Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-rkzlw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-05T08:42:10.030Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effects of L2 exposure at school on the cognitive development of children from monolingual backgrounds: A longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2025

Gloria Chamorro*
Affiliation:
Departamento de Filologías Extranjeras y sus Lingüísticas, https://ror.org/02msb5n36 Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia , Madrid, Spain
Inés de la Viña
Affiliation:
Departamento de Filologías Extranjeras y sus Lingüísticas, https://ror.org/02msb5n36 Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia , Madrid, Spain
Vikki Janke
Affiliation:
Department of English Language and Linguistics, https://ror.org/00xkeyj56 University of Kent , Canterbury, UK
*
Corresponding author: Gloria Chamorro; Email: gchamorro@flog.uned.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This longitudinal study examined the cognitive development of Spanish children from monolingual backgrounds attending schools with varying levels of English exposure (13%–83%) to assess whether higher L2 exposure results in advantages over time. 229 children (ages 6–7) completed background (nonverbal reasoning (NVR), working memory (WM), L1 vocabulary, L2 vocabulary) and experimental tests measuring attentional/executive functions (selective attention, divided attention, switching, inhibition) at the beginning and end of year 1 of primary education. Generalized linear mixed-effects models, accounting for factors such as family educational level, onset of L2 exposure and language exposure outside of school, indicated that children’s cognitive skills benefit from (high) L2 exposure at school, with greater L2 exposure being linked to more enhanced attentional/executive skills as well as to a larger L2 vocabulary. These findings support the positive effects of immersion programs, suggesting that L2 exposure in school settings alone can contribute to more developed attentional/executive skills.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Highlights

  • Monolingual children in immersion programs can develop a bilingual advantage.

  • Higher L2 exposure leads to better selective attention, switching and inhibition.

  • Length of time and intensity of the immersion program can affect attention benefits.

  • Higher L2 exposure at school leads to greater L2 vocabulary.

  • Higher L2 exposure at/outside school may lead to a slightly smaller L1 vocabulary.

1. Introduction

The cognitive effects of child bilingualism continue to be a debated issue in bilingualism research. While there is evidence supporting cognitive benefits in certain executive functions (Bialystok & Martin, Reference Bialystok and Martin2004; Cape et al., Reference Cape, Vega-Mendoza, Bak and Sorace2018; Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008; Hernández et al., Reference Hernández, Martin, Barceló and Costa2013), the reliability or extent of these benefits is sometimes questioned, as other external factors may also influence outcomes (Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Hernández, Antón, Macizo, Estévez, Fuentes and Carreiras2014; Paap et al., Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2015). Moreover, most of this literature is based on cross-sectional studies (Bialystok & Barac, Reference Bialystok and Barac2012; Poarch & van Hell, Reference Poarch and van Hell2012) or examines children raised with two languages at home or in the community (De Cat, Reference De Cat2020; Shokrkon & Nicoladis, Reference Shokrkon and Nicoladis2021), making it difficult to identify the amount of exposure needed for potential advantages to develop or to extend the results to other populations such as children exposed to educational bilingualism.

The present study is longitudinal and examines executive function skills in Spanish (L1) children from monolingual backgrounds, attending schools with different English (L2) exposure levels, namely, non-bilingual, bilingual and immersion.Footnote 1 Our aim is to explore whether children from schools with higher L2 exposure reveal cognitive advantages over time.

1.1. Bilingualism and cognitive development

Executive function skills refer to domain-general cognitive processes such as inhibitory control (the ability to suppress dominant or automatic responses and resist distractions), cognitive flexibility (the capacity to switch between different tasks or mental frameworks) and monitoring (the ability to manipulate and update information in working memory [WM]) (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000; Miyake & Friedman, Reference Miyake and Friedman2012). Bialystok (Reference Bialystok2010) suggests that bilinguals have enhanced cognitive abilities due to the frequent need to switch between languages and inhibit one while using the other. A large body of research suggests that bilingualism can enhance inhibition and cognitive flexibility (Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik, Klein and Viswanathan2004; Bialystok & Barac, Reference Bialystok and Barac2012; Bialystok & Shapero, Reference Bialystok and Shapero2005; Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008; Garbin et al., Reference Garbin, Sanjuan, Forn, Bustamante, Rodríguez-Pujadas, Belloch, Hernández, Costa and Ávila2010; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Macizo, Duñabeitia, Saldaña, Carreiras, Fuentes and Bajo2016; Prior & MacWhinney, Reference Prior and Macwhinney2010; Tran et al., Reference Tran, Arredondo and Yoshida2019; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Kalashnikova and Antoniou2019). However, more attention has been paid to measuring inhibition than cognitive flexibility (see Planckaert et al., Reference Planckaert, Duyck and Woumans2023). Some evidence in favor of enhanced inhibition in simultaneous and early sequential bilinguals has been obtained using common conflict-resolution tasks such as the Simon task, the Stroop task, or the Flanker task (Bialystok & Barac, Reference Bialystok and Barac2012; Diaz & Farrar, Reference Diaz and Farrar2018; Gerstadt et al., Reference Gerstadt, Hong and Diamond1994; Grote et al., Reference Grote, Scott and Gilger2021; Verhagen et al., Reference Verhagen, Grassmann and Küntay2017). To examine cognitive flexibility, researchers have used measures like the dimensional change card sort (DCCS) or the opposite worlds task, where participants must focus their attention and switch between different rules. Here, too, advantages have been found for simultaneous and sequential bilinguals (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok1999; Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2010; Castillo et al., Reference Castillo, Khislavsky, Altman and Gilger2022; Prior & MacWhinney, Reference Prior and Macwhinney2010; Tran et al., Reference Tran, Arredondo and Yoshida2019), with stronger evidence found in younger children (up to age 6) (Planckaert et al., Reference Planckaert, Duyck and Woumans2023).

Other studies have failed to replicate the so-called ‘bilingual advantage’. For instance, a US study by Arizmendi et al. (Reference Arizmendi, Alt, Gray, Hogan, Green and Cowan2018) found no evidence of an advantage for inhibition, shifting, or updating in 7–9-year-old Spanish-English bilinguals. Children attended English-only schools but spoke Spanish at home. The authors suggest that although these children were proficient in both languages, they may not have had enough opportunities to switch between languages for a bilingual advantage to materialize. Shokrkon and Nicoladis (Reference Shokrkon and Nicoladis2021) also failed to find a cognitive flexibility advantage in Mandarin Chinese-English bilingual preschoolers. They replicated key aspects of Bialystok and Martin’s (Reference Bialystok and Martin2004) study, which had originally found a bilingual advantage using the DCCS, and tested 4–5-year-olds on this task but did not observe the same effects. More recently, Troesch et al. (Reference Troesch, Weiner-Bühler and Grob2023) conducted a longitudinal study examining executive functioning among 332 children who grew up as simultaneous bilinguals, sequential bilinguals, or German monolinguals in Switzerland at ages 4, 6 and 7. The results did not indicate a bilingual advantage for selective attention.

Planckaert et al. (Reference Planckaert, Duyck and Woumans2023) provide an overview of research on inhibition and switching in bilingual children under 12 years old, in which they show that advantages in these skills were more frequently observed during a critical age (3–6), with benefits favoring younger bilinguals for both inhibition and switching. The authors suggest that discrepancies in findings may stem from differences in experimental designs (e.g., cross-sectional versus longitudinal), the types of tasks employed (e.g., delay versus conflict tasks) and the age groups studied (e.g., critical versus post-critical periods in children, young versus ageing adults).

Research failing to replicate a bilingual advantage suggests that previous studies may have overlooked important factors that can affect cognitive functioning in this population, such as families’ educational background, immigrant status, onset/type/amount of L2 exposure, L2 proficiency and language-switching frequency (Arizmendi et al., Reference Arizmendi, Alt, Gray, Hogan, Green and Cowan2018; Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Hernández, Antón, Macizo, Estévez, Fuentes and Carreiras2014; Engel Engel de Abreu et al., Reference Engel de Abreu, Cruz-Santos, Tourinho, Martin and Bialystok2012; Green & Abutalebi, Reference Green and Abutalebi2013; Paap et al., Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2015; Shokrkon & Nicoladis, Reference Shokrkon and Nicoladis2021; Troesch et al., Reference Troesch, Weiner-Bühler and Grob2023). These variations shape the bilingual experience and affect the impact of bilingualism, which can also vary depending on the type and extent of bilingual exposure (e.g., sequential versus simultaneous bilingualism). Here, we focus on educational bilingualism, where children are educated in two languages but not raised bilingually at home or in the community.

1.2. Educational bilingualism

A few studies have been conducted on the potential cognitive benefits of early L2 acquisition through bilingual or immersion education.Footnote 2 Bialystok and Barac (Reference Bialystok and Barac2012) carried out two studies: the first involved children in grades 2 and 3 enrolled in a Hebrew L2 immersion program, and the second included children in grades 2 and 5 in a French L2 immersion program. In both studies, longer exposure to the immersion language was associated with better task switching and interference inhibition skills.

In a cross-sectional study, Nicolay and Poncelet (Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013) found that 8-year-old children enrolled in an English immersion program for 3 years outperformed those enrolled in a French monolingual school in tasks assessing alerting, selective attention, divided attention and mental flexibility, but not on response or interference inhibition tasks. They used the test for attentional performance for children (KiTAP) (Zimmermann & Fimm, Reference Zimmermann, Fimm, Leclercq and Zimmermann2002) and the Attentional Network Test (Fan et al., Reference Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz and Posner2002), and the groups were matched for age, verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning (NVR) and socio-economic status (SES). The same authors (Nicolay & Poncelet, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015) conducted a follow-up study to rule out the possibility that the cognitive advantages observed in the immersion group were due to preexisting differences. They addressed this by testing 51 5-year-olds at the start of an English immersion program and 50 5-year-olds in a monolingual French program, assessing both groups from a baseline. The results confirmed that 3 years of immersion resulted in cognitive benefits in attention and mental flexibility measures (response inhibition and interference inhibition were not reevaluated, as previous results had not shown significant effects for these skills). The authors suggest that such benefits arise from the dual challenge of learning new academic content while simultaneously acquiring the L2. In these demanding environments, children compensate for their limited L2 fluency by relying more heavily on attentional control, which appears to enhance their overall cognitive abilities, particularly in areas related to focus and task management.

Following this research, Barbu et al. (Reference Barbu, Gonzalez, Gillet and Poncelet2019) conducted a study with 8-year-olds to test whether the same effects could be observed after one year of immersion. They tested 59 French children enrolled in an English immersion program and 57 children attending a monolingual French school. To assess cognitive functioning, they also used the KiTAP, which measures alerting, selective attention, divided attention and cognitive flexibility. They found an advantage in the selective auditory attention task, but not in alerting, divided attention, or cognitive flexibility. The authors argue that one year of immersion may not be enough to positively impact all skills. The enhanced auditory attention observed in immersed children may have resulted from adaptations to their learning environment. Immersion students face the challenge of having to process complex academic input in a language they are not fluent in, while monolinguals do not encounter the same cognitive demands, as they learn their subjects in a ‘highly automatized and fluent’ language. Notably, 30 of the 59 immersed children in this study received 50% of their school subjects in English (and the rest 75%), unlike Nicolay and Poncelet (Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015), where all children were exposed to 75% of their courses in English.

More recently, Chamorro and Janke (Reference Chamorro and Janke2023) conducted a longitudinal study to examine the cognitive development of 59 Spanish children enrolled in bilingual English-Spanish programs or non-bilingual programs. Children were grouped according to their exposure to the L2 (English) at school: high exposure (40%), low exposure (30%) and monolingual (10%). They were tested at the end of years 1 and 2 of primary education (PE) on the complete battery of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch2; Manly et al., Reference Manly, Robertson, Galloway and Hawkins2016) and several background measures (NVR, WM, L1 vocabulary, L2 vocabulary). Results showed that bilingual children, particularly those in the high exposure group, outperformed their monolingual peers in L2 vocabulary in both years and that there were no group differences in L1 vocabulary. Bilingual advantages were also observed in year 1 in tasks relating to interference suppression and response inhibition, although these seemed to disappear after the second year (see also Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro, Janke, Dionne and Covas2021). Interestingly, children who reported having exposure to English outside of school outperformed those who did not in tasks involving interference suppression in year 2.

Some studies within the field of bilingual education, however, have found no evidence of cognitive advantages when comparing executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children (Carlson & Meltzoff, Reference Carlson and Meltzoff2008; Kaushanskaya et al., Reference Kaushanskaya, Gross and Buac2014; Poarch & van Hell, Reference Poarch and van Hell2012; Simonis et al., Reference Simonis, Van der Linden, Galand, Hiligsmann and Szmalec2020). For instance, Kaushanskaya et al. (Reference Kaushanskaya, Gross and Buac2014) compared two groups of 7-year-old English children in the US attending either a monolingual program or a bilingual program with a 90–10 Spanish-English model. The bilingual children had an average of 1.96 years of dual-immersion experience, while the monolingual group had no L2 exposure. Both groups were matched for age, gender and SES. Using a version of the DCSS, the authors failed to find an advantage in task switching in the bilingual group. Relatedly, Purić et al. (Reference Purić, Vuksanović and Chondrogianni2017) did not find differences for interference inhibition or switching in a study comparing three groups of 7-year-old Serbian-speaking children with different L2 exposure levels: high exposure (5 hours/day), low exposure (1.5 hours/day) and a monolingual group. A more recent study by Simonis et al. (Reference Simonis, Van der Linden, Galand, Hiligsmann and Szmalec2020) compared 128 10-year-old French-speaking children learning L2 Dutch or English with 102 same-age monolingual French children of the same age. The immersion programs provided 12–15 hours/week of L2 exposure. They found no bilingual advantage on cognitive flexibility or inhibition after 5 years of immersion. This finding contrasts with earlier studies (Nicolay & Poncelet, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015) and suggests that immersion benefits might also depend on factors such as the intensity of the immersion program, the frequency and type of language switching (see Verreyt et al., Reference Verreyt, Woumans, Vandelanotte, Szmalec and Duyck2016), or the timing when cognitive advantages become apparent (see also Nicolay & Poncelet, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015; Paap, Reference Paap2018).

A few studies have focused on minority languages. For instance, Cape et al. (Reference Cape, Vega-Mendoza, Bak and Sorace2018) compared executive function skills in two groups of 9–10-year-old children: 29 in Gaelic-medium education and 30 in English-medium education, all of whom had been exposed to English from birth. Using tasks from the TEA-Ch (Manly et al., Reference Manly, Robertson, Anderson and Nimmo-Smith1999), they found that Gaelic-medium students outperformed their peers in response inhibition but not in task switching, which the authors interpret as being the result of the bilingual experience of children, who are restricted to the minority/second language at school. With one clear dominant language throughout the day, they do not engage in frequent language switching but rather have to make a big effort to suppress their dominant language. This suppression resembles response inhibition rather than switching (see also Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella and Sebastián-Gallés2009; Green & Abutalebi, Reference Green and Abutalebi2013; Prior & Gollan, Reference Prior and Gollan2011).

Other studies have documented an advantage in children attending bilingual education on skills other than executive control, such as NVR (Woumans et al., Reference Woumans, Surmont, Struys and Duyck2016), abstract thinking (Planas, Reference Planas2014; Salekhova & Tuktamishov, Reference Salekhova and Tuktamishov2019), novel-word learning (Kaushanskaya & Rechtzigel, Reference Kaushanskaya and Rechtzigel2012) and WM (Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2008; Kaushanskaya et al., Reference Kaushanskaya, Gross and Buac2014; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Craik, Moreno and Bialystok2013; Purić et al., Reference Purić, Vuksanović and Chondrogianni2017; Trebits et al., Reference Trebits, Koch, Ponto, Bruhn, Adler and Kersten2021). With regard to L1 development, research shows that enrollment in immersion programs does not hinder L1 skills (Björklund & Mård-Miettinen, Reference Björklund, Mård-Miettinen, Tedick, Christian and Fortune2011; Bostwick, Reference Bostwick, Noguchi and Fotos2001; Ha, Reference Ha2001; Mehisto & Asser, Reference Mehisto and Asser2007; Serrano & Howard, Reference Serrano, Howard and Sayahi2003). While children may experience a temporary lag when first entering bilingual or immersion programs, this is typically short lived, and studies have consistently shown that L1 skills remain strong and continue to develop alongside the L2 (Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Tucker and d’Anglejan1973; Montanari, Reference Montanari2013; Padilla et al., Reference Padilla, Fan, Xu and Silva2013). In his review of immersion programs, Genesee (Reference Genesee, Bhatia and Ritchie2004) concluded that L1 development is not negatively impacted by immersion education. He observed that students in bilingual programs, including students from low SES backgrounds and those with below-average academic abilities, typically achieve L1 proficiency levels comparable to their peers in monolingual programs. Trebits et al. (Reference Trebits, Koch, Ponto, Bruhn, Adler and Kersten2021) also suggested that bilingual education can mitigate the disadvantages often found in children with low SES. This is a particularly important finding when considered with studies that have shown how SES-related differences can influence children’s cognitive development and academic achievement (see also Lindholm-Leary, Reference Lindholm-Leary2014; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Song, Villacis and Santiago-Bonilla2021).

In conclusion, the number of discrepant studies in the field of educational bilingualism (and child bilingualism in general) has grown in recent years. These findings suggest that disparate bilingual experiences result in various possibilities for advantages across different aspects of executive functions. Further, targeted research would help clarify the picture for educational bilingualism. Longitudinal studies are particularly important here as they can offer more information regarding the developmental trajectories followed by children exposed to bilingualism in the context of bilingual/immersion schools.

1.3. The present study

Considering the controversial findings with regard to child bilingualism in general and educational bilingualism in particular, the present study aimed to explore the potential benefits of different amounts of L2 exposure at school for the cognitive development of children educated bilingually yet raised in monolingual backgrounds. Our research contributes to the existing literature by investigating this issue longitudinally and controlling for a large number of variables (age, gender, family educational level, immigration status, other languages spoken at home,Footnote 3 age of first exposure to English, exposure to English outside of school, exposure to other languages outside of school) and background measures (L1 vocabulary, L2 vocabulary, WM, NVR), with the aim of meeting the methodological concerns mentioned in the previous section and exploring more closely the cognitive repercussions of amount and length of L2 exposure in this population.

To achieve this, we recruited children from several schools, where different amounts of L2 exposure were provided, and engaged them in tasks at the start and end of year 1 of PE to track their development and answer the following research questions:

(1) Does higher L2 exposure at school confer more enhanced attentional/executive skills in children from monolingual backgrounds? If so, do these advantages remain constant, increase, or disappear over time?

(2) Is children’s performance on the attentional/executive measures associated with any of the background measures (L1 vocabulary, L2 vocabulary, WM, NVR) or the other variables (age, gender, family educational level, immigration status, other languages spoken at home, age of first exposure to English, exposure to English outside of school, exposure to other languages outside of school)?

2. Method

2.1. Participants

A total of 231 typically developing Spanish children from 10 different schools in Madrid took part in the initial testing phase at the beginning of primary 1 (T0). Nine months later, in the testing phase at the end of primary 1 (T1), 229 of the original children participated again, as two children had left their respective schools. Children were classified as sequential bilinguals (see footnote 3) because they had been exposed to Spanish (their L1) at home and to English (their L2) through formal education. However, we controlled for several language-acquisition-related variables (age of onset of L2 exposure, L2 exposure outside of school, exposure to other languages outside of school, other languages spoken at home) to ensure any relevant differences could be accounted for (see Section 2.4).Footnote 4

Children were recruited from the three types of schools found in the Spanish educational system, which represent the different levels of L2 exposure available. Four schools followed a non-bilingual program, with a curriculum entirely in Spanish except for 3 hours of English language per week, which equated to 13.33% of their instruction in English. Five schools followed a bilingual program, with a curriculum in English that varied between schools, ranging from 32% to 41.11%. One school followed an immersion program, with a curriculum entirely in English except for 4.5 hours of Spanish language per week, which equated to 82.86% of English exposure (see Table 1 for participant numbers, age range and mean per type of school at T0 and T1). These schools included 6 state schools (3 non-bilingual, 3 bilingual), 3 semiprivate schools (1 non-bilingual, 2 bilingual) and 1 private school (the immersion school).

Table 1. Number of participants, age range and age mean (SD) per type of school in T0 and T1

2.2. Materials

2.2.1. Background measures

We measured children’s NVR, WM and receptive vocabulary in both L1 and L2. For NVR, we used the Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., Reference Raven, Court and Raven1998), where children are shown a series of visual patterns and asked to identify the missing piece from the six options provided. For WM, the forward digit span (FDS) task from the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-Revised (WISC-R; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1974) was used, where children are read sequences of numbers and asked to repeat them in the same order. To monitor children’s receptive vocabulary, we employed the Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (PPVT-III; Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Dunn and Arribas2006) for Spanish (L1) and the British Picture Vocabulary Scales (BPVS3; Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn, Dunn and Styles2009) for English (L2). Except for WM, where the span was used, raw scores were used in the analyses for all background measures to retain the full range of individual differences in performance (see Section 2.4).

The same tests were readministered by the same researchers at T1 to track children’s development and evaluate whether the amount of English exposure received at school influenced their performance on any of the other background measures or on the attentional/executive tasks.

In addition, parents/guardians completed a questionnaire that gathered relevant information on SES and language background (e.g., immigrant status, family educational level, languages spoken at home, children’s amount and type of language exposure outside of school).Footnote 5

2.2.2. Experimental measures

To assess cognitive skills, children completed a standardized and normed clinical battery of attentional and executive tests (TEA-Ch). We selected 5 tasks (7 measures) to assess 4 types of attentional/executive functions (selective attention, divided attention, switching, response inhibition). Each task is explained below, in the order completed by the children as recommended by the test administration manual:

  1. (1) SkySearch (selective attention): Children must find as many targets (pairs of identical spaceships) as possible on a sheet filled with distractors (pairs of different spaceships). The timing score is based on both accuracy and time taken to complete the task.

  2. (2) SkySearchDT (divided attention): Children perform two tasks simultaneously: finding spaceships on a sheet (as in SkySearch) and counting sounds they hear on a recording. The timing score accounts for both accuracy and time taken to complete the task.

  3. (3) CreatureCounting (switching): Children are presented with a sequence of ‘creatures’ along a path, interrupted with arrows pointing up or down (7 trials). They count the creatures aloud, switching to counting upward or downward based on the arrows encountered. Two separate measures are recorded: accuracy (number of trials where counting is correct) and timing (average taken in those accurate trials – only provided if there was a minimum of 3 correct trials).

  4. (4) Walk/Don’tWalk (response inhibition): Children are asked to mark steps along a path, using a pen, after each tone they hear on a recording (20 trials). Unpredictably, one tone ends differently from the rest, signaling them to stop. The test measures whether the child is able to stop marking when this signal occurs or is ‘carried away’ into a task-driven ‘automatic’ style of responding (accuracy).

  5. (5) OppositeWorlds (switching): Children are presented with strings of digits ‘1’ and ‘2’. The digits must be read as presented in the same world (congruent condition) but in the opposite world (incongruent condition); ‘1’ must be read as ‘2’ and ‘2’ as ‘1’. There are 4 trials (2 for the same world, 2 for the opposite world), providing 2 timing measures: congruent and incongruent. The speed with which the children perform the cognitive reversal (incongruent) is the crucial measure.

All tasks start with practice trials. They were not computerized. Timing was recorded manually using the stopwatch provided with the test, following the administration manual.

2.3. Procedure

Before testing started, written consent was obtained from schools and parents. Together with the consent form, parents completed the background questionnaire in advance of our visit, and they were collected upon arrival. Child assent was also obtained by explaining the tasks in a child-friendly way and ensuring their understanding and continued willingness to participate. Breaks were provided as needed/requested. Children completed the tasks individually in a quiet room at their respective schools. The tasks were administered over three 30-minute sessions, which took place on different days, each administered by a different trained and experienced researcher. In session 1, children completed the Raven’s, WM and BPVS tests, in that order, with one researcher. In session 2, a different experimenter administered a theory-of-mind task,Footnote 6 followed by the PPVT. In session 3, they completed the TEA-Ch with a third researcher.Footnote 7 All tasks were conducted in Spanish, as this was the children’s L1, except for the BPVS, which was administered by an English native speaker.

2.4. Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using the R statistical platform (version 4.1.1, R Core Team, 2021). Data were analyzed using generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) using the glmmTMB package. Separate models were fitted for each background measure (L1 vocabulary, L2 vocabulary, NVR, WM) and each attentional/executive task (seven measures).Footnote 8 Models were first fitted for the data at T0, followed by models for the comparison between T0 and T1 scores. The primary independent variable in each analysis was the amount of English exposure at school, with time (T0, T1) and its interaction included for the time point comparison.

Initially, a baseline model for each outcome measure was fitted including several covariates: age, gender, family educational level (as a proxy for SES), age of first exposure to English, other languages spoken at home, weekly exposure to English outside of school, and weekly exposure to other languages outside of school.Footnote 9 The models comparing performance over time include a random intercept for subjects to account for repeated measures. Subsequent analyses involved equating multiple GLMMs to ascertain the most suitable model for the data. Interaction terms were included initially but removed if not significant. However, the main effects of time and English at school and their interaction remained in the models regardless of their significance. Each model employed the maximal random effects structure that would converge (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Levy, Scheepers and Tily2013). We report significant main effects and interactions below. Full model outputs are included in the Supplementary Materials.

3. Results

3.1. Background measures

First, we report the results of the background measures at T0, which enabled us to check whether the different schools showed any significant differences at the start of their PE. Then, we present the results that compare changes in performance from T0 to T1.Footnote 10

Initial analyses on the background measures confirmed that all groups were matched for age, NVR, WM and L1 Spanish vocabulary, but not for L2 English vocabulary (β = 0.025, standard error [SE= 0.002, z = 9.187, p < .001), where children at schools with higher L2 levels of exposure achieved higher scores (see means and standard deviations [SDs] in Table 2). This was not surprising as 84.85% of the children had attended preschool in their respective schools and had therefore been exposed to different amounts of English before starting PE (i.e., immersion>bilingual>non-bilingual). However, to confirm that differences in L2 vocabulary were indeed linked to this earlier L2 exposure, we examined this relationship in the children who attended preschool. We found a significant positive association between L2 exposure at school and L2 vocabulary (β = 0.034, SE = 0.003, t = 10.92, p < .001), indicating that children who had attended preschool in those schools with greater English exposure were the ones who achieved higher L2 vocabulary scores.

Table 2. Means (SDs) for age, NVR, WM, L1 vocabulary and L2 vocabulary per type of school at T0

After analyzing the initial data collected at T0, models were run to analyze each background measure, comparing scores between T0 and T1, with English at school, time and their interaction, as fixed effects, while also accounting for the covariates. All models revealed a significant improvement over time, i.e., scores in all background measures were significantly higher at T1 than at T0 (ps < .05). Means and SDs for background measures at T1 are provided in Table 3 and improvement of each group from T0 to T1 in Figure 1.

Table 3. Means (SDs) for age, NVR, WM, L1 vocabulary and L2 vocabulary per type of school at T1

Figure 1. Improvement in background measures from T0 to T1 by groups. Note: dots represent mean values, error bars indicate standard errors of the means, and lines show the progression over time.

For NVR, the model yielded no significant main effect of English at school (β = −0.004, SE = 0.003, z = −1.305, p = .192). However, it revealed a positive effect for age (β = 0.882, SE = 0.145, z = 6.046, p < .001), with older participants having higher NVR scores. Additionally, both L1 (β = 0.246, SE = 0.047, z = 5.165, p < .001) and L2 vocabulary (β = 0.113, SE = 0.056, z = 1.987, p = .046) were positively associated with Raven’s, suggesting that greater vocabulary in both languages is linked to better performance on NVR.

Similarly, there was no main effect of English at school on WM (β = 0.008, SE = 0.002, z = 0.337, p = .735). The model revealed significant effects of parent education (β = 0.436, SE = 0.168, z = 2.589, p = .009) and L1 vocabulary (β = 0.148, SE = 0.036, z = 4.038, p < .001), suggesting that higher education and greater L1 vocabulary are linked to enhanced WM.

For L2 vocabulary, results revealed that higher L2 exposure at school led to increased gains in this measure (β = 0.025, SE = 0.002, z = 10.865, p < .001), although the effect of English at school slightly decreased over time, as indicated by the interaction between L2 exposure and time (β = −0.005, SE = 0.003, z = −2.189, p = .029) (see Figure 2 Footnote 11). This suggests that while children with increased L2 exposure continued to increase their vocabulary over time, the gap between groups with different levels of exposure narrowed between T0 and T1. In addition, age (β = 0.465, SE = 0.092, z = 4.951, p < .001) and parent education (β = 0.551, SE = 0.162, z = 3.571, p < .001) emerged as significant covariates, meaning that older children and those with parents with higher education performed significantly better in L2 vocabulary. Exposure to English outside of school also had a positive effect on L2 vocabulary (β = 1.036, SE = 0.186, z = 5.588, p < .001), indicating a significant benefit from extracurricular language use.

Figure 2. Interaction effect between English at school and time on L2 vocabulary.

In contrast, English at school had a marginally negative effect on L1 vocabulary (β = −0.006, SE = 0.003, z = −1.796, p = .073), with participants in schools with higher L2 exposure performing marginally worse on this measure (see Figure 3). Other effects were observed for gender (β = −0.193, SE = 0.096, z = −2.000, p = .045), with females performing better than males and parent education (β = 1.202, SE = 0.235, z = 5.112, p < .001), with children whose parents have higher levels of education achieving better L1 vocabulary scores. Similarly, performance on the Raven’s (β = 0.206, SE = 0.039, z = 5.188, p < .001) positively influenced L1 vocabulary, indicating that higher NVR is associated with stronger vocabulary skills. L2 exposure outside of school revealed a negative association with L1 vocabulary (β = −0.556, SE = 0.268, z = −2.072, p = .038), suggesting that children with more English exposure outside of school had a smaller Spanish vocabulary.

Figure 3. Interaction effect between English at school and time on L1 vocabulary.

3.2. Attentional and executive measures

The models analyzing each attentional/executive measure at T0 (see Table 4)Footnote 12 included English at school as a fixed effect along with the aforementioned covariates. The results revealed significant differences for ‘English at school’ in five of the seven measures.

Table 4. Means (SDs) for each attentional/executive measure per type of school at T0

The main effect of English at school was significant in SkySearch (selective attention) (β = −0.004, SE = 0.002, z = −2.438, p = .015), suggesting that greater L2 exposure is associated with lower timing scores. The model also revealed significant effects of PPVT (β = −0.105, SE = 0.032, z = −3.280, p = .001) and Raven’s (β = −0.105, SE = 0.032, z = −3.339, p = .001), that is, higher scores on L1 vocabulary and NVR are associated with better performance. Gender was also significant (β = 0.123, SE = 0.062, z = 1.986, p = .047), with male participants performing better than females.

English at school did not yield a significant main effect for SkySearchDT (divided attention) (β = −0.002, SE = 0.001, z = −1.19, p = .235). However, age of exposure to English (β = −0.073, SE = 0.012, z = −3.720, p < .001), Raven’s (β = −0.147, SE = 0.029, z = −5.10, p < .001) and FDS (β = −0.127, SE = 0.042, z = −3.01, p = .002) emerged as significant covariates, suggesting that earlier onset of L2 exposure, higher NVR and higher WM are associated with lower timing scores.

Similarly, for CreatureCounting (switching), English at school did not influence the accuracy scores (β = −0.002, SE = 0.004, z = −0.591, p = .554). However, BPVS emerged as a significant predictor (β = 0.249, SE = 0.087, z = 2.872, p = .004), indicating that higher L2 vocabulary scores are associated with greater accuracy in this task. Interestingly, the model for CreatureCounting timing Footnote 13 revealed a main effect of English at school (β = −0.004, SE = 0.002, z = −3.130, p = .002), suggesting that greater L2 exposure is associated with lower timing scores. Age also emerged as a significant covariate (β = −0.268, SE = 0.083, z = −3.211, p = .001), indicating that older children tend to perform this task faster. Additionally, higher Raven’s scores led to lower timing scores (β = −0.051, SE = 0.024, z = −2.146, p = .031), while exposure to English outside of school was associated with slower performance (β = 0.236, SE = 0.105, z = 2.249, p = .024).

The model for Walk/Don’tWalk (response inhibition) showed a significant main effect of English at school (β = 0.035, SE = 0.010, z = 3.130, p = .001). This indicates that children attending schools with higher levels of English exposure achieved higher scores. The model also revealed significant effects of Raven’s (β = 0.733, SE = 0.228, z = 3.216, p = .001) and FDS (β = 0.764, SE = 0.351, z = 2.176, p = .029), with higher NVR and WM being associated with better performance.

Finally, the models fitted for OppositeWorlds (switching) revealed a main effect of English at school in both the congruent (β = −0.031, SE = 0.001, z = −4.95, p < .001) and incongruent (β = −0.003, SE = .001, z = −4.46, p < .001) conditions. In the congruent condition, higher NVR (β = −0.064, SE = 0.013, z = −5.07, p < .001) and WM scores (β = −0.054, SE = 0.017, z = −3.11, p = .002) were linked to better performance. The incongruent model also revealed a significant effect of Raven’s (β = −0.045, SE = 0.014, z = −2.67, p = .007) and PPVT (β = −0.037, SE = 0.015, z = −2.52, p = .012), indicating that higher NVR and L1 vocabulary are associated with lower timings.

The results on attentional/executive measures are in line with what was found for L2 vocabulary, as within those children who had previously attended preschool in their respective schools (84.85%), those who had been exposed to higher L2 levels had a larger L2 vocabulary (see Section 3.1). This can explain why at T0 we already see that higher English exposure at school correlates with better performance in most of the attentional/executive tasks.

Next, models were run to evaluate changes in attentional/executive skills over time (T0 versus T1). They included English at school, time and their interaction as fixed effects, along with the covariates. Across all models, overall performance significantly improved over time (p s < .05), while English at school had a significant main effect on six of the seven attentional/executive measures (see Table 5 for descriptive statistics).

Table 5. Means (SDs) for each attentional/executive measure per type of school at T1

For SkySearch, there was again a positive main effect of English at school (β = −0.003, SE = 0.002, z = −2.01, p = .044). The model also revealed that males outperformed females (β = 0.103, SE = 0.045, z = 2.26, p = .024) and that higher levels of parental education are associated with quicker performance (β = −0.226, SE = 0.111, z = −2.04, p = .042). Furthermore, both L1 vocabulary (β = −0.069, SE = 0.025, z = −2.74, p = .006) and Raven’s (β = −0.081, SE = 0.024, z = −3.42, p < .001) were significant predictors, with higher scores associated with faster performance.

As in T0, there was no main effect of English at school on SkySearchDT (β = −0.001, SE = 0.001, z = −1.53, p = .127), though a small positive interaction with time suggested a slight cumulative benefit over time (β = 0.003, SE = 0.002, z = 2.00, p = .045). Age of exposure to English had a negative effect (β = −0.029, SE = 0.012, z = −2.30, p = .022), which suggests that earlier onset of exposure to English is associated with faster timing responses. Raven’s (β = −0.076, SE = 0.018, z = −4.15, p < .001) and FDS (β = −0.055, SE = 0.025, z = −2.20, p = .027) were also predictors of this task, with children with higher NVR and greater WM capacity revealing a better performance.

For CreatureCounting, results showed a significant positive effect of English at school on the accuracy scores (β = 0.008, SE = 0.004, z = 2.620, p = .009). However, the negative interaction revealed between English at school and time (β = −0.009, SE = 0.003, z = −2.973, p = .003) suggests that the impact of English at school on performance decreased over time, indicating that while higher L2 exposure was associated with better accuracy, this initial benefit lessened from T0 to T1 (see Figure 4). Raven’s (β = 0.146, SE = 0.036, z = 4.092, p < .001) and BPVS (β = 0.148, SE = 0.046, z = 3.243, p = .001) emerged as significant covariates, indicating that higher NVR and larger L2 vocabulary are associated with better performance. As for CreatureCounting timing, the model revealed a significant positive effect of English exposure at school (β = −0.004, SE = 0.001, z = −3.130, p = .002), suggesting that higher L2 exposure is associated with faster task completion. Several covariates significantly influenced performance. Females outperformed males, as indicated by their lower timing scores (β = −0.134, SE = 0.043, z = −3.121, p = .002). Age also played a role, with older participants performing better (β = −0.211, SE = 0.053, z = −4.022, p < .001). Additionally, family education showed a small effect (β = 0.204, SE = 0.098, z = 2.090, p = .037), suggesting that children from families with lower educational levels were slightly quicker at completing this task. Since this model only included data from the 64 children who were able to achieve, in T0, the minimum score of 3 in the CreatureCounting accuracy task needed to gain a score in the timing task, a second model was run, which included all the children who achieved this score in T1. The second model included 175 participants and, again, a significant effect of English at school was found (β = −0.002, SE = 0.001, z = −2.20, p = .027), as well as a positive effect of NVR (β = −0.048, SE = 0.019, z = −2.47, p = .013).

Figure 4. Interaction effect between English at school and time on CreatureCounting accuracy (left) and CreatureCounting timing (right).

In Walk/Don’tWalk, English at school positively influenced performance, revealing that higher L2 exposure is associated with higher accuracy (β = 0.040, SE = 0.011, z = 3.742, p < .001). The model showed significant positive effects of L1 vocabulary (β = 0.594, SE = 0.189, z = 3.146, p = .002) and Raven’s (β = 0.561, SE = 0.172, z = 3.259, p = .001).

In OppositeWorlds, English at school was associated with faster performance in both the congruent (β = −0.003, SE = 0.001, z = −5.33, p < .001) and incongruent conditions (β = −0.003, SE = 0.001, z = −4.333, p < .001). An increase in the benefit of English at school over time was observed in the congruent condition (β = 0.001, SE = 0.000, z = 2.28, p = .023), with a marginally significant effect in the incongruent condition (β = 0.001, SE = 0.001, z = 1.951, p = .051) (see Figure 5). Additional positive predictors in both conditions included Raven’s (congruent: β = −0.044, SE = 0.008, z = −5.36, p < .001; incongruent: β = −0.036, SE = 0.010, z = −3.422, p = .006) and WM (congruent: β = −0.029, SE = 0.011, z = −2.56, p = .010; incongruent: β = −0.027, SE = 0.013, z = −2.051, p = .040). PPVT played a role in the incongruent condition (β = −0.024, SE = 0.011, z = −2.145, p = 0.031), with children with greater L1 vocabulary performing better.

Figure 5. Interaction effect between English at school and time on OppositeWorlds congruent (left) and OppositeWorlds incongruent (right).

4. Discussion

The present study investigated the potential cognitive benefits of L2 exposure at school in children raised in monolingual backgrounds. To this end, a total of 231 children from different types of schools (non-bilingual, bilingual, immersion) with varying levels of L2 exposure (13.33%–82.86%) were assessed using a battery of background and attentional/executive measures at the beginning (T0) and end (T1) of year 1 of PE. We addressed two main research questions: (1) Does higher L2 exposure at school confer more enhanced attentional/executive skills in children from monolingual backgrounds? If so, do these advantages remain constant, increase, or disappear over time? (2) Is children’s performance on the attentional/executive measures associated with any of the background measures (L1 vocabulary, L2 vocabulary, WM, NVR) or the other variables (age, gender, family educational level, immigration status, other languages spoken at home, age of first exposure to English, exposure to English outside of school, exposure to other languages outside of school)?

4.1. Background measures

The testing phase at T0 enabled us to confirm that children from all schools started PE matched on age, NVR, WM and L1 vocabulary. However, already at this stage, their L2 vocabulary differed, with children from schools with higher levels of English exposure exhibiting a larger L2 vocabulary. In addition, of those children who had previously attended preschool in their respective schools (84.85%), those who had been exposed to higher L2 levels had a larger L2 vocabulary. This finding suggests that higher levels of L2 exposure during preschool years are enough to develop children’s L2 vocabulary to a larger extent than those exposed to lower levels and that these differences are already visible at the start of PE.

During the testing phase at T1, we revisited the children 9 months later to administer the same tasks with the aim of tracking their development and exploring whether any initial advantages found would remain constant, increase, or disappear after one academic year. We found that all children had significantly improved their performance from T0 to T1 on the background measures. NVR and WM continued to be unaffected by the different levels of L2 exposure, with L1 vocabulary (and L2 vocabulary for NVR) positively affecting performance on these tasks. Results from previous studies have been mixed, with some indicating a bilingual advantage for NVR (Woumans et al., Reference Woumans, Surmont, Struys and Duyck2016), others for WM (Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Macizo, Duñabeitia, Saldaña, Carreiras, Fuentes and Bajo2016; Purić et al., Reference Purić, Vuksanović and Chondrogianni2017) and others for WM but not NVR (Kaushanskaya et al., Reference Kaushanskaya, Gross and Buac2014; Trebits et al., Reference Trebits, Koch, Ponto, Bruhn, Adler and Kersten2021). However, differences exist between those studies and ours. Focusing on the longitudinal ones, Woumans et al. (Reference Woumans, Surmont, Struys and Duyck2016) tested 35 children who had never been exposed to the L2 prior to entering PE and compared them to 29 monolinguals, while 84.85% of our children had previously attended preschool in their respective schools, and all of them had received some exposure to the L2, even those in non-bilingual schools (13.33%). Crucially, they did not find a group difference between the bilingual and the monolingual group, only that the bilingual group improved significantly more than the monolingual group from the testing phase at the beginning of the academic year to the testing phase at the end. Trebits et al.’s (Reference Trebits, Koch, Ponto, Bruhn, Adler and Kersten2021) children also belonged to either the immersion program (N = 16) or the regular program (N = 23), with the former receiving considerably more L2 exposure than our bilingual schools (76.77%) (although slightly less than our immersion school) and the latter less than our non-bilingual schools (two 45-minute lessons/week). In addition, they used three tasks from the WISC-IV to assess WM (letter–number-sequencing task, FDS, backward digit span), while we only used the FDS.

Similarly, L2 vocabulary continued to show differences in T1, with participants with higher English exposure at school (as well as those exposed to English outside of school) achieving higher scores. Other studies have also found that L2 exposure in a classroom setting in immersion/bilingual contexts is sufficient to develop children’s L2 vocabulary (Kaushanskaya et al., Reference Kaushanskaya, Gross and Buac2014; Simonis et al., Reference Simonis, Van der Linden, Galand, Hiligsmann and Szmalec2020), and that higher or earlier L2 exposure at school results in a larger L2 vocabulary (Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro, Janke, Dionne and Covas2021, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023; Uchikoshi, Reference Uchikoshi2014). Interestingly, our results showed that the effect of English at school slightly decreased over time. While children with higher L2 exposure continued to show greater vocabulary over time, children from the lower L2 exposure groups slightly narrowed the gap between them and the higher-exposure children from T0 to T1. Future visits (children’s development will be monitored for 3 more years) will prove crucial to continuing to explore L2 development. The effect of English at school might decrease or, as would be expected, increase, with longer exposure to the L2 leading to larger differences between the groups.

Another interesting finding was that, in T1, L1 vocabulary showed a marginal negative effect for children with higher L2 exposure at school as well as a negative effect with those exposed to English outside of school, revealing a slightly smaller Spanish vocabulary for these children. These findings, however, should be interpreted with caution due to their small effect size (and marginal statistical significance in the case of L2 exposure at school) and the lack of further measures assessing expressive vocabulary or grammar. Although most research shows that children’s L1 is not negatively affected by bilingual or immersion education (Björklund & Mård-Miettinen, Reference Björklund, Mård-Miettinen, Tedick, Christian and Fortune2011; Bostwick, Reference Bostwick, Noguchi and Fotos2001; Genesee, Reference Genesee, Bhatia and Ritchie2004; Ha, Reference Ha2001; Mehisto & Asser, Reference Mehisto and Asser2007; Serrano & Howard, Reference Serrano, Howard and Sayahi2003; Woumans et al., Reference Woumans, Surmont, Struys and Duyck2016), some initial temporary lag in L1 development when children enter these programs is not uncommon (Lambert et al., Reference Lambert, Tucker and d’Anglejan1973; Montanari, Reference Montanari2013; Padilla et al., Reference Padilla, Fan, Xu and Silva2013). Therefore, as with L2 vocabulary, it will be important to continue tracking L1 vocabulary in the upcoming years to see whether this negative effect becomes larger or is indeed just a temporary initial lag. Future testing will incorporate additional language measures, including the production of L1 syntactic structures, which will enable us to explore L1 development in more depth.

One more finding with respect to the background measures that deserves attention is that family educational level, which in this study was used as a proxy for SES, emerged as a significant predictor of performance for L1 and L2 vocabulary and WM. Those children with parents with higher educational levels had larger L1 and L2 vocabularies, and higher WM, than children from families with lower educational levels. Other studies have also pointed to the impact of SES on children’s performance and development (Calvo & Bialystok, Reference Calvo and Bialystok2014; Genesee, Reference Genesee, Bhatia and Ritchie2004). This finding also highlights the importance of controlling for variables such as this in bilingualism research (see Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Hernández, Antón, Macizo, Estévez, Fuentes and Carreiras2014; Paap et al., Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2015).

4.2. Attentional and executive measures

With regard to attentional/executive functions, the L2 exposure children received before PE also seemed to influence this skill, as at T0, children attending schools with higher levels of English performed better in five out of the seven measures employed. Advantages were found in those measures assessing selective attention, switching and response inhibition, but not in the task examining divided attention. As with the background measures, at T1, all children had significantly improved in their performance on all attentional/executive tasks since T0. In addition, the L2 exposure at school variable influenced one more measure than it had in T0 (six out of seven), with divided attention (assessed by one measure: SkySearchDT) once again being the only one that showed no significant differences. Therefore, children exposed to a higher amount of English at school performed better in all the other measures: selective attention (assessed by one measure: SkySearch), switching (assessed by four measures: CreatureCounting accuracy, CreatureCounting timing, OppositeWorlds congruent, OppositeWorlds incongruent) and response inhibition (assessed by one measure: Walk/Don’tWalk).

Our findings for selective attention align with previous research, suggesting that early and consistent L2 exposure may promote the ability to focus selectively on relevant information while filtering out distractions (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2010; Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020; Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008; Nicolay & Poncelet, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015). Interestingly, our study found this selective attention advantage before participants had even started PE, suggesting that the benefits emerge early (preschool years) and are retained after one year of PE. However, other studies with a more limited immersion exposure did not find an advantage in selective attention. For instance, Poarch and van Hell (Reference Poarch and van Hell2012) and Woumans et al. (Reference Woumans, Surmont, Struys and Duyck2016) did not observe such an effect after one year of exposure. Chamorro and Janke (Reference Chamorro and Janke2020), on the other hand, found an advantage for the bilingual group after year 1 of PE in one of the three tasks that assessed this skill, but this effect disappeared by year 2 (Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023). Barbu et al. (Reference Barbu, Gillet and Poncelet2024) and did not show a bilingual advantage for selective attention either after two years of exposure to a content-and-language-integrated-learning program. This suggests that onset of exposure and the length of time in an immersion program may be important factors for selective attention gains to be detected in this population.

With respect to switching, our results resonate with previous literature that has suggested that bilingual environments can enhance cognitive flexibility (Bialystok & Martin, Reference Bialystok and Martin2004; Castillo et al., Reference Castillo, Khislavsky, Altman and Gilger2022; Nicolay & Poncelet, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015; Prior & MacWhinney, Reference Prior and Macwhinney2010; Tran et al., Reference Tran, Arredondo and Yoshida2019). With regard to research on bilingual education in monolingual contexts, Nicolay and Poncelet (Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2015), who conducted a study very similar to ours, reported benefits in switching in the immersed group after three years of enrollment in this program. Similarly, Bialystok and Barac (Reference Bialystok and Barac2012) found better switching skills in those students with longer exposure to the immersion setting. Barbu et al. (Reference Barbu, Gonzalez, Gillet and Poncelet2019), Chamorro and Janke (Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023) and Purić et al. (Reference Purić, Vuksanović and Chondrogianni2017), on the other hand, did not find an advantage for switching, but their children were tested after only one or two years of immersion, which may not have been enough length of exposure. In the present study, we found an advantage after preschool years for three out of the four measures assessing switching, and after year 1 of PE for all four measures, for those students with higher L2 exposure at school. This suggests that increased L2 exposure may be necessary for a more enhanced mental flexibility to develop in immersion education in monolingual contexts. Since these students have fewer switching opportunities than bilinguals raised with two languages at home or the community, for example (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok1999; Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok2010; Castillo et al., Reference Castillo, Khislavsky, Altman and Gilger2022; Prior & MacWhinney, Reference Prior and Macwhinney2010; Tran et al., Reference Tran, Arredondo and Yoshida2019), length of exposure in this setting seems to be a crucial factor for this skill to develop and for a bilingual advantage to appear.

We also observed a bilingual advantage in response inhibition in T0 and T1, which aligns with other studies that have shown similar advantages in response inhibition using various tasks (Bialystok & Shapero, Reference Bialystok and Shapero2005; Cape et al., Reference Cape, Vega-Mendoza, Bak and Sorace2018; Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020; Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Bialystok and McLaughlin2004; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Kalashnikova and Antoniou2019). Others, on the other hand, report null results (Carlson & Meltzoff, Reference Carlson and Meltzoff2008; Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020; Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, Reference Martin-Rhee and Bialystok2008; Nicolay & Poncelet, Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013). However, Carlson and Meltzoff (Reference Carlson and Meltzoff2008) and Martin-Rhee and Bialystok (Reference Martin-Rhee and Bialystok2008) tested simultaneous bilinguals. Compared to the bilingual experience of these bilinguals, children in our study have a clear dominant language (Spanish) which needs to be constantly inhibited. This experience closely resembles the inhibition of a habitual response (see Cape et al., Reference Cape, Vega-Mendoza, Bak and Sorace2018), which is what participants need to do in Walk/Don’tWalk. However, a bilingual boost in this skill was not found by other studies on bilingual education, such as Nicolay and Poncelet (Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013) or Chamorro and Janke (Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023), who found an advantage after year 1 of PE for the group with the highest L2 exposure but not after year 2. On the one hand, Chamorro and Janke’s (Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023) children received a maximum of 40% of the curriculum in English (versus the 82.86% that our immersed students received). This suggests that our children have to inhibit their dominant language much more frequently than those in Chamorro and Janke (Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023) and sustain this inhibition for longer as they have all subjects except for Spanish language in English, which may give them an advantage to successfully perform this task. The experience of Nicolay and Poncelet’s (Reference Nicolay and Poncelet2013) participants is closer to that of our children as they were exposed to 75% of the curriculum in English. However, their response inhibition task was a go/no-go test measuring timing response (as well as that of Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023), whereas the task used in this study is an accuracy measure. In addition, none of these studies tested children before they started PE as a baseline measure, so it is possible that the lack of effects may be due to initial group differences. These discrepancies in the findings across bilingualism research underscore once again how task demands, bilingual experiences and participant characteristics, among other factors, may influence whether cognitive advantages develop (and can be detected) in bilingual children.

The lack of an effect for divided attention suggests that this skill may be less sensitive to L2 exposure, possibly due to its reliance on distinct cognitive processes from those involved in selective attention and inhibition (see Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000). This could indicate that while bilingual education may enhance certain facets of attention, it may require children to focus just on one type of sensory input (i.e., auditory or visual) but not both at the same time (Barbu et al., Reference Barbu, Gonzalez, Gillet and Poncelet2019). Thus, the bilingual classroom experience in this study may not consistently push children to multitask, aligning with previous findings that divided attention benefits often arise when bilinguals face high-level cognitive demands across multiple settings (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella and Sebastián-Gallés2009). That is, being in a bilingual educational setting may not equate to the cognitive challenges of simultaneous bilinguals, for example, who need to adapt to varying demands and contexts, such as conversations with speakers of different languages or bilingual interactions that require quick shifts in focus and attention (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2010; Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella and Sebastián-Gallés2009). Notably, our analyses revealed that earlier onset of exposure to the L2 impacted divided attention, suggesting that this skill may be more sensitive to the cognitive demands of early bilingual exposure than simpler tasks like sustained attention (Antón et al., Reference Antón, Duñabeitia, Estévez, Hernández, Castillo and Fuentes2014; Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2010; Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008). This indicates that timing and amount of L2 exposure may affect attention differently, as specific attention domains may respond variably to the quality versus quantity of bilingual exposure (Barbu et al., Reference Barbu, Gonzalez, Gillet and Poncelet2019; Carlson & Meltzoff, Reference Carlson and Meltzoff2008).

Interestingly, NVR emerged as a constant significant predictor in T0 and T1 for all types of attentional/executive functions (in six out of the seven measures in T0, and in all seven measures in T1). Previous studies have also identified a strong connection between NVR and attentional/executive skills (Bialystok & Barac, Reference Bialystok and Barac2012; Genesee & Gauthier, Reference Genesee and Gauthier1995). WM also emerged as a significant predictor in T0 and T1 for three out of the seven measures. This patterns with previous studies which have indicated that WM capacity supports performance in attention-demanding tasks (Bialystok & Barac, Reference Bialystok and Barac2012; De Cat, Reference De Cat2020; Verhagen & Leseman, Reference Verhagen and Leseman2016), such as divided attention and switching, as observed in our data. On the other hand, family educational level did not seem to play a major role in attentional/executive measures in our study, which differs from previous research linking SES to cognitive outcomes in bilingual education (Calvo & Bialystok, Reference Calvo and Bialystok2014; Trebits et al., Reference Trebits, Koch, Ponto, Bruhn, Adler and Kersten2021). One possible explanation for the absence of SES effects may be the way it was measured in this study. While we used family educational level as a proxy, other studies, such as Calvo and Bialystok (Reference Calvo and Bialystok2014) and Trebits et al. (Reference Trebits, Koch, Ponto, Bruhn, Adler and Kersten2021), included several factors, such as parental income, occupation and access to educational resources, which may have been a more fine-grained SES measure to detect any effects on attentional/executive functions.

Altogether, our findings present a positive picture for monolingually raised children whose L2 exposure is mostly restricted to the school environment, with advantages being revealed at the early stages of PE in L2 vocabulary and cognitive skills (specifically, selective attention, switching and response inhibition), especially for those children with higher levels of L2 exposure at school. When interpreted, considering other studies on bilingual education, it can be concluded that the length of time in a bilingual education program, the intensity of the bilingual experience and the proficiency level achieved in the L2 can affect whether benefits in attentional/executive functions show themselves (Carlson & Meltzoff, Reference Carlson and Meltzoff2008; Trebits & Kersten, Reference Trebits and Kersten2019). Therefore, our upcoming testing phases will be crucial to see whether a bilingual effect persists or increases after a longer exposure to immersion education or whether it decreases or disappears and if children from non-bilingual schools catch up with their bilingual peers.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728925100448.

Data availability statement

The data supporting this study are part of an ongoing longitudinal project. As data collection is still underway, we are unable to share the dataset at this time to protect participant confidentiality and preserve the integrity of future analyses. The full dataset will be made available upon completion of the study.

Acknowledgements

This project was funded by the Comunidad de Madrid (Atracción de Talento Investigador, T1/HUM-19952). We would also like to thank the participating schools, parents and children.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical standard

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

1 See Section 2.1 for an explanation of how we operationalize the terms ‘non-bilingual’, ‘bilingual’ and ‘immersion’ in this study.

2 Unless otherwise specified, the terms ‘bilingual education’ and ‘immersion education’ are used interchangeably in this paper to refer to programs in which subjects other than the second language are delivered in a foreign language, regardless of the percentage of exposure of each language.

3 Our aim was to recruit students only exposed to Spanish at home. However, given the large number of participating schools and students, to be sure that this was indeed the case, we included questions about the languages that parents/caretakers used at home in our background questionnaire (see Section 2). A small number of families (N = 20; 8.66%) reported speaking another language (in addition to Spanish). These children were kept in the study to explore whether they performed differently from the rest. Importantly, the covariate ‘other languages spoken at home’ was included in the analyses to control for this variable.

4 For further information on the background variables, see the Supplementary Materials.

5 No students were excluded from the study, as they performed within the standardized score for the age on the background tests – except for the BPVS, which is expected and consistent with studies on children with L2 English (see Mahon & Crutchley, Reference Mahon and Crutchley2006), the standardized scores being based on L1 speakers.

6 The theory-of-mind data are analyzed in separate work so not further discussed in this paper.

7 The order of tasks was informed by previous studies (Chamorro & Janke, Reference Chamorro and Janke2020, Reference Chamorro and Janke2023) and decided based on their cognitive demands, progressing from more to less demanding tasks within each session.

8 Background measures were normalized using the var function in R, and the models used a Gaussian distribution. For attention models, depending on the type of data, either gamma (for timing response variables), zero-inflated negative binomial, or Gaussian distributions were used.

9 Preliminary analyses for collinearity showed that family immigrant status had a moderate negative correlation with parent education level (r = −0.396, p < .001). Additionally, a significant relationship was found between school category (state, semiprivate, private) and English exposure at school (F(2,459) = 774.4, p < .001), indicating that much of the variability in English exposure is influenced by school category. Given these findings, both family immigrant status and school category were excluded from further analyses to prevent multicollinearity.

10 Two measures of English exposure are used to present the results. The amount of ‘English exposure at school’ (expressed in percentages) was used for detailed GLMM analyses to capture nuanced and granular exposure levels, particularly within the bilingual schools, which varied across six levels. For descriptive data in tables, the categorical variable (‘type of school’) was employed to succinctly summarize the results across the different educational contexts (non-bilingual, bilingual, immersion), enhancing their interpretability.

11 In all figures displaying interaction plots (Figures 25), shaded areas around the lines represent 95% confidence intervals of the predicted values.

12 For tasks measuring timing, lower scores (and negative coefficients) reveal a better performance; for tasks measuring accuracy, higher scores (and positive coefficients) indicate a better performance.

13 CreatureCounting timing was analyzed separately with a subset of the participants who performed this task, as only those who scored 3 or more in CreatureCounting accuracy received a timing score. This subset included 14 out of 66 participants from the non-bilingual schools (21.21%), 64 out of 140 participants from the bilingual schools (45.71%) and 16 out of 25 from the immersion school (64%).

References

Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., et al. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398.Google Scholar
Arizmendi, G. D., Alt, M., Gray, S., Hogan, T. P., Green, S., & Cowan, N. (2018). Do bilingual children have an executive function advantage? Results from inhibition, shifting, and updating tasks. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(3), 356378. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbu, C., Gillet, S., & Poncelet, M. (2024). Fluctuating cognitive benefits in children attending early bilingual immersive instruction. Journal of Immersion and Content-Based Language Education, 12(2), 192220. https://doi.org/10.1075/jicb.22020.cri.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbu, C., Gonzalez, A., Gillet, S., & Poncelet, M. (2019). Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for one year. Psychologica Belgica, 59(1), 416435. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636644. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail making tasks in bilingual and monolingual children: The role of executive control. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 415427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.001.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325339. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 6773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2010). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Cognition, 118(2), 100114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.01.007.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(4), 859873. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Shapero, D. (2005). Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. Developmental Science, 8(6), 595604. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00451.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Björklund, S., & Mård-Miettinen, K. (2011). Integrating multiple languages in immersion: Swedish immersion in Finland. In Tedick, D. J., Christian, D., & Fortune, T. W. (Eds.), Immersion education: Practices, policies, possibilities (pp. 1335). Multilingual Matters & Channel View Publications. https://doi.org/10.2307/jj.29308515.7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bostwick, M. (2001). Bilingual education of children in Japan: Year four of a partial immersion program. In Noguchi, M. G. & Fotos, S. (Eds.), Studies in Japanese bilingualism (pp. 164183). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cape, R., Vega-Mendoza, M., Bak, T. H., & Sorace, A. (2018). Cognitive effects of Gaelic medium education on primary school children in Scotland. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(7), 120. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2018.1543648.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282298. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castillo, A., Khislavsky, A., Altman, M., & Gilger, J. W. (2022). Executive function developmental trajectories kindergarten to first grade: Monolingual, bilingual and English language learners. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(3), 11011119. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2020.1742649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamorro, G., & Janke, V. (2020). Investigating the bilingual advantage: The impact of L2 exposure on the social and cognitive skills of monolingually-raised children in bilingual education. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(10), 17651781.10.1080/13670050.2020.1799323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamorro, G., & Janke, V. (2021). Does bilingual education benefit the social and cognitive development of monolingually-raised children? Evidence from a longitudinal study. In Dionne, & Covas, L.-A. V. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 45th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 108119). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Chamorro, G., & Janke, V. (2023). Educational bilingualism: Reflections on a longitudinal study of children’s cognitive and linguistic development. Ampersand, 10, 100115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amper.2023.100115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 5986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Cat, C. (2020). Predicting language proficiency in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42(2), 279325. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. (2018). The missing explanation of the false-belief advantage in bilingual children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12594. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234251. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Arribas, D. (2006). PPVT-III PEABODY: Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M., & Styles, B. (2009). The British Picture Vocabulary Scale - Third Edition (BPVS 3). Brentford, UK: GL Assessment.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 13641371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340347. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodríguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Ávila, C. (2010). Bridging language and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53(4), 12721278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F., & Gauthier, S. (1995). Language and schooling in Canada: A handbook for parents and educators. Newbury House.Google Scholar
Genesee, F. (2004). What do we know about bilingual education for majority language students? In Bhatia, T. K. & Ritchie, W. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism and multiculturalism (pp. 547576). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 3½–7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognitive Development, 9(1), 83100. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90009-4.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grote, K. S., Scott, R. M., & Gilger, J. (2021). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Evidence from a low-income sample. First Language, 41(6), 677700. https://doi.org/10.1177/01427237211024220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ha, J. H. (2001). English-speaking students’ written language development in Korean in a Korean/English two-way immersion program. The Korean Language in America, 6, 299307.Google Scholar
Hansen, L. B., Macizo, P., Duñabeitia, J. A., Saldaña, D., Carreiras, M., Fuentes, L. J., & Bajo, M. T. (2016). Emergent bilingualism and working memory development in school aged children. Language Learning, 66(S2), 5175. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Martin, C. D., Barceló, F., & Costa, A. (2013). Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? Journal of Memory and Language, 69(3), 257276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., & Buac, M. (2014). Effects of classroom bilingualism on task-shifting, verbal memory, and word learning in children. Developmental Science, 17(4), 564583. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaushanskaya, M., & Rechtzigel, K. (2012). Concreteness effects in bilingual and monolingual word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(5), 935941. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0271-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, W. E., Tucker, G. R., & d’Anglejan, A. (1973). Cognitive and attitudinal consequences of bilingual schooling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65(2), 141159. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindholm-Leary, K. (2014). Bilingual and biliteracy skills in young Spanish-speaking low-SES children: Impact of instructional language and primary language proficiency. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(2), 144159. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2013.866625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, R., Song, L., Villacis, C., & Santiago-Bonilla, G. (2021). Parental beliefs and knowledge, children’s home language experiences, and school readiness: The dual language perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 661208. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.661208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, L., Craik, F. I., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 2834. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahon, M., & Crutchley, A. (2006). Performance of typically-developing school-age children with English as an additional language on the British picture vocabulary scales II. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22(3), 333351. https://doi.org/10.1191/0265659006ct311xx.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M., & Hawkins, K. (2016). Test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch). Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1999). TEA-Ch: The test of everyday attention for children manual. Thames Valley Test Company Limited.Google Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in young monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 8193. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehisto, P., & Asser, H. (2007). Stakeholder perspectives: CLIL programme Management in Estonia. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(5), 683701. https://doi.org/10.2167/beb466.0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 814. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montanari, S. (2013). A case study of bi-literacy development among children enrolled in an Italian–English dual language program in Southern California. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(5), 509525. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2013.833892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolay, A.-C., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for three years. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(3), 597607. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolay, A.-C., & Poncelet, M. (2015). Cognitive benefits in children enrolled in an early bilingual immersion school: A follow up study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 789795. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padilla, A. M., Fan, L., Xu, X., & Silva, D. (2013). A mandarin/English two-way immersion program: Language proficiency and academic achievement. Foreign Language Annals, 46(4), 564582. https://doi.org/10.1111/flan.12060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R. (2018). Cognitive advantages in bilinguals: Fact or fiction? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 453470. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000684.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Planas, N. (2014). One speaker, two languages: Learning opportunities in the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 87(1), 5166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-014-9553-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Planckaert, N., Duyck, W., & Woumans, E. (2023). Is there a cognitive advantage in inhibition and switching for bilingual children? A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1191816. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1191816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 682691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., & Macwhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purić, D., Vuksanović, J., & Chondrogianni, V. (2017). Cognitive advantages of immersion education after 1 year: Effects of amount of exposure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 296309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (version 4.1.1). R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1998). Progressive coloured matrices. Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, E. B., Bialystok, E., & McLaughlin, S. M. (2004). Bilingualism and cognitive control: Evidence from bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88(2), 147157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.03.003.Google Scholar
Salekhova, I. V., & Tuktamishov, R. A. (2019). The role of bilingual education in the development of abstract thinking in bilingual children. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 12(2), 3246. https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2019.0203.Google Scholar
Serrano, R., & Howard, E. R. (2003). Maintaining Spanish proficiency in the United States: The influence of English on the Spanish writing of native Spanish speakers in two-way immersion programs. In Sayahi, L. (Ed.), Selected proceedings of the first workshop on Spanish sociolinguistics (pp. 7788). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Simonis, M., Van der Linden, L., Galand, B., Hiligsmann, P., & Szmalec, A. (2020). Executive control performance and foreign-language proficiency associated with immersion education in French-speaking Belgium. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 355370. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891900021X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shokrkon, A., & Nicoladis, E. (2021). Absence of a bilingual cognitive flexibility advantage: A replication study in preschoolers. PLoS One, 16(8), e0255157. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0255157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tran, C. D., Arredondo, M. M., & Yoshida, H. (2019). Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 714732. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trebits, A., & Kersten, K. (2019). Bilingual education trumps socioeconomic status: L1 and L2 development in primary school EFL learners. In EUROSLA conference. Sweden: Lund.Google Scholar
Trebits, A., Koch, M. J., Ponto, K., Bruhn, A., Adler, M., & Kersten, K. (2021). Cognitive gains and socioeconomic status in early second language acquisition in immersion and EFL learning settings. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 25(7), 26682681. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2021.1943307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troesch, L. M., Weiner-Bühler, J. C., & Grob, A. (2023). Longitudinal examination of potential bilingual advantage effects for selective attention and cognitive functioning in young children. Language Learning and Development, 20(2), 174193. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2023.2246451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uchikoshi, Y. (2014). Development of vocabulary in Spanish-speaking and Cantonese-speaking English language learners. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 35(1), 119153. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 6582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhagen, J., Grassmann, S., & Küntay, A. C. (2017). Monolingual and bilingual children’s resolution of referential conflicts: Effects of bilingualism and relative language proficiency. Cognitive Development, 41, 1018.10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W. (2016). The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 181190. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children–revised. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Woumans, E., Surmont, J., Struys, E., & Duyck, W. (2016). The longitudinal effect of bilingual immersion schooling on cognitive control and intelligence. Language Learning, 66(S1), 7691. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeng, Z., Kalashnikova, M., & Antoniou, M. (2019). Integrating bilingualism, verbal fluency, and executive functioning across the lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Development, 20(5), 656679. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1648267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, P., & Fimm, B. (2002). A test battery for attentional performance. In Leclercq, M. & Zimmermann, P. (Eds.), Applied neuropsychology of attention: Theory, diagnosis and rehabilitation (pp. 110151). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Number of participants, age range and age mean (SD) per type of school in T0 and T1

Figure 1

Table 2. Means (SDs) for age, NVR, WM, L1 vocabulary and L2 vocabulary per type of school at T0

Figure 2

Table 3. Means (SDs) for age, NVR, WM, L1 vocabulary and L2 vocabulary per type of school at T1

Figure 3

Figure 1. Improvement in background measures from T0 to T1 by groups. Note: dots represent mean values, error bars indicate standard errors of the means, and lines show the progression over time.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Interaction effect between English at school and time on L2 vocabulary.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Interaction effect between English at school and time on L1 vocabulary.

Figure 6

Table 4. Means (SDs) for each attentional/executive measure per type of school at T0

Figure 7

Table 5. Means (SDs) for each attentional/executive measure per type of school at T1

Figure 8

Figure 4. Interaction effect between English at school and time on CreatureCounting accuracy (left) and CreatureCounting timing (right).

Figure 9

Figure 5. Interaction effect between English at school and time on OppositeWorlds congruent (left) and OppositeWorlds incongruent (right).

Supplementary material: File

Chamorro et al. supplementary material

Chamorro et al. supplementary material
Download Chamorro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 4.6 MB