Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T14:20:35.090Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conflict adaptation during multilingual language production as evidenced by the n-3 effect

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 April 2020

Mathieu Declerck*
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium
Stefanie Schuch
Affiliation:
RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
Andrea M. Philipp
Affiliation:
RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
*
Address for correspondence: Mathieu Declerck, E-mail: declerckmat@gmail.com

Abstract

Several multilingual language production models assume that language control is instigated by conflict monitoring. In turn, conflict adaptation, a control process which makes it easier to resolve interference if previously a high-interference context was detected, should also occur during multilingual production, as it is triggered by conflict monitoring. Because no evidence has been provided for conflict adaptation in the multilingual production literature, we set out to investigate this process using the n-3 effect. Our study showed that the n-3 effect can be observed during multilingual production, and thus provides evidence for conflict adaptation during multilingual production.

Type
Research Notes
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abutalebi, J, Annoni, J, Zimine, I, Pegna, AJ, Seghier, ML, Lee-Jahnke, H, Lazeyras, F, Cappa, SF, & Khateb, A (2007). Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex 18, 14961505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J, & Green, D (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics 20, 242275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, RH, Davidson, DJ, & Bates, DM (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babcock, L, & Vallesi, A (2015). Language control is not a one-size-fits-all languages process: evidence from simultaneous interpretation students and the n-2 repetition cost. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 1622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, DJ, Levy, R, Scheepers, C, & Tily, HJ (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68, 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, MM, Braver, TS, Barch, DM, Carter, CS, & Cohen, JD (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108, 624652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, MM, Cohen, JD, & Carter, CS (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 539546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branzi, FM, Calabria, M, Boscarino, ML, & Costa, A (2016). On the overlap between bilingual language control and domain-general executive control. Acta Psychologica 166, 2130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branzi, FM, Della Rosa, PA, Canini, M, Costa, A, & Abutalebi, J (2015). Language control in bilinguals: Monitoring and response selection. Cerebral Cortex 26, 23672380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brysbaert, M (2013). Lextale_FR: A fast, free, and efficient test to measure language proficiency in French. Psychologica Belgica 53, 2337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M, Branzi, FM, Marne, P, Hernández, M, & Costa, A (2015). Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18, 6578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M, Grainger, J, Koch, I, & Philipp, AM (2017). Is language control just a form of executive control? Evidence for overlapping processes in language switching and task switching. Journal of Memory and Language 95, 138145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M, Ivanova, I, Grainger, J, & Duñabeitia, JA (2020). Are similar control processes implemented during single and dual language production? Evidence from switching between speech registers and languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M, & Philipp, AM (2015). A review of control processes and their locus in language switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22, 16301645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Declerck, M, & Philipp, AM (2018). Is inhibition implemented during bilingual production and comprehension? n-2 language repetition costs unchained. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 33, 608617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M, Thoma, AM, Koch, I, & Philipp, AM (2015). Highly proficient bilinguals implement inhibition - Evidence from n-2 language repetition costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41, 19111916.Google ScholarPubMed
Eben, C, & Declerck, M (2019). Conflict monitoring in bilingual language comprehension? Evidence from a bilingual flanker task. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 34, 320325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egner, T (2007). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12, 374380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW, & Abutalebi, J (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 515530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, T, Liu, F, Chen, B, & Li, S (2013). Inhibition of non-target languages in multilingual word production: Evidence from Uighur-Chinese-English trilinguals. Acta Psychologica 143, 277283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, T, Ma, F, & Liu, F (2013). An ERP study of inhibition of non-target languages in trilingual word production. Brain and Language 127, 1220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, TF (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jylkkä, J, Lehtonen, M, Lindholm, F, Kuusakoski, A, & Laine, M (2018). The relationship between general executive functions and bilingual switching and monitoring in language production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21, 505522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, I, Gade, M, Schuch, S, & Philipp, AM (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 17, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemhöfer, K, & Broersma, M (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English. Behavior Research Methods 44, 325343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, U, & Keele, SW (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129, 426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philipp, AM, Gade, M, & Koch, I (2007). Inhibitory processes in language switching: Evidence from switching language-defined response sets. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19, 395416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philipp, AM, & Koch, I (2009). Inhibition in language switching: What is inhibited when switching between languages in naming tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, 11871195.Google ScholarPubMed
Prior, A, & Gollan, TH (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17, 682691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuch, S, Dignath, D, Steinhauser, M, & Janczyk, M (2019). Monitoring and control in multitasking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 222240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuch, S, & Grange, JA (2015). The effect of N–3 on N–2 repetition costs in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41, 760767.Google ScholarPubMed
Schuch, S, & Grange, JA (2019). Increased cognitive control after task conflict? Investigating the N-3 effect in task switching. Psychological Research 83, 17031721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmer, K, Calabria, M, Branzi, FM, Baus, C, & Costa, A (2018). On the reliability of switching costs across time and domains. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 1032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmer, K, Calabria, M, & Costa, A (2019). Non-linguistic effects of language switching training. Cognition 182, 1424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed