Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 41
  • Cited by
    This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Yang, Hwajin Yang, Sujin and Kang, Carissa 2014. The relationship between phonological awareness and executive attention in Chinese-English bilingual children. Cognitive Development, Vol. 30, p. 65.


    Greenberg, Anastasia Bellana, Buddhika and Bialystok, Ellen 2013. Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive Development, Vol. 28, Issue. 1, p. 41.


    Arredondo, Maria M. Hu, Xiao-Su Satterfield, Teresa and Kovelman, Ioulia 2015. Bilingualism alters children's frontal lobe functioning for attentional control. Developmental Science, p. n/a.


    YANG, HWAJIN and YANG, SUJIN 2015. Are all interferences bad? Bilingual advantages in working memory are modulated by varying demands for controlled processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, p. 1.


    Bialystok, Ellen 2016. The signal and the noise: Finding the pattern in human behavior. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism,


    Morales, Julia Calvo, Alejandra and Bialystok, Ellen 2013. Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Vol. 114, Issue. 2, p. 187.


    Akhtar, Nameera and Menjivar, Jennifer A. 2012. Advances in Child Development and Behavior Volume 42.


    Paap, Kenneth R. Johnson, Hunter A. and Sawi, Oliver 2016. Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue?. Cortex, Vol. 74, p. 305.


    Woumans, Evy and Duyck, Wouter 2015. The bilingual advantage debate: Moving toward different methods for verifying its existence. Cortex, Vol. 73, p. 356.


    Bialystok, Ellen Craik, Fergus I.M. and Luk, Gigi 2012. Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 16, Issue. 4, p. 240.


    Chamberlin-Quinlisk, Carla and Senyshyn, Roxanna M. 2012. Language teaching and intercultural education: making critical connections. Intercultural Education, Vol. 23, Issue. 1, p. 15.


    Antoniou, Kyriakos Grohmann, Kleanthes K. Kambanaros, Maria and Katsos, Napoleon 2016. The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition, Vol. 149, p. 18.


    Poarch, Gregory J. and Bialystok, Ellen 2015. Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review, Vol. 35, p. 113.


    Calvo, Alejandra and Bialystok, Ellen 2014. Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, Vol. 130, Issue. 3, p. 278.


    Tekin, Ali Kemal 2016. Attitudes of Omani early childhood preservice teachers toward bilingual early childhood education: Benefits, challenges, and solutions. Child & Youth Services, Vol. 37, Issue. 1, p. 78.


    Paap, Kenneth R. Johnson, Hunter A. and Sawi, Oliver 2014. Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences?. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 26, Issue. 6, p. 615.


    Soliman, Abdrabo Moghazy 2014. Bilingual advantages of working memory revisited: A latent variable examination. Learning and Individual Differences, Vol. 32, p. 168.


    Dong, Yanping and Li, Ping 2015. The Cognitive Science of Bilingualism. Language and Linguistics Compass, Vol. 9, Issue. 1, p. 1.


    Feng, Li Gai, Yunwei and Chen, Xiaoning 2014. Family learning environment and early literacy: A comparison of bilingual and monolingual children. Economics of Education Review, Vol. 39, p. 110.


    Zhou, Beinan and Krott, Andrea 2016. Data trimming procedure can eliminate bilingual cognitive advantage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Vol. 23, Issue. 4, p. 1221.


    ×

Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language*

  • SUJIN YANG (a1), HWAJIN YANG (a2) and BARBARA LUST (a3)
  • DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000611
  • Published online: 07 April 2011
Abstract

This study investigated whether early especially efficient utilization of executive functioning in young bilinguals would transcend potential cultural benefits. To dissociate potential cultural effects from bilingualism, four-year-old U.S. Korean–English bilingual children were compared to three monolingual groups – English and Korean monolinguals in the U.S.A. and another Korean monolingual group, in Korea. Overall, bilinguals were most accurate and fastest among all groups. The bilingual advantage was stronger than that of culture in the speed of attention processing, inverse processing efficiency independent of possible speed-accuracy trade-offs, and the network of executive control for conflict resolution. A culture advantage favoring Korean monolinguals from Korea was found in accuracy but at the cost of longer response times.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Barbara Lust, Department of Human Development, Martha Van Rensselaer Hall, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USABCL4@cornell.edu
Footnotes
Hide All

This work was supported by research grant awards from the College of Human Ecology and the Mario Einaudi Center for International Studies at Cornell University. We thank M. Rosario Rueda and Jin Fan for consultation on the Attention Network Test and to Ellen Bialystok, Suzanne Flynn, James Gair, and three anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments on the manuscript. Many thanks also go to the directors of daycare centers (NEW Christian Academy in New Jersey, Cornell University Early Childhood Center under the direction of Elizabeth Stilwell in New York, and Good Morning daycare in Korea) for their support, as well as parents and children who willingly participated in the study.

Footnotes
Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

S. A. Ahadi , M. K. Rothbart , & R. Ye (1993). Children's temperament in the US and China similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359377.

E. Bialystok (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636644.


E. Bialystok (2009). Claiming evidence from non-evidence: A reply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12, 499501.

E. Bialystok (2010). Global–local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 93105.

E. Bialystok , F. I. M. Craik , R. Klein , & M. Viswanathan (2004). Bilingualism, aging and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychology and aging, 19, 290303.


E. Bialystok , & M. M. Martin (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7, 325339.

S. M. Carlson , & A. N. Meltzoff (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282298.

X. Chen , P. D. Hastings , K. H. Rubin , H. Chen , G. Cen , & S. L. Stewart (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34, 677686.

A. Costa , M. Hernández , & N. Sebastián-Gallés (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 5986.

B. A. Eriksen , & C. W. Eriksen (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143149.

J. Fan , B. D. McCandliss , T. Sommer , A. Raz , & M. I. Posner (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340347.

J. M. Farver , & Y. Lee-Shin (2000). Acculturation and Korean–American children's social and play behavior. Social Development, 9, 316336.


A. M. Kovács , & J. Mehler (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. PNAS, 106, 65566560.

T. LaFramboise , H. L. K. Coleman , & J. Gerton (1993). Psychological impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 395412.

Y. Levy , J. Smith , & H. Tager-Flusberg (2003). Word reading and reading-related skills in adolescents with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 576587.


V. Marian , & M. Kaushanskaya (2004). Self-construal and emotion in bicultural bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 51 (2), 190201.

E. Mezzacappa (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75, 13731386.

G. A. Miller , & J. P. Chapman (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 4048.

J. P. Morton , & S. N. Harper (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719726.

S. Oh , & C. Lewis (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development, 79, 8099.


M. I. Posner , & M. K. Rothbart (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 123.


M. R. Rueda , J. Fan , B. D. McCandliss , J. D. Halparin , D. B. Gruber , L. P. Lercari , & M. I. Posner (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 10291040.

M. R. Rueda , M. I. Posner , & M. K. Rothbart (2005). The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 573594.

M. A. Sabbagh , F. Xu , S. M. Carlson , L. J. Moses , & K. Lee (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 7480.

P. G. Vinden (2001). Parenting attitudes and children's understanding of mind: A comparison of Korean American and Anglo-American families. Cognitive Development, 16, 793809.

Q. Wang (2006). Relations to maternal style and child self-concept to autobiographical memories in Chinese, Chinese immigrant and European American 3-year-olds. Child Development, 77, 17641809.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition
  • ISSN: 1366-7289
  • EISSN: 1469-1841
  • URL: /core/journals/bilingualism-language-and-cognition
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords: