FMRI studies have successfully predicted sales of music and chocolate (Berns & Moore, Reference Berns and Moore2011; Kühn et al., Reference Kühn, Strelow and Gallinat2016), the success of advertisements (Venkatraman et al., Reference Venkatraman, Dimoka, Pavlou, Vo, Hampton, Bollinger, Hershfield, Ishihara, Winer, Venkatraman, Dimoka, Pavlou, Vo and Hampton2015), microloan and crowdfunding ventures (Genevsky & Knutson, Reference Genevsky and Knutson2015; Genevsky et al., Reference Genevsky, Yoon and Knutson2017) and public health campaigns (Falk et al., Reference Falk, O'Donnel, Tompson, Gonzalez, Cin, Strecher, Cummings and An2015), generally through some combination of NAcc and mPFC activity.

If neuroimaging studies can establish robust prediction of national sales of appliances with energy-efficient and eco-labeled options and verify the neural circuits involved in this prediction, these can serve as targets for optimization. For example, if NAcc activation predicts national sales, then future eco-labels can be chosen in part based on their ability to elicit NAcc activation. While neuroimaging studies that predict national behavior to date have used pre-existing stimuli (e.g., public crowdfunding campaigns, products or advertisements), such techniques could be used earlier in the design process to decide on energy labeling or environmental messaging before deployment as an A/B testing ground. Because fMRI designs can dissociate the influence of different informational attributes on both choice and neural activity, labels or other messaging could be deconstructed across a number of dimensions to understand which constituent elements are the most impactful at eliciting neural activity that scales to predict choice at the population level. Of course, the goal of the policy-maker is not purely to increase appliance sales, but rather to increase the market share of products that offer the largest gains in energy efficiency. This argues for balanced labeling schemes that not only elicit activation patterns that are the most predictive of success, but also retain enough focus on energy consumption gains that consumers can make informed decisions.

Residential energy usage and intertemporal trade-offs

Energy economists have found variation in the individual financial discount rates implied in purchasing behavior for different domestic energy technologies, clustering in the 5–40% range, but reaching as high as 300% for air conditioning (Wilson & Dowlatabadi, Reference Wilson and Dowlatabadi2007). This suggests both heterogeneity in individual discount rates within the population as well as heterogeneity that is dependent on the type of appliance. One can imagine a number of contingencies that may contribute to such disparities; air conditioners may be bought when the need for them is greatest, leading to greater inattention to product specifics and more frequent use of satisficing heuristics. Similarly, pragmatic constraints (e.g., dimensions of the appliance) or brand dominance may overwhelm efficiency considerations (Young et al., Reference Young, Hwang, Mcdonald and Oates2010) and lead to higher implicit discount rates in specific appliance types. By granting the ability to disaggregate responses to individual product attributes, neuroeconomic experiments can identify potential disparities in neural response and subsequent behavior between appliance types. Armed with an understanding the dynamics underlying this heterogeneity, policy-makers can evaluate whether a one-size-fits-all approach to eco-labeling and information provision is appropriate or whether different messaging or behavioral nudges need to be deployed based on the specific product.

Neuroeconomic experiments can also address differences in discount rates between individuals. Addressing this heterogeneity could be of paramount importance to messaging approaches, with individuals who possess high financial discount rates (i.e., those who steeply devalue future profits against near-term gains) potentially also devaluing information about annual energy operating costs, since research has found no significant difference in individuals' discount rates in financial versus environmental domains (Hardisty & Weber, Reference Hardisty and Weber2009). These individuals may need to be reached through alternative methods, perhaps via emotional appeals or by emphasizing financial or environmental losses, which are less subject to discounting than gains (Hardisty & Weber, Reference Hardisty and Weber2009).

Temporal discounting has been well-studied in neuroeconomics (McClure, Reference McClure2004; McClure et al., Reference McClure, Ericson, Laibson, Loewenstein and Cohen2007; Ballard & Knutson, Reference Ballard and Knutson2009), with the NAcc appearing more responsive to immediate rewards and particularly sensitive to reward magnitude. This predilection toward short-term rewards is unsurprising given the striatum's involvement in addictive and impulsive behaviors (Diekhof et al., Reference Diekhof, Falkai and Gruber2008; Ballard & Knutson, Reference Ballard and Knutson2009). Yet the NAcc's association with positive arousal (Knutson & Greer, Reference Knutson and Greer2008) may also mean that positive emotional messages, or those that appeal to impulsivity (e.g., by offering an evaluative shortcut), may help close the energy efficiency gap in high discounters. Since we have seen that eco-labeling can increase WTP for goods via NAcc activation (Linder et al., Reference Linder, Uhl, Fliessbach, Trautner, Elger and Weber2010), it is entirely possible that the Energy Star elicits its price premium through the same mechanism. By measuring individual discount rates either inside or outside of fMRI and then evaluating appliance purchases, researchers could indeed evaluate whether different types of energy information have differential influences on individuals based on their discount rate. With most consumers acting “as if” they have a very high discount rate where energy efficiency is concerned, this could be a powerful optimization tool.

Personal values, social signaling and social norms

Beyond discount rates, it is important to identify the degree to which policy interventions and behavioral nudges are effective across a population that possesses a diverse range of environmental attitudes. Individuals endeavor to make decisions that are consistent with their own self-identity, and the degree to which they self-identify as an environmentalist can, naturally enough, correlate with pro-environmental behaviors (Heimlich & Ardoin, Reference Heimlich and Ardoin2008).

Neuroimaging of environmental charitable giving has revealed that widely used survey measures of pro-environmental attitudes such as the revised New Ecological Paradigm (NEP) scale (Dunlap et al., Reference Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig and Jones2000) can be used to examine how neural response during decision-making changes as a function of differences in these environmental attitudes (Sawe & Knutson, Reference Sawe and Knutson2015). The study showed that individuals with stronger pro-environmental attitudes exhibited increased anterior insula activity when they were shown proposed land uses that had a higher destructive impact on the natural landscape (e.g., mining), and this anterior insula activity predicted a greater likelihood of donation to block the destructive land uses and conserve the natural landscapes across the study population (Sawe & Knutson, Reference Sawe and Knutson2015). Notably, NAcc activation in response to positive environmental stimuli (e.g., iconic landscapes) did not significantly differ as a function of pro-environmental attitudes. This example emphasizes the capacity of neuroimaging to distinguish the specific mechanisms by which attitudes can translate into actions, enabling policy-makers to better understand when and how to apply behavioral levers.

Nor is this capacity limited to cases of “preaching to the choir,” describing only the behavior of strongly pro-environmental individuals. Neuroimaging studies could reveal the mechanisms by which messaging can reach those who care little for the environment, but who may be moved through appeals to other salient identities, as in the famously successful “Don't Mess with Texas” anti-littering campaign that appealed to citizens' state pride. As discussed in an eco-labeling context, messaging can be compared and deconstructed to study how variations in each component can influence neural activity and subsequent decision-making. But as the study of environmental philanthropy shows, the efficacy of each component at eliciting pro-environmental behaviors can be assessed as a function of individual differences and attitudes. This process could, for example, identify messaging components that may be aversive to individuals with specific beliefs or attitudes, but rarely explicitly discussed through other modalities (e.g., behavioral focus groups).

However, tailoring behavioral nudges in such a way can be interpreted as paternalistic or not directly beneficial to the target population (Hausman & Welch, Reference Hausman and Welch2010). This suggests that the ethical implications and welfare benefits of each potential application of nudges (identity-related and otherwise) should be articulated and debated, especially when leveraging insights from neuroscience studies.

However, in cases where pro-environmental behaviors are consistent with an individual's identity, social norms and spillover effects can have powerful impacts on the adoption and diffusion of positive behaviors and technological solutions. This is especially true of high-visibility actions such as the adoption of solar photovoltaic panels; Bollinger and Gillingham (Reference Bollinger and Gillingham2012) find that an additional solar panel installation increases the likelihood of another installation in the same zip code by 0.78%. Consistent with Ajzen's Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB), consumers are more likely to adopt behaviors if they perceive them to be a social norm (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen1991). TPB has also been employed to describe and predict electric vehicle adoption patterns (Lane & Potter, Reference Lane and Potter2007).

Academic work showing the influence of social norms on energy and water conservation (Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein and Griskevicius2007; Goldstein et al., Reference Goldstein, Cialdini and Griskevicius2008; Nolan et al., Reference Nolan, Schultz, Cialdini, Goldstein and Griskevicius2008) has led to a growing focus on social comparisons by startups and utility companies aiming to reduce energy and water use. Opower is one of the most frequently cited success stories, using comparisons to the energy use of similar households to incite residential energy use reductions of approximately 2.0% (Allcott, Reference Allcott2011b). Promisingly, these reductions appear to sustain over time through at least one year of follow-up (Ayres et al., Reference Ayres, Raseman and Shih2013).

Social cognition recruits an array of brain regions that often activate together in networks, with specific regions appearing to process theory of mind (e.g., temporoparietal junction and posterior superior temporal sulcus), moral decision-making (e.g., dorsal mPFC) and other aspects of complex social interactions (for a detailed account, see reviews from Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2007; Doré et al., Reference Doré, Zerubavel and Ochsner2015; Tremblay et al., Reference Tremblay, Sharika and Platt2017, among others). Of particular interest to the instantiation of environmental social norms, a meta-analysis of fMRI studies explicitly assessed the neural correlates of social conformity (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Luo and Feng2016). Their findings suggest an affective account similar to the AIM framework: when individual actions deviated from group norms, this was accompanied by deactivation of the ventral striatum and increased activation in the anterior insula and dorsal mPFC (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Luo and Feng2016). This implied negative emotional responses toward deviating from group behavior, and furthermore, the dorsal mPFC activity predicted that people would adjust their behavior to conform (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Luo and Feng2016). Lastly, stimuli that were endorsed by others elicited a stronger response in the ventral striatum (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Luo and Feng2016). Given the region's role in assessing the magnitude of rewarding stimuli during valuation, this suggests that social endorsement adds positive weight to the behaviors under consideration.

These findings provide a foundation for experimentation to better understand the influence of social norms on pro-environmental behavior, especially the framing effects that may impact this relationship. For instance, adding a simple emoticon – a smiling face to denote good energy use performance – alongside social comparison information was sufficient to prevent a rebound effect (where individuals who received positive feedback would increase energy usage) (Schultz et al., Reference Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein and Griskevicius2007). Such messaging variations could be tested in a similar fashion to the eco-labels discussed earlier in order to provide a more complete picture of the evoked mechanisms. For instance, does the emoticon evoke higher dorsal mPFC or ventral striatal activity during consideration, and can this activity predict reduced rebound effects and sustained behavior change? If activation in one circuit is increased but not the other, what implications does this have for how social norms impact environmental behavior and the framing effects that should be employed or avoided?

Climate change belief and pro-environmental behaviors

A discussion of environmental decision-making would be remiss if it did not address climate change, especially where intertemporal trade-offs are concerned. Climate change suffers from yet another paradox: while polling finds that the majority of the USA views its risks as a serious problem, it also repeatedly ranks near the bottom in terms of priorities (Leiserowitz, Reference Leiserowitz2006). In another facet of bounded rationality, a “finite pool of worry” forces us to prioritize which risks we address, and increasing concern for one risk can decrease concern for another (Weber, Reference Weber2006). Unfortunately for climate change, many of the dimensions upon which humans prioritize risks – ones that happen in the here and now, to us personally and for certain – do not work in its favor (Gattig & Hendrickx, Reference Gattig and Hendrickx2007), with outcomes that are ambiguous in their timeline, exact nature, geographic impact and severity.

This ambiguity and uncertainty can compound as one transitions from climate change predictions to impact assessments, in what has previously been called a cascade of uncertainty or an uncertainty explosion (Jones, Reference Jones2000). Ellsberg (Reference Ellsberg1961) first provided evidence of aversion to ambiguity where our decision-making curves to avoid ambiguous circumstances. Information credibility and disagreement among experts have since been cited as key sources of ambiguity (Camerer & Weber, Reference Camerer and Weber1992) and likely contribute greatly to public disengagement with climate change. Furthermore, unfavorable information under ambiguity is less likely to be integrated into decision-making when open to subjective interpretation (Peysakhovich & Karmarkar, Reference Peysakhovich and Karmarkar2015), promoting dismissal of cautionary climate change data.

The demotivation caused by the perceived ambiguity of climate change risk is further compounded by individual beliefs about the inefficacy of their personal environmental actions (Cleveland et al., Reference Cleveland, Kalamas and Laroche2005), as well as poor conceptions of the actual energy impacts of different behaviors (off by a factor of 2.8 on average; Attari et al., Reference Attari, DeKay, Davidson and Bruine de Bruin2010).

Neuroimaging may provide insight into the roles of self-efficacy and ambiguity as barriers to pro-environmental behavior. Anecdotal discussions with participants in an fMRI study of environmental philanthropy (Sawe & Knutson, Reference Sawe and Knutson2015) indicated that those who rarely donated rationalized their actions by commenting on the small scale of their impact relative to the scale of the problem; low donation was associated with increased mPFC activity, indicating that the process of weighing costs and benefits during value calculation may have convinced individuals that self-interested behavior trumped pro-environmental actions. Measures of environmental locus of control (Cleveland et al., Reference Cleveland, Kalamas and Laroche2005) could be combined with neuroimaging to more explicitly test this relationship.

FMRI comparison of ambiguity versus probabilistic uncertainty (Hsu et al., Reference Hsu, Bhatt, Adolphs, Tranel, Camerer, Adolphus, Tranel and Camerer2005) reveals that ambiguous circumstances preferentially elicit activation in the amygdala, often correlated with fear and aversion responses (Kang & Camerer, Reference Kang and Camerer2013), dorsal mPFC (implicated in modulation of amygdala activity) and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (implicated in emotional and cognitive information integration). In contrast, the striatum responded more to probabilistic risk. This provides some foundation for study of the degree to which ambiguity- or even fear-based processing influences disengagement from climate change messaging and associated low engagement in pro-environmental behaviors. If it is found that perceived ambiguity in the extent and severity of climate change outcomes is related to amygdala activity, messaging approaches and information presentation methods that successfully shift individuals to instead recruit circuitry involved in the assessment of probabilistic risks may encourage more strategic and engaged environmental decision-making. More broadly, if neuroscientists find a replicable neural response to ambiguity in environmental decision contexts that mirrors findings in other tasks (including attempting to predict temperatures; Hsu et al., Reference Hsu, Bhatt, Adolphs, Tranel, Camerer, Adolphus, Tranel and Camerer2005), this could be used to understand when, how, and why ambiguity aversion may be influencing the public's environmental choices.

Ultimately, discussion of climate change is a political one, and belief in climate change risk has been shown to transcend an individual's science literacy and numeracy, relating instead to their liberal or conservative values (and indeed, polarizing in the USA along party lines as individuals become more learned) (Kahan et al., Reference Kahan, Peters, Braman, Slovic, Wittlin, Ouellette and Mandel2011). Policy-makers will have to decide the degree to which climate change should be invoked in messaging when engaging individuals to undertake pro-environmental actions. Here, too, fMRI's capacity to not only examine individual differences in decision-making, but also to predict population-level responses to campaign appeals (e.g., to quit smoking; Falk et al., Reference Falk, Berkman and Lieberman2012) can facilitate an understanding of which messaging may be most impactful or most generalizable.

Conclusion

Neuroimaging studies have examined a rapidly expanding array of decision-making contexts in recent years, but environmental applications remain relatively unexplored. However, neuroeconomic areas of study such as consumer behavior and temporal discounting have clear implications for how humanity makes decisions with significant environmental impacts. Environmental choice paradigms can similarly benefit neuroeconomic researchers by providing a complex, real-world context where neuroimaging's capacity for prediction and understanding of mechanisms can benefit policy-makers and practitioners. In particular, the development of behavioral nudges and better information provision methods to optimize individual decisions surrounding energy efficiency is a promising area of research, with outcomes that both save money for the individual and benefit the environment.

The primary benefits of neuroimaging studies for environmental policy fall broadly into three categories: understanding how constituent elements of informational and behavioral nudges can influence the decision process; understanding how decision-making processes may differ across individuals; and employing neural data as a tool to predict national behavior. These may overlap and interact: for instance, seeing how different types of individuals react to specific messaging elements, or what types of decision processes are the most predictive of national behavior. Armed with a more detailed understanding of the decision process and its behavioral drivers, environmental policy-makers can design more precise and tailored policies and behavioral interventions to meet the needs of a diverse population.

Neuroeconomic methods should be considered as a complement to more traditional survey and econometric methods of studying environmental decision-making, offering a window into the mind as we each make choices that, in aggregate, have far-reaching consequences across the globe.

Footnotes

1 While care must be taken to avoid reverse inference and overly specific interpretations of the functional roles of different regions, meta-analyses and related tools (e.g., NeuroSynth) provide insight into broader categories of functionality.

References

Ajzen, I. (1991), ‘The theory of planned behavior’, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50: 179211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.Google Scholar
Allcott, H. (2011a), ‘Consumers' perceptions and misperceptions of energy costs’, American Economic Review, 101: 98104. doi:10.1257/aer.101.3.98.Google Scholar
Allcott, H. (2011b), ‘Social norms and energy conservation’, Journal of Public Economics, 95: 10821095. doi:10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003.Google Scholar
Attari, S. Z., DeKay, M. L., Davidson, C. I. and Bruine de Bruin, W. (2010), ‘Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107: 1605416059. doi:10.1073/pnas.1001509107.Google Scholar
Ayres, I., Raseman, S. and Shih, A. (2013), ‘Evidence from Two Large Field Experiments that Peer Comparison Feedback Can Reduce Residential Energy Usage’, The Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization, doi:10.1093/jleo/ews020.Google Scholar
Ballard, K. and Knutson, B. (2009), ‘Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting’, Neuroimage, 45: 143150. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.004.Google Scholar
Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T. and Kable, J. W. (2013), ‘The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value’, Neuroimage, 76: 412427.Google Scholar
Berns, G. S., Bell, E., Capra, C. M., Prietula, M. J., Moore, S., Anderson, B., Ginges, J. and Atran, S. (2012), ‘The price of your soul: neural evidence for the non-utilitarian representation of sacred values’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367: 754–62.Google Scholar
Berns, G. S. and Moore, S. E. (2011), ‘A neural predictor of cultural popularity’, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.05.001.Google Scholar
Bollinger, B. and Gillingham, K. (2012), ‘Peer Effects in the Diffusion of Solar Photovoltaic Panels’, Marketing Science, 31: 900912.Google Scholar
Bonini, N., Ritov, I. and Graffeo, M. (2008), ‘When does a referent public problem affect financial and political support for public action?’, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, doi:10.1002/bdm.580.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M. and Lang, P. J. (1999), ‘Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings’, Univ. Florida Cent. Res. Psychophysiol.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. and Weber, M. (1992), ‘Recent developments in modeling preferences: Uncertainty and ambiguity’, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5: 325370. doi:10.1007/BF00122575.Google Scholar
Cleveland, M., Kalamas, M. and Laroche, M. (2005), ‘Shades of green: linking environmental locus of control and pro-environmental behaviors’, Journal of Consumer Marketing, 22: 198212. doi:10.1108/07363760510605317.Google Scholar
Clithero, J. A. and Rangel, A. (2013), ‘Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9: 12891302.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. D. (2005), ‘The Vulcanization of the Human Brain: A Neural Perspective on Interactions Between Cognition and Emotion’, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19: 324. doi:10.1257/089533005775196750.Google Scholar
Daily, G. C., Polasky, S., Goldstein, J., Kareiva, P. M., Mooney, H. A., Pejchar, L., Ricketts, T. H., Salzman, J. and Shallenberger, R. (2009), ‘Ecosystem services in decision making: time to deliver’, Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 7: 2128. doi:10.1890/080025.Google Scholar
Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D. and Ochsner, K. N. (2012), ‘A Meta-analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Self- and Other Judgments Reveals a Spatial Gradient for Mentalizing in Medial Prefrontal Cortex’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00233.Google Scholar
Diekhof, E. K., Falkai, P. and Gruber, O. (2008), ‘Functional neuroimaging of reward processing and decision-making: A review of aberrant motivational and affective processing in addiction and mood disorders’, Brain Research Reviews, 59: 164184. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.004.Google Scholar
Dolnicar, S. and Grun, B. (2009), ‘Environmentally Friendly Behavior: Can Heterogeneity Among Individuals and Contexts/ Environments Be Harvested for Improved Sustainable Management?’, Environment and Behavior, 41: 693714. doi:10.1177/0013916508319448.Google Scholar
Doré, B. P., Zerubavel, N. and Ochsner, K. N. (2015), ‘Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core systems’, APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology. Volume 1: Attitudes and Social Cognition, 1: 693720. doi:10.1037/14341-022.Google Scholar
Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G. and Jones, R. E. (2000), ‘Measuring Endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A Revised NEP Scale’, Journal of Social Issues, 56: 425442. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00176.Google Scholar
Ellsberg, D. (1961), ‘Risk, Ambiguity, and the Savage Axioms’, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, doi:10.2307/1884324.Google Scholar
Falk, E., O'Donnel, M., Tompson, S., Gonzalez, R., Cin, S., Strecher, V., Cummings, K. and An, L. (2015), ‘Functional brain imaging predicts public health campaign success’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. e-ahead of, 111. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv108.Google Scholar
Falk, E. B., Berkman, E. T. and Lieberman, M. D. (2012), ‘From Neural Responses to Population Behavior: Neural Focus Group Predicts Population-Level Media Effects’, Psychological Science, 23: 439–45. doi:10.1177/0956797611434964.Google Scholar
Gardner, G. T. and Stern, P. C. (2002), Environmental Problems and Human Behavior, 2nd Ed. ed. Pearson Custom Publishing, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Gattig, A. and Hendrickx, L. (2007), ‘Judgmental Discounting and Environmental Risk Perception: Dimensional Similarities, Domain Differences, and Implications for Sustainability’, Journal of Social Issues, 63: 2139. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00494.x.Google Scholar
Genevsky, A. and Knutson, B. (2015), ‘Neural Affective Mechanisms Predict Market-Level Microlending’, Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Genevsky, A. and Knutson, B. (2015), ‘Neural affective mechanisms predict market-level microlending’, Psychological Science, 26: 14111422. doi:10.1177/0956797615588467.Google Scholar
Genevsky, A., Yoon, C. and Knutson, B. (2017), ‘When brain beats behavior: Neuroforecasting crowdfunding outcomes’, Journal of Neuroscience, 37: 1633–16. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1633-16.2017.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. and Selten, R. (Eds.) (2002), Bounded Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gillingham, K. and Palmer, K. (2014), ‘Bridging the Energy Efficiency Gap: Policy Insights from Economic Theory and Empirical Evidence’, Review of Environmental Economics and Policy, 8: 1838. doi:10.1093/reep/ret021.Google Scholar
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B. and Griskevicius, V. (2008), ‘A Room with a Viewpoint: Using Social Norms to Motivate Environmental Conservation in Hotels’, Journal of Consumer Research, 35: 472482. doi:10.1086/586910.Google Scholar
Gowdy, J. M. (2008), ‘Behavioral economics and climate change policy’, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 68: 632644. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2008.06.011.Google Scholar
Grabenhorst, F. and Rolls, E. T. (2011), ‘Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal cortex’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15: 5667. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.004.Google Scholar
Granade, H. C., Creyts, J., Derkach, A., Farese, P., Nyguist, S. and Ostrowski, K. (2009), ‘Unlocking Energy Efficiency in the U.S. Economy’, McKinsey Global Energy and Materials, 1165. doi:10.1016/j.enbuild.2006.06.009.Google Scholar
Hardisty, D. J. and Weber, E. U. (2009), ‘Discounting future green: money versus the environment’, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 138: 329–40. doi:10.1037/a0016433.Google Scholar
Hausman, D. M. and Welch, B. (2010), ‘Debate: To nudge or not to nudge’, Journal of Political Philosophy, 18: 123136. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9760.2009.00351.x.Google Scholar
Hausman, J. A. (1979), ‘Individual Discount Rates and the Purchase and Utilization of Energy-Using Durables’, Bell Journal of Economics, 10: 3354. doi:10.2307/3003318.Google Scholar
Heimlich, J. and Ardoin, N. (2008), ‘Understanding behavior to understand behavior change: a literature review’, Environmental Education Research, 14: 215237. doi:10.1080/13504620802148881.Google Scholar
Houde, S. (2011), ‘How Consumers Respond to Product Certification: A Welfare Analysis of the Energy Star Program’, Work. Pap.Google Scholar
Hsu, M., Bhatt, M., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Camerer, C. F., Adolphus, R., Tranel, D. and Camerer, C. F. (2005), ‘Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making’, Science 310: 16801683. doi:10.1126/science.1115327.Google Scholar
Huettel, S. and McCarthy, G. (2001), ‘The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fMRI activation’, Neuroreport, 12: 24112416.Google Scholar
Jaffe, A. B. and Stavins, R. N. (1994), ‘The energy-efficiency gap What does it mean?’, Energy Policy, 22: 804810. doi:10.1016/0301-4215(94)90138-4.Google Scholar
Jones, R. N. (2000), ‘Managing uncertainty in climate change projections–issues for impact assessment’, Climatic Change, doi:10.1016/j.urology.2013.09.061.Google Scholar
Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Braman, D., Slovic, P., Wittlin, M., Ouellette, L. L. and Mandel, G. (2011), ‘The Tragedy of the Risk-Perception Commons: Culture Conflict, Rationality Conflict, and Climate Change’, Cult. Cogn. Proj. Work. Pap.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2003), ‘Maps of Bounded Rationality: Psychology for Behavioral Economics’, American Economic Review, 93: 14491475.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Ilana, R. and Schkade, D. (1999), ‘Economic Preferences or Attitude Expressions?: An Analysis of Dollar Responses to Public Issues’, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19: 203235.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Ritov, I. (1994), ‘Determinants of stated willingness to pay for public goods: A study in the headline method’, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, doi:10.1007/BF01073401.Google Scholar
Kang, M. J. and Camerer, C. F. (2013), ‘FMRI evidence of a hot-cold empathy gap in hypothetical and real aversive choices’, Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7: 104. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00104.Google Scholar
Karmarkar, U. R. and Yoon, C. (2016), ‘Consumer neuroscience: Advances in understanding consumer psychology’, Current Opinion in Psychology, 10: 160165. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.01.010.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, L., Wood, G., Rubinsten, O. and Henik, A. (2011), ‘Meta-analyses of developmental fMRI studies investigating typical and atypical trajectories of number processing and calculation’, Developmental Neuropsychology, 36: 763787. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.549884.Google Scholar
Knutson, B. and Greer, S. M. (2008), ‘Anticipatory affect: neural correlates and consequences for choice’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363: 37713786.Google Scholar
Knutson, B., Katovich, K. and Suri, G. (2014), ‘Inferring affect from fMRI data’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18: 422428.Google Scholar
Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G. E., Prelec, D. and Loewenstein, G. (2007), ‘Neural Predictors of Purchases’, Neuron, 53: 147156.Google Scholar
Knutson, B., Taylor, J., Kaufman, M., Peterson, R. and Glover, G. (2005), ‘Distributed neural representation of expected value’, Journal of Neuroscience, 25: 48064812.Google Scholar
Kühn, S., Strelow, E. and Gallinat, J. (2016), ‘Multiple “buy buttons” in the brain: Forecasting chocolate sales at point-of-sale based on functional brain activation using fMRI’, Neuroimage, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.021.Google Scholar
Lane, B. and Potter, S. (2007), ‘The adoption of cleaner vehicles in the UK: exploring the consumer attitude-action gap’, Journal of Cleaner Production, 15: 10851092. doi:10.1016/j.jclepro.2006.05.026.Google Scholar
Leard, B. (2013), Consumer Heterogeneity and the Energy Paradox.Google Scholar
Lee, D. (2008), ‘Game theory and neural basis of social decision making’, Nature Neuroscience, 11: 404409. doi:10.1038/nn2065.Google Scholar
Lee, J. H., Durand, R., Gradinaru, V., Zhang, F., Goshen, I., Kim, D.-S., Fenno, L. E., Ramakrishnan, C. and Deisseroth, K. (2010), ‘Global and local fMRI signals driven by neurons defined optogenetically by type and wiring’, Nature, 465: 788792. doi:10.1038/nature09108.Google Scholar
Leiserowitz, A. (2006), ‘Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and values’, in Climatic Change, 4572. doi:10.1007/s10584-006-9059-9.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2007), ‘Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes’, Annual Review of Psychology, 58: 259–89.Google Scholar
Lim, S.-L., O'Doherty, J. P. and Rangel, A. (2011), ‘The Decision Value Computations in the vMPFC and Striatum Use a Relative Value Code That is Guided by Visual Attention’, Journal of Neuroscience, 31: 1321413223. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1246-11.2011.Google Scholar
Linder, N. S., Uhl, G., Fliessbach, K., Trautner, P., Elger, C. E. and Weber, B. (2010), ‘Organic labeling influences food valuation and choice’, Neuroimage, 53: 215220. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.077.Google Scholar
McClure, S. M. (2004), ‘Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards’, Science (80-.), 306: 503507. doi:10.1126/science.1100907.Google Scholar
McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G. and Cohen, J. D. (2007), ‘Time discounting for primary rewards’, Journal of Neuroscience, 27: 57965804.Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., King-Casas, B. and Cohen, J. D. (2006), ‘Imaging valuation models in human choice’, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29: 417448. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112903.Google Scholar
Newell, R. G. and Siikamäki, J. V. (2013), ‘Nudging Energy Efficiency Behavior: The Role of Information Labels’, Journal of the Association of Environmental and Resource Economists, 1: 555598. doi:10.1016/j.yexmp.2014.03.001.Google Scholar
Nolan, J. M., Schultz, P. W., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J. and Griskevicius, V. (2008), ‘Normative Social Influence is Underdetected. Personal’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34: 913923. doi:10.1177/0146167208316691.Google Scholar
Peysakhovich, A. and Karmarkar, (2015), ‘Asymmetric Effects of Favorable and Unfavorable Information on Decision Making Under Ambiguity’, Management Science.Google Scholar
Plassmann, H. and O'Doherty, J., Rangel, A. (2007), ‘Orbitofrontal Cortex Encodes Willingness to Pay in Everyday Economic Transactions’, Journal of Neuroscience, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2131-07.2007.Google Scholar
Preuschoff, K., Quartz, S. R. and Bossaerts, P. (2008), ‘Human Insula Activation Reflects Risk Prediction Errors As Well As Risk’, Journal of Neuroscience, 28: 27452752. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4286-07.2008.Google Scholar
Ritov, I., Kahneman, D. (1997), ‘How people value the environment: Attitudes versus economic values’, in Psychological Perspectives to Environmental and Ethical Issues.Google Scholar
Sahoo, A. and Sawe, N. (2015), ‘The Heterogeneous Effects of Eco-Labels on Internalities and Externalities’.Google Scholar
Sallee, J. M. (2014), ‘Rational inattention and energy efficiency’, The Journal of Law and Economics, 57: 781820. doi:10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004.Google Scholar
Samanez-Larkin, G. R. and Knutson, B. (2015), ‘Decision making in the ageing brain: changes in affective and motivational circuits’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16: 278289. doi:10.1038/nrn3917.Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G. (2007), ‘Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience’, Science, 318: 598602.Google Scholar
Sawe, N., 2017. ‘Using neuroeconomics to understand environmental valuation’, Ecological Economics, 135. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2016.12.018.Google Scholar
Sawe, N. and Knutson, B. (2015), ‘Neural valuation of environmental resources’, Neuroimage, 122: 8795. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.010.Google Scholar
Scholz, C., Baek, E. C., Brook, M., Donnell, O., Suk, H., Cappella, J. N. and Falk, E. B. (2017), ‘A neural model of valuation and information virality’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114: 28812886. doi:10.1073/pnas.1615259114.Google Scholar
Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J. and Griskevicius, V. (2007), ‘The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms: Research article’, Psychological Science, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B., Ward, A., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., White, K. and Lehman, D. R. (2002), ‘Maximizing versus satisficing: Happiness is a matter of choice’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83: 11781197. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1178.Google Scholar
Shogren, J. F. and Taylor, L. O. (2008), ‘On behavioral-environmental economics’, Review of Environmental Economics and Policy, 2: 2644. doi:10.1093/reep/rem027.Google Scholar
Train, K. (1985), ‘Discount rates in consumers’ energy-related decisions: A review of the literature’, Energy, doi:10.1016/0360-5442(85)90135-5.Google Scholar
Tremblay, S., Sharika, K. M. and Platt, M. L. (2017), ‘Social Decision-Making and the Brain: A Comparative Perspective’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21: 265276. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.007.Google Scholar
Ungemach, C., Camilleri, A. R., Johnson, E. J., Larrick, R. P. and Weber, E. U. (2017), ‘Translated Attributes as Choice Architecture: Aligning Objectives and Choices Through Decision Signposts’, Management Science, 115. doi:10.1287/mnsc.2016.2703.Google Scholar
US Environmental Protection Agency (2016), ‘Energy Star Overview of 2015 Achievements’.Google Scholar
Venkatachalam, L., (2008), ‘Behavioral economics for environmental policy’, Ecological Economics, 67: 640645. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2008.01.018.Google Scholar
Venkatraman, V., Dimoka, A., Pavlou, P. A., Vo, K., Hampton, W., Bollinger, B., Hershfield, H. E., Ishihara, M., Winer, R. S., Venkatraman, V., Dimoka, A., Pavlou, P. A., Vo, K. and Hampton, W. (2015), ‘Predicting Advertising Success Beyond Traditional Measures: New Insights from Neurophysiological Methods and Market Response Modeling’, Journal of Marketing Research, LII: 141218074858006. doi:215.204.1409.Google Scholar
Wallis, J. D. (2012), ‘Cross-species studies of orbitofrontal cortex and value-based decision-making’, Nature Neuroscience, 15: 13–9. doi:10.1038/nn.2956.Google Scholar
Ward, D. O., Clark, C. D., Jensen, K. L., Yen, S. T. and Russell, C. S. (2011), ‘Factors influencing willingness-to-pay for the ENERGY STAR® label’, Energy Policy, 39: 14501458. doi:10.1016/j.enpol.2010.12.017.Google Scholar
Weber, E. U. (2006), ‘Experience-Based and Description-Based Perceptions of Long-Term Risk: Why Global Warming does not Scare us (Yet)’, Climatic Change, 77: 103120. doi:10.1007/s10584-006-9060-3.Google Scholar
Wilson, C. and Dowlatabadi, H. (2007), ‘Models of Decision Making and Residential Energy Use’, Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 32: 169203. doi:10.1146/annurev.energy.32.053006.141137.Google Scholar
Wu, C. C., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Katovich, K. and Knutson, B. (2014), ‘Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of incentive anticipation’, Neuroimage, 84: 279289. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.055.Google Scholar
Wu, H., Luo, Y. and Feng, C. (2016), ‘Neural signatures of social conformity: A coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies’, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 71: 101111. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.038.Google Scholar
Young, W., Hwang, K., Mcdonald, S. and Oates, C. J. (2010), ‘Sustainable Consumption: Green Consumer Behaviour when Purchasing Products’, Sustainable Development, 18: 2031.Google Scholar