Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

Relations between vocabulary and executive functions in Spanish–English dual language learners

  • VRINDA KALIA (a1), M. PAULA DANERI (a2) and MAKEBA PARRAMORE WILBOURN (a3)

Abstract

The role of dual language exposure in children's cognitive development continues to be debated. The majority of the research with bilingual children in the US has been conducted with children becoming literate in only one of their languages. Dual language learners who are becoming literate in both their languages are acutely understudied. We compared dual language learners (n = 61) in a Spanish–English dual language immersion program to monolingual English speaking children (n = 55) who were in a traditional English only school. Children (kindergarten to 3rd grade) completed standardized vocabulary tasks and two measures of executive functions. Despite having significantly smaller English vocabularies, the dual language learners outperformed the monolingual children on the executive function measures. Implications for our understanding of the relations between oral language development and executive function in bilingual children are discussed.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Vrinda Kalia, Department of Psychology-Miami University, 90 North Patterson Avenue, Oxford, OH 45056kaliav@miamioh.edu

Footnotes

Hide All

We thank the International Literacy Association for supporting this project through the Elva Knight Research Grant.

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Anton, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estevez, A., Hernandez, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., Davidson, D. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398
Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83, 413422. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707.x
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 699714. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003
Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G., Laneri, M., Fletcher, K., & Metevia, L. (2001). Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 541556. doi:10.1023/A:1012233310098
Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., & Petitto, L.A. (2013). Should bilingual children learn reading in two languages at the same time or in sequence? Bilingual Research Journal, 36 (1), 3560. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2013.779618
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81 (6), 16411660. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy and cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 525531. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990423
Bialystok, E., Peets, K. F., & Moreno, S. (2014). Producing bilinguals through immersion education: Development of metalinguistic awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35 (01), 177191. doi:10.1017/S0142716412000288
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122, 6773. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003.
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9 (2), 117121. doi:10.1111/cdep.12116
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78 (2), 647663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x
Brysbaert, M. (1998). Word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence against the existence of two separate lexicons. Psychologica Belgica, 38 (3-4), 163175.
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28 (2), 595616. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 (2), 282298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x
Child Trends. (2014). America's Hispanic children: Gaining ground, looking forward. (No. 2014–38). Washington, DC: Author.
Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P. (1989). How quickly can immigrants become proficient in school English. Journal of Educational Issues of Language Minority Students, 5 (1), 2638.
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (2), 302312. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.302
Conboy, B. T., & Thal, D. J. (2006). Ties between the lexicon and grammar: Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of bilingual toddlers. Child Development, 77 (3), 712735. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00899.x
Cragg, L., & Nation, K. (2010). Language and the development of cognitive control. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2 (4), 631642. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01080.x
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test, fourth edition: Manual. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.
Dunn, L. M., Padilla, E., Lugo, D., & Dunn, L. (1986). Test de vocabulario en imagenes peabody – adaptacion hispanoamericana. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology, 61, 234251.
Esposito, A. G., & Baker-Ward, L. (2013). Dual-language education for low-income children: Preliminary evidence of benefits for executive function. Bilingual Research Journal, 36, 295310. doi:10.1080/15235882.2013.837848
Espy, K. A., Bull, R., Martin, J., & Stroup, W. (2006). Measuring the development of executive control with the shape school. Psychological Assessment, 18 (4), 373381. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.373
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2014). Bilingual children show advantages in nonverbal auditory executive function task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18 (6), 717729. doi: 10.1177/1367006912472263
Francis, W. S. (1999). Cognitive integration of language and memory in bilinguals: Semantic representation. Psychological Bulletin, 125 (2), 193222. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.193
Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Guasch, N. V., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011
Gomez, L., Freeman, D., & Freeman, Y. (2005). Dual language education: A promising 50–50 model. Bilingual Research Journal: The Journal of the National Association for Bilingual Education, 29, 145164. doi:10.1080/15235882.2005.10162828
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system . Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 6781. doi:10.1017/S1366728998000133
Groot, A. S., De Sonneville, L. M., Stins, J. F., & Boomsma, D. I. (2004). Familial influences on sustained attention and inhibition in preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45 (2), 306314.
Gutierrez, K. D., Zepeda, M., & Castro, D. C. (2010). Advancing early literacy learning for all children: Implications of the NELP report for dual language learners. Educational Researcher, 39, 334339. doi:10.3102/0013189X10369831
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (2), 6573. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003
Hakuta, K. (1986). Mirror of language: The debate on bilingualism. New York: Basic Books
Hammer, C. S., Jia, G., & Uchikoshi, Y. (2011). Language and literacy development of dual language learners growing up in the United States: A call for research. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 49. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00140.x
Hermanto, N., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Linguistic and metalinguistic outcomes of intense immersion education: How bilingual? International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 15 (2), 131145. doi:10.1080/13670050.2011.652591
Hernandez, A., Li, P., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 220225. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.003
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on non-linguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 18, 625658. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7
Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617644. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The flexible item selection task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 573591. doi:10.1207/S15326942DN2003_2
Kirkham, N. Z., Cruess, L. M., & Diamond, A. (2003). Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task. Developmental Science, 6, 449467. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00300
Kolb, B., & Gibb, R. (2011). Brain plasticity and behaviour in the developing brain. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20 (4), 265276.
Kolb, B., Mychasiuk, R., Muhammad, A., Li, Y., Frost, D. O., & Gibb, R. (2012). Experience and the developing prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 1718617193. doi:10.1073/pnas.1121251109.
Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Wilbourn, M. P., Vernon-Feagans, L., Blair, C. B., & The Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Development, 85 (5), 18981914. doi:10.1111/cdev.12249.
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43 (4), 947959. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947
McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Wanless, S. B., & Murray, A. (2007). Executive function, behavioral self-regulation, and social-emotional competence: Links to school readiness. In Saracho, O. N. & Spodek, B. (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on research in social learning in early childhood education, (pp. 83–107). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41 (1), 49100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Morton, J. B. (2010). Language, bilingualism, and executive functioning in early development. Psychological Reports, 107, 888890. doi:10.2466/04.11.28.PR0.107.6.888-890
Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x
Neter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., & Wasserman, W. (1996). Applied linear statistical models. Boston, MA: McGraw Hill College.
Nicolay, A.C., & Poncelet, M. (2015). Cognitive benefits in children enrolled in an early bilingual immersion school: A follow-up study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (3), 789795.
Nicolay, A.C., & Poncelet, M. (2013a). Cognitive advantage in children enrolled in a second-language immersion elementary school program for 3 years. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 (3), 597607.
Nicolay, A.C., & Poncelet, M. (2013b). Cognitive abilities underlying second-language vocabulary acquisition in an early second-language immersion education context: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 655671.
Oh, S., & Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development, 70, 8099. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01112.x
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265278.
Páez, M. M., Tabors, P. O., & López, L. M. (2007). Dual language and literacy development of Spanish- speaking preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 85102. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2006. 12.007
Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., Bedore, L. M., & Bohman, T. M. (2011). Risk for poor performance on a language screening measure for bilingual preschoolers and kindergarteners. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 302314. doi:10.1044/1058-0360
Peña, E. D., & Halle, T. G. (2011). Assessing preschool dual language learners: Traveling a multiforked road. Child Development Perspectives, 5 (1), 2832. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00143.x
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2‐year‐olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child development, 82 (6), 18341849. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.x
Poarch, G. J., & Van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 535551. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013
Potowski, K. (2005). Latino children's Spanish use in a Chicago dual-immersion classroom. In Farr, M. (Ed.), Latino language and literacy in ethnolinguistic Chicago (pp. 157186). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rojas, R., & Iglesias, A. (2013). The language growth of Spanish‐speaking English language learners. Child Development, 84 (2), 630646. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01871.x
Singer, B. D., & Bashir, A. S. (1999). What are executive functions and self-regulation and what do they have to do with language-learning disorders? Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 265273. doi:doi:10.1044/0161-1461.3003.265
Smith, M. C. (1997). How do bilinguals access lexical information? In Groot, A. M. B. de, & Kroll, J. F. (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 145168). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wilbourn, M. P., Kurtz, L. E., & Kalia, V. (2012). The Lexical Stroop Sort (LSS) picture-word task: A computerized task for assessing the relationship between language and executive functioning in school-aged children. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 270286. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0142-4
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297301. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006. 46
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Bilingualism: Language and Cognition
  • ISSN: 1366-7289
  • EISSN: 1469-1841
  • URL: /core/journals/bilingualism-language-and-cognition
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed