Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T13:23:14.549Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bilingual advantage in executive control when task demands are considered*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 March 2015

LI QU*
Affiliation:
Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
JOEL JIA WEI LOW
Affiliation:
Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
TING ZHANG
Affiliation:
South West University, China
HONG LI
Affiliation:
South West University, China; Liaoning Normal University, China
PHILIP DAVID ZELAZO
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Li Qu, Division of Psychology, Nanyang Technological University, 14 Nanyang Drive, Singapore637332quli@ntu.edu.sg.

Abstract

To examine how task demands influence bilingual advantage in executive control over monolinguals, we tested 32 Chinese monolinguals and 32 Chinese–English bilinguals with four versions of a color-shape switching task. During switching trials, the task required participants to suppress one set of conflicting (or non-conflicting) responses and simultaneously to activate another set of conflicting (or non-conflicting) responses. The results showed that compared to monolinguals, (i) when suppressing conflicting responses or (ii) activating non-conflicting responses, bilinguals had significantly smaller switching costs though similar mixing costs; (iii) when suppressing one set of conflicting responses and simultaneously activating another set of conflicting responses, bilinguals had significantly smaller switching costs though larger mixing costs; and (iv) when suppressing one set of non-conflicting responses and simultaneously activating another set of non-conflicting responses, bilinguals had similar switching costs and mixing costs. These findings indicate that task demands affect bilingual advantage in executive control.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We wish to acknowledge the funding support for this project from Nanyang Technological University under the Undergraduate Research Experience on Campus (URECA) programme. We would also like to thank Chong Pei Shan, Lim Wei Li Dawn, Cheng Cheng Jiang, and Qiao Dai for their involvement in data collection. We also thank the two anonymous reviewers for comments on a previous version of the manuscript.

References

Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A., & Green, D. W. (2013). Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex, 49, 905911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In Umilta, C. & Moscovitch, M. (eds.), Attention and performance XV, pp. 421452. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. (2004). An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review, 111, 10361060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanism in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102, 68100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 6879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859873.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2009). Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language, 109, 93100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112, 494500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calmels, C., Foutren, M., & Stam, C. J. (2011). Influences of instructions and expertise on the mechanisms involved during a working memory task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25, 105115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F.-M., Kidd, J. R., Kivak, K. J., Pakstis, A. J., & Kidd, K. K. (1996). The world-wide distribution of allele frequencies at the human dopamine D4 receptor locus. Human Genetics, 98, 91101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, J. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chevalier, N., & Blaye, A. (2009). Setting goals to switch between tasks: Effect of cue transparency on children's cognitive flexiblity. Developmental Psychology, 45, 782797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coderre, E. L., Van Heuven, W. J. B., & Conklin, K. (2013). The timing and magnitude of Stroop interference and facilitation in monolinugals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 420441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Cost-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 5986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children's executive function. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 335341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vos, G. A., & Slote, W. H. (eds.). (1998). Confucianism and the family. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Dreisbach, G., Haider, H., & Kluwe, R. (2002). Preparatory processes in the task-switching paradigm: Evidence from the use of probability cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 468483.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2006). Examiner's manual for the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Fourth Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaser, M. O., & Glaser, W. R. (1982). Time course analysis of the Stroop phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 875894.Google ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33, 12201234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In Gorfein, D. S. & MacLeod, C. M. (eds.), The place of inhibition in cognition, pp. 145162. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 625658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubner, M., Kluwe, R., Luna-Rodriguez, A., & Peters, A. (2004). Task preparation and stimulus-evoked competition. Acta Psychologica, 115, 211234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamwangamalu, N. M., & Lee, C.-L. (1991). Chinese–English code-mixing: A case of matrix language assignment. World Englishes, 10, 247261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kray, J., Li, K., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Age differences in task switching components: The role of task uncertainty. Brain and Cognition, 49, 363381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G. D., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 575599.Google ScholarPubMed
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 1680816813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Green, D. W., Abutalebi, J., & Grady, C. L. (2012). Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 14791488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 2941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marecová, A., Asanowicz, D., Krivá, L., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). The effects of bilingualism on efficiency and lateralization of attentional networks. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 608623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 8193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., & Friedman, K. C. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 4464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, W., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action. In Davidson, R. J., Schwartz, G. E. & Shapiro, D. (eds.), Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research and theory (vol. 4), pp. 118. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 253262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qu, L., Finestone, D. L., Loh, J. Q., & Leong, Z. R. (2012). Focused but fixed: The impact of expectation of external rewards on inhibitory control and flexibility in preschoolers. Emotion, 13, 562572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qu, L., Gao, S., Yip, C., Li, H., & Zelazo, P. D. (2012). Affective decision making in European Canadian, Chinese Canadian, Hong Kong Chinese, and mainland Chinese preschoolers. Child Studies in Diverse Contexts, 2, 123132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qu, L., Wijeya, S., Zelazo, P. D., & Craik, F. I. M. (2007). Executive function across the life span: Investigation through task-switching. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Qu, L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). The facilitative effect of positive stimuli on 3-year-olds’ flexible rule use. Cognitive Development, 22, 456473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 763797.Google ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Sharpsburg, PA: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Treccani, B., Argyri, E., Sorace, A., & Sala, S. D. (2009). Spatial negative priming in bilingualism. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 320327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waggoner, T. L. (ed.). Ishihara Pseudoisochromatic Plate Compatible (PIPIC) Color Vision Test, 24 Plate Edition. Retrieved on Jan 5 2010, from http://colorvisiontesting.com/ishihara.htm.Google Scholar
Whitson, L. R., Karayanidis, F., & Michie, P. T. (2012). Task practice differentially modulates task-switching performance across the adult lifespan. Acta Psychologica, 139, 124136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, C. M., Christensen, B. K., King, J. P., Qu, L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2008). Decomposing perseverative errors among undergraduates scoring high on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire. Schizophrenia Research, 106, 312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 412422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood (Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 68.3). Wiley Online Library.Google ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In Schneider, W., Schumann-Hengsteler, R. & Sodian, B. (eds.), Young children's cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind, pp. 7193. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Qu supplementary material

Appendix

Download Qu supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 56.8 KB