Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa

Evidence of an advantage in visuo-spatial memory for bilingual compared to monolingual speakers

  • LUCY KERRIGAN (a1), MICHAEL S. C. THOMAS (a2), PETER BRIGHT (a1) and ROBERTO FILIPPI (a3)
Abstract

Previous research has indicated that bilinguals outperform monolinguals in cognitive tasks involving spatial working memory. The present study examines evidence for this claim using a different and arguably more ecologically valid method (the change blindness task). Bilingual and monolingual participants were presented with two versions of the same scenes and required to press a key as soon as they identified the alteration. They also completed the word and alpha span tasks, and the Corsi blocks task. The results in the change blindness task, controlled for group differences in non-verbal reasoning, indicated that bilinguals were faster and more accurate than monolinguals at detecting visual changes. Similar group differences were found on the Corsi block task. Unlike previous findings, no group differences were found on the verbal memory tasks. The results are discussed with reference to mechanisms of cognitive control as a locus of transfer between bilingualism and spatial working memory tasks.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Dr. Roberto Filippi, Anglia Ruskin University, Faculty of Science & Technology, East Road, Cambridge CB1 1PTroberto.filippi@anglia.ac.uk
Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

A. M. Bacon , F. B. Parmentier , & P. Barr (2013). Visuospatial memory in dyslexia: Evidence for strategic deficits. Memory, 21 (2), 189209.

A. Baddeley (1992). Working memory. Science, 255 (5044), 556559.

E. Bialystok (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press.

E. Bialystok , & M. M. Martin (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental science, 7 (3), 325339.

E. Bialystok , F. I. Craik , D. W. Green , & T. H. Gollan (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10 (3), 89129.

E. Bialystok , F. I. Craik , R. Klein , & M. Viswanathan (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19 (2), 290.

S. M. Carlson , & A. N. Meltzoff (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental science, 11 (2), 282298.

F. I. Craik , E. Bialystok , & M. Freedman (2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75 (19), 17261729.

A. de Bruin , B. Treccani , & S. Della Sala (2015). Cognitive Advantage in Bilingualism An Example of Publication Bias? Psychological science, 26 (1), 99107.

J. A. Duñabeitia , J. A. Hernández , E. Antón , P. Macizo , A. Estévez , L. J. Fuentes , & M. Carreiras (2015). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? Experimental psychology, 61 (3), 234.

R. Filippi , F. M. Richardson , F. Dick , R. Leech , D. W. Green , M. S. Thomas , & C. J. Price (2011). The right posterior paravermis and the control of language interference. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31 (29), 1073210740.

T. H. Gollan , R. I. Montoya , C. Fennema-Notestine , & S. K. Morris (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33 (7), 12201234.

D. W. Green (1986). Control, activation, and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and language, 27 (2), 210223.

M. D. Hilchey , & R. M. Klein (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 18 (4), 625658.

D. T. Levin , & D. J. Simons (1997). Failure to detect changes to attended objects in motion pictures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4 (4), 501506.

L. Luo , F. I. Craik , S. Moreno , & E. Bialystok (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and aging, 28 (1), 28.

J. B. Morton , & S. N. Harper (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 719726.

S. R. Mitroff , D. J. Simons , & D. T. Levin (2004). Nothing compares 2 views: Change blindness can occur despite preserved access to the changed information. Perception & Psychophysics, 66 (8), 12681281.

A. Miyake , N. P. Friedman , M. J. Emerson , A. H. Witzki , A. Howerter , & T. D. Wager (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41 (1), 49100.

K. R. Paap , & Z. I. Greenberg (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66 (2), 232258.

K. R. Paap , H. A. Johnson , & O. Sawi (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences?. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26 (6), 615639.

L. Pessoa , & L. G. Ungerleider (2004). Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness in a working memory task. Cerebral Cortex, 14 (5), 511520.

R. A. Rensink (2002). Change detection. Annual review of psychology, 53 (1), 245277.

R. A. Rensink , J. K. O'Regan , & J. J. Clark (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological science, 8 (5), 368373.

D. J. Saer (1923). The effect of bilingualism on intelligence. British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 14 (1), 2538.

D. J. Simons , & D. T. Levin (2003). What makes change blindness interesting?. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 42, 295322.

D. J. Simons , & R. A. Rensink (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9 (1), 1620.

M. Toepper , H. Gebhardt , T. Beblo , C. Thomas , M. Driessen , M. Bischoff , C. R. Blecker , D. Vaitl , & G. Sammer (2010). Functional correlates of distractor suppression during spatial working memory encoding. Neuroscience, 165 (4), 12441253.

A. Vandierendonck , E. Kemps , M. C. Fastame , & A. Szmalec (2004). Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. British Journal of Psychology, 95 (1), 5779.

M. Vega-Mendoza , H. West , A. Sorace , & T. H. Bak (2015). The impact of late, non-balanced bilingualism on cognitive performance. Cognition, 137, 4046.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition
  • ISSN: 1366-7289
  • EISSN: 1469-1841
  • URL: /core/journals/bilingualism-language-and-cognition
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords:

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 6
Total number of PDF views: 70 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 304 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 27th March 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.