Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Assessing bilingual language switching behavior with Ecological Momentary Assessment

  • Jussi Jylkkä (a1), Anna Soveri (a1), Matti Laine (a1) (a2) and Minna Lehtonen (a1) (a3) (a4)

Abstract

The putative bilingual executive advantage has been argued to stem from lifelong experience with executively demanding language behaviors, such as switching between the two languages. However, studies testing for possible associations between language switching frequency and EF in bilinguals have yielded inconsistent results. One reason for this could lie in the methods used that have evaluated the frequency and type of language switches with retrospective self-reports, as well as in problems in reliability and convergent validity of the executive tasks. By using Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) as a reference point for self-reports of language switches, we examined the validity of general retrospective self-reports of language switching. Additionally, we examined associations between language switching and EF using multilevel models. Our results indicated that the commonly used retrospective self-reports of language switching may lack convergent validity. However, we found tentative evidence that contextual language switches, assessed with EMA, may be associated with better inhibitory control, set shifting, and working memory.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Jussi Jylkkä, E-mail: jjylkka@abo.fi

References

Hide All
Bialystok, E (2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 12(1), 311. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003477
Botvinick, MM, Braver, TS, Barch, DM, Carter, CS and Cohen, JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108(3), 624–52. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11488380
Carlson, S, Martinkauppi, S, Rämä, P, Salli, E, Korvenoja, A and Aronen, HJ (1998) Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex 8(8), 743752. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/8.8.743
Cohen, J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
de Bruin, A, Treccani, B and Della Sala, S (2015) Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science 26(1), 99107. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614557866
Dolcos, F and Cabeza, R (2002) Event-related potentials of emotional memory: Encoding pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2(3), 252263. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.2.3.252
Eriksen, BA and Eriksen, CW (1974) Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics 16(1), 143149. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267
Gratton, G, Coles, MG and Donchin, E (1992) Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 121(4), 480506. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1431740
Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25(5), 116. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
Hartanto, A and Yang, H (2016) Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150, 1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016
Homma, Y, Ando, T, Yoshida, M, Kageyama, S, Takei, M, Kimoto, K, Ishizuka, O, Gotoh, M and Hashimoto, T (2002) Voiding and incontinence frequencies: Variability of diary data and required diary length. Neurourology and Urodynamics 21(3), 204209. https://doi.org/10.1002/nau.10016
Jeffreys, H (1961) Theory of Probability (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Johnson, HA, Sawi, O and Paap, KR (2015) Language switching frequency in bilinguals is inconsistently linked to executive functioning. In Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society.
Jylkkä, J (2017) Bilingual Language Switching and Executive Functions. Åbo Akademi. Retrieved from http://www.doria.fi/bitstream/handle/10024/147587/jylkka_jussi.pdf?sequence=2
Jylkkä, J, Lehtonen, M, Lindholm, F, Kuusakoski, A and Laine, M (2018) The relationship between general executive functions and bilingual switching and monitoring in language production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21(3). https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000104
Jylkkä, J, Soveri, A, Wahlström, J, Lehtonen, M, Rodríguez-Fornells, A and Laine, M (2017) Relationship between language switching experience and executive functions in bilinguals: An Internet-based study. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29(4), 404419. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1282489
Kerns, JG (2006) Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex activity in an FMRI study of trial-to-trial adjustments on the Simon task. NeuroImage 33(1), 399405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.012
Laine, M and Lehtonen, M (2018) Cognitive consequences of bilingualism: where to go from here? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 18. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1462498
Lehtonen, M, Soveri, A, Laine, A, Järvenpää, J, de Bruin, A and Antfolk, J (2018) Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 144(4), 394425.
Lexell, JE and Downham, DY (2005) How to assess the reliability of measurements in rehabilitation. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation 84(9), 719723. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.phm.0000176452.17771.20
Linck, JA, Schwieter, JW and Sunderman, G (2012) Inhibitory control predicts language switching performance in trilingual speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15(03), 651662. https://doi.org/doi:10.1017/S136672891100054X
Luce, KH and Crowther, JH (1999) The reliability of the Eating Disorder Examination-Self-Report Questionnaire version (EDE-Q). The International Journal of Eating Disorders 25(3), 349351. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10192002
Nee, DE, Wager, TD and Jonides, J (2007) Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 7(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.1.1
Paap, KR and Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66(2), 232258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
Paap, KR, Johnson, HA and Sawi, O (2016) Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex 74(February 2016), 305314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.010
Paap, KR, Myuz, HA, Anders, RT, Bockelman, MF, Mikulinsky, R and Sawi, OM (2017) No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29(2), 89112. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1248436
Paap, KR and Sawi, O (2014) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology 5(962), 115. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962
Podsakoff, PM, MacKenzie, SB, Lee, J-Y and Podsakoff, NP (2003) Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. The Journal of Applied Psychology 88(5), 879903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879
Prior, A and Gollan, TH (2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17(4), 682691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000580
Reinert, DF and Allen, JP (2002) The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): A review of recent research. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research 26(2), 272279. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11964568
Rodriguez-Fornells, A, De Diego Balaguer, R and Münte, TF (2006) Executive control in bilingual language processing. Language Learning 56(s1), 133190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00359.x
Rodriguez-Fornells, A, Krämer, UM, Lorenzo-Seva, U, Festman, J and Münte, TF (2012) Self-assessment of individual differences in language switching. Frontiers in Psychology 3(388), 115. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00388
Rogers, RD and Monsell, S (1995) Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124(2), 207231. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
Sallis, JF and Saelens, BE (2000) Assessment of physical activity by self-report: Status, limitations, and future directions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 71(2), 114. Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/02701367.2000.11082780
Shiffman, S, Stone, AA and Hufford, MR (2008) Ecological Momentary Assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 4, 132. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1005-9_947
Shrier, LA, Shih, M.-C. and Beardslee, WR (2005) Affect and sexual behavior in adolescents: A review of the literature and comparison of momentary sampling with diary and retrospective self-report methods of measurement. Pediatrics 115(5), 573581. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2004-2073
Siemann, J, Herrmann, M and Galashan, D (2018) The effect of feature-based attention on flanker interference processing: An fMRI-constrained source analysis. Scientific Reports 8(1), 1580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-20049-1
Simon, JR and Rudell, AP (1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. The Journal of Applied Psychology 51(3), 300304. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6045637
Soveri, A, Lehtonen, M, Karlsson, LC, Lukasik, K, Antfolk, J and Laine, M (2016) Test–retest reliability of five frequently used executive tasks in healthy adults. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult 0(0), 111. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2016.1263795
Soveri, A, Rodriguez-Fornells, A and Laine, M (2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology 2(183), 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183
Strauss, E, Sherman, EMS and Spreen, O (2006) A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford: Oxford UP.
Vaz, S, Falkmer, T, Passmore, AE, Parsons, R and Andreou, P (2013) The case for using the repeatability coefficient when calculating test–retest reliability. PLoS ONE 8(9), e73990. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0073990
Verreyt, N, Woumans, E, Vandelanotte, D, Szmalec, A and Duyck, W (2016) The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000352
Wonderlich, JA, Lavender, JM, Wonderlich, SA, Peterson, CB, Crow, SJ, Engel, SG, Le Grange, D, Mitchell, JE and Crosby, RD (2015) Examining convergence of retrospective and ecological momentary assessment measures of negative affect and eating disorder behaviors. The International Journal of Eating Disorders 48(3), 305311. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22352
Yim, O and Bialystok, E (2012) Degree of conversational code-switching enhances verbal task switching in Cantonese–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15(04), 873883. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000478

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed