Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

The effects of linguistic experience on the flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning*

  • MARINA KALASHNIKOVA (a1), KAREN MATTOCK (a2) and PADRAIC MONAGHAN (a3)

Abstract

Mutual Exclusivity (ME) is a prominent constraint in language acquisition, which guides children to establish one-to-one mappings between words and referents. But how does unfolding experience of multiple-to-one word-meaning mappings in bilingual children's environment affect their understanding of when to use ME and when to accept lexical overlap? Three-to-five-year-old monolingual and simultaneous bilingual children completed two pragmatically distinct tasks, where successful word learning relied on either the default use of ME or the ability to accept overlapping labels. All children could flexibly use ME by following the social-pragmatic directions available in each task. However, linguistic experience shaped the development of ME use, whereby older monolinguals showed a greater reliance on the one-to-one mapping assumption, but older bilinguals showed a greater ability to accept lexical overlap. We suggest that flexible use of ME is thus shaped by pragmatic information present in each communicative interaction and children's individual linguistic experience.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Marina Kalashnikova, The MARCS Institute, University of Western Sydney, Locked Bag 1797, Penrith, 2751, Sydney, NSW, Australia m.kalashnikova@uws.edu.au

Footnotes

Hide All
*

We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable feedback on the earlier versions of this manuscript. We are also grateful to the children, parents, and schools who participated in this research.

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Au, T. K. F., & Glusman, M. (1990). The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: To honor or not to honor? Child Development, 67, 14741490.
Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010). Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 485508.
Byers-Heinlein, K., Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2013). The development of associative word learning in monolingual and bilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 198205.
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants’ language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science, 12, 815823.
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Lexicon structure and the disambiguation of novel words: Evidence from bilingual infants. Cognition, 128, 407416.
Clark, E. V., & Grossman, J. B. (1998). Pragmatic directions and children's word learning. Journal of Child Language, 25, 118.
Davidson, D., Jergovic, D., Imami, Z., & Theodos, V. (1997). Monolingual and bilingual children's use of the mutual exclusivity constraint. Journal of Child Language, 24, 324.
Davidson, D., & Tell, D. (2005). Monolingual and bilingual children's use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects. Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 2545.
Deák, G. O., & Maratsos, M. (1998). On having complex representations of things: Preschoolers use multiple words for objects and people. Developmental Psychology, 34, 224240.
Diesendruck, G. (2005). The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology, 41, 451463.
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630641.
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M., Sewell, J., Styles, B., Brzyska, B., Shamsan, Y., & Burge, B. (2009). British Picture Vocabulary Scale: Third edition. London: GL Assessment Limited.
Frank, I., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2002). Young monolingual and bilingual children's responses to violation of the mutual exclusivity principle. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 125146.
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference. Developmental Science, 13, 252263.
Halberda, J. (2003). The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition, 87, B23B34.
Halberda, J. (2006). Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults. Cognitive Psychology, 53, 310344.
Hall, G. (1996). Preschooler's default assumptions about word meaning: Proper names designate unique individuals. Developmental Psychology, 32, 177186.
Haryu, E. (1998). Effects of knowledge about cross-language equivalents on children's use of mutual exclusivity in interpreting novel labels. Japanese Psychological Research, 40, 8291.
Houston-Price, C., Caloghiris, Z., & Raviglione, E. (2010). Language experience shapes the development of the mutual exclusivity bias. Infancy, 15, 125150.
Jaswal, V. K. (2010). Explaining the disambiguation effect: Don't exclude mutual exclusivity. Journal of Child Language, 37, 95113.
Kalashnikova, M., Mattock, K., & Monaghan, P. (2014). Disambiguation of novel labels and referential facts: A developmental perspective. First Language, 34, 125135.
Liittschwager, J. C., & Markman, E. M. (1994). Sixteen- and 24-month olds’ use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning. Developmental Psychology, 30, 955968.
Markman, E. M. (1990). Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 14, 5777.
Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121157.
Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B. (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 241275.
Mather, E., & Plunkett, K. (2010). Mutual exclusivity and phonological novelty constrain word learning at 16 months. Journal of Child Language, 38, 933950.
Meisel, J. M. (1989). Early differentiation of languages in bilingual children. In Hyltenstam, K. & Obler, L. (eds.), Bilingualism across the life span: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss, pp. 1340. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Merriman, W. E. (1986). Some reasons for the occurrence and eventual correction of children's naming errors. Child Development, 57, 942952.
Merriman, W. E. (1991). The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning: A reply to Woodward and Markman. Developmental Review, 11, 164191.
Merriman, W. E., & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning (Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 54).
Merriman, W. E., & Kutlesic, V. (1993). Bilingual and monolingual children's use of two lexical acquisition heuristics. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 229249.
Mervis, C. B., Golinkoff, R. M., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Two-year-olds readily learn multiple labels for the same basic-level category. Child Development, 65, 11631177.
Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Saudino, K. J., Petrill, S. A., Pike, A., & Plomin, R. (2002). The validity of a parent-based assessment of cognitive abilities of three-year-olds. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 337348.
Pearson, B., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93120.
Pearson, B., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D. K. (1995). Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 22, 345368.
Piccin, T. B., & Blewitt, P. (2007). Resource conservation as a basis for the mutual exclusivity effect in children's word learning. First Language, 27, 528.
Savage, S. L., & Au, K. F. T. (1996). What word learners do when input contradicts the mutual exclusivity assumption. Child Development, 67, 31203134.
Saylor, M. M., Sabbagh, M. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2002). Children use whole–part juxtaposition as a pragmatic cue to word meaning. Developmental Psychology, 38, 9931003.
Woodward, A. L., & Markman, E. M. (1991). Constraints on learning as default assumptions: Comments on Merriman and Bowman's “The Mutual Exclusivity Bias in Children's Word Learning”. Developmental Review, 11, 137163.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition
  • ISSN: 1366-7289
  • EISSN: 1469-1841
  • URL: /core/journals/bilingualism-language-and-cognition
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed