Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-tj2md Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T14:06:16.068Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The paradoxical effect of COVID-19 outbreak on loneliness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2021

David Bartrés-Faz*
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Barcelona, Spain; and Guttmann Institute, University Institute of Neurorehabilitation affiliated to the Autonomous University of Barcelona (UAB), Spain
Dídac Macià
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, University of Barcelona, Spain
Gabriele Cattaneo
Affiliation:
Guttmann Institute, University Institute of Neurorehabilitation affiliated to the Autonomous University of Barcelona (UAB), Spain; and Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain
Roger Borràs
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, Institute of Cllinical Neurosciences, Clinical and Provincial Hospital of Barcelona, Spain
Clara Tarrero
Affiliation:
Guttmann Institute, University Institute of Neurorehabilitation affiliated to the Autonomous University of Barcelona (UAB), Spain; and Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain
Javier Solana
Affiliation:
Guttmann Institute, University Institute of Neurorehabilitation affiliated to the Autonomous University of Barcelona (UAB), Spain; and Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain
José M. Tormos
Affiliation:
Guttmann Institute, University Institute of Neurorehabilitation affiliated to the Autonomous University of Barcelona (UAB), Spain; and Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain
Alvaro Pascual-Leone
Affiliation:
Guttmann Institute, University Institute of Neurorehabilitation affiliated to the Autonomous University of Barcelona (UAB), Spain; and Hinda and Arthur Marcus Institute for Aging Research, Hebrew SeniorLife, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA.
*
Correspondence: David Bartrés-Faz. Email: dbartres@ub.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

As in previous periods of quarantine, lockdown confinement measures dictated to control SARS-CoV-2 would be expected to negatively affect mental health. We investigated the immediate effects (over a 10 day period) of a strict nationwide stay-at-home order imposed in Spain, one of the countries most affected by the COVID-19 pandemic. Focusing our analysis on the feelings of loneliness, we obtained our measures within a social context characterised by strong and continuous public and governmental support for increasing social bonds and cooperation in order to face the common public threat. Leveraging data from the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative, a prospective population-based study cohort, the short UCLA Loneliness Scale was administered to 1604 participants 2 years and 1 year before the stay-at-home lockdown and repeated, on average, 10 days after the official confinement order issued by the Spanish government. Ratings of loneliness remained stable during the 2 years before lockdown; however, they decreased significantly during the early stages of home confinement. This effect was particularly significant for the item ‘feeling excluded from others’ and was also observed among individuals who were confined alone. Overall, the results suggest that gestures and manifestations of appreciation by people for the labour and efforts of certain individuals, along with official campaigns designed to promote feelings of inclusion and belonging, may have beneficial effects on feelings of loneliness, a negative emotional state strongly regarded as a risk factor for impaired mental and general health status. Further assessments during the later stages of home confinement are now warranted.

Type
Short report
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

In early April 2020, almost 1.5 million cases of COVID-19 had been confirmed in more than 200 countries, and over 80 000 deaths had already been recorded. Spain had the second highest number of confirmed COVID-19 cases after the USA, and the highest rate of cases per inhabitant.Reference Worldometer1 As in many countries, the government in Spain issued a nationwide stay-at-home order imposing strict lockdown measures in mid-March.2

The potentially negative psychological effects of quarantine have been described elsewhere,Reference Brooks, Webster, Smith, Woodland, Wessely and Greenberg3 including observations of the associations between loneliness and mental health issues (e.g. anxiety, depression, suicidal ideation) within the context of the COVID-19 pandemic,Reference Tso and Park4 and of increases in feelings of loneliness following the stay-at-home restrictions in both general community samplesReference Killgore, Cloonan, Taylor, Miller and Dailey5 and specific populations such as university students.Reference Elmer, Mepham and Stadtfeld6 However, across the world people have been coming together during the pandemic in new ways to cope with the crisis and the challenge of confinement. For example, in Spain, every evening at 20:00 h, from windows, front doors, balconies or terraces, thousands of people applauded frontline health professionals and acknowledged them as ‘heroes of the nation’. Families and friends found new ways to connect for online meals, drinks or games. As in other countries, the official motto of Spain's governmental COVID-19 campaign, ‘éste virus lo paramos unidos’ (united we will stop this virus) reinforced the importance of social bonds and cooperation.

In this study, we wanted to assess whether feelings of loneliness, defined as the feeling that one's desired quantity or quality of social connections is unfulfilled,Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo7,Reference Peplau and Perlman8 in particular those referring to the subjective sense of social belonging, underwent changes during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic and, if so, whether they increased or decreased.

Method

Study participants were part of the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative (BBHI, www.bbhi.cat/en), an ongoing longitudinal cohort study investigating the determinants of brain and mental health in middle and advanced age. The BBHI involves periodic medical, cognitive, brain imaging and biological assessments.Reference Cattaneo, Bartrés-Faz, Morris, Sánchez, Macià and Tarrero C9 The present study included a subsample of 1604 participants demographically representative of the full BBHI cohort (n = 5592 as of April 2020). The mean age of the subsample was 55.7 years (s.d. 7.3); 65% were women, and 75% had completed higher education, 22% secondary and 3% only primary. The corresponding figures for the full BBHI cohort were: mean age 54.4 years (s.d. 7.2), 67% women, 71% completed higher education, 25% secondary and 4% only primary.

None of the respondents reported a diagnosis of a psychiatric or neurological disorder by a physician, and all completed the three waves of loneliness assessment using the short UCLA Loneliness Scale,Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo10 which consists of three items in which participants had to rate the frequency of several experiences (‘How often do you feel isolated from others?’, ‘How often do you feel excluded?’ and ‘How often do you feel that you lack company’) on a three-point Likert scale (options: ‘1: Hardly ever’, ‘2: Some of the time’, or ‘3: Often’). The scale was administered through the BBHI web platform (https://bbhi.cat/en/), a personalised platform where participants are requested to fill in a series of questionnaires that assess their self-perceived health and engagement in various lifestyles (cognitive, physical, sleep, social and nutrition habits). For the current study, UCLA ratings were available for 2 years prior to the COVID-19 lockdown and were obtained again some 10 days (range 9–13, s.d. 1.2) after official confinement. The authors certify that all procedures contributing to this study comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human participants or patients were approved by the Unió Catalana d'Hospitals ethics committee; approval numbers: CEIC 17/06 (phase 1), CEI 18/07 (phase 2). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants or patients. The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, D.B.F., upon reasonable request.

The longitudinal three-item UCLA score (the sum of the three items of the scale) was modelled as a continuous variable using a linear mixed model allowing for a random intercept per participant. We also analysed each of the three individual items of the scale separately, after dichotomisation of the responses. As there were very few level 3 answers (‘often feeling lacking company, excluded, or isolated’: <4%, see Fig. 1), we collapsed them with level 2 (‘some of the times feeling …’) and opposed them to level 1 (‘hardly ever feeling …’). Thus, we obtained a separate binary longitudinal outcome for each of the three items of the scale, which we modelled with binomial linear mixed models including a random intercept per participant. Fixed effect parameters could be interpreted as odds ratios relative to the reference timepoint (2 years before the COVID-19 outbreak). Confidence intervals were estimated from model parameter standard deviations. All analyses were carried out with the functions lmer and glmer of the lme4 R package.Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker11 Psychometric statistics were calculated following the standard formulas implemented in the psych R package.Reference Revelle12

Fig. 1. Frequency of loneliness responses before and during COVID-19 confinement.

Results

Ratings from the three UCLA items remained stable for the two pre-COVID-19 years (expected fixed effect intercepts: (2 years before) = 3.73, CI 3.70–3.75, (1 year before) = 3.73, CI 3.71–3.76). In clear contrast, scores fell significantly in the during COVID-19 assessment (3.52, CI 3.50–3.54) (see also descriptive statistics in Fig. 1).

Furthermore, this decrease did not occur homogeneously across all items of the UCLA scale. Instead, one item (feelings of being excluded from others, item 2) drove most of the reduction. Thus, the odds of ‘sometimes’ or ‘often’ feeling a lack of company (item 1), excluded from others (item 2) or isolated (item 3) presented reductions of varying degree, the largest emerging for feelings of being excluded (item 2). When the analysis was restricted to participants confined alone (N = 169), loneliness was overall higher than in those not confined alone before and during COVID-19: (2 years before) = 4.33, CI 4.15–4.53; (1 year before) = 4.28, CI 4.09–4.47 and (during COVID-19) = 4.14, CI 3.95–4.30. Investigating the UCLA items separately, we observed comparable patterns of significant reductions in loneliness during the confinement; once again, the largest reductions were observed in feelings of exclusion (Table 1).

Table 1 Odds ratios of loneliness items before and during COVID-19 confinement

Odds ratios compare the odds of often or sometimes feeling lacking company, excluded or isolated compared with hardly ever in the two PRE-COVID-19 assessments (left column) and in the DURING-COVID-19 compared with the PRE-COVID-19 (1 y) assessment (right column). Top: odds obtained from all participants having answered the three loneliness assessments. Bottom: odds obtained from participants confined alone after COVID-19 outbreak and who already lived alone before the outbreak. Odds ratios are derived from a binomial linear mixed model with a random intercept for each participant; 95% confidence intervals are obtained from the estimated standard deviation of the regression coefficients.

a. 95% CI not including lack of effect (OR = 1).

The differences in the decreases in item score probabilities suggested a potential change in the internal coherence of the scale (cross-item correlation) after the pandemic outbreak. Indeed, we found that the DURING-COVID-19 pattern of scoring had altered the internal consistency of the UCLA scale. The two PRE-COVID-19 assessments exhibited good internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.76, CI 0.74–0.78, α = 0.75, CI 0.73–0.77) in agreement with the published literature,Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo10 but in the DURING-COVID-19 evaluation, the consistency fell below psychometrically acceptable levels (α = 0.63, CI 0.60–0.66). The main driver of this disruption was again the change in scores for exclusion, its item-scale correlation dropping from r = 0.63 and 0.62 in the two PRE-COVID-19 assessments to r = 0.42 in the DURING-COVID-19 assessment.

Discussion

Leveraging data from the BBHI study cohort, we observed that the influence of home confinement due to COVID-19 on the various subcomponents of loneliness presented differences, to the point of altering the established internal consistency of the UCLA scale. In other words, the subcomponents of the Loneliness scale were notably decoupled by the confinement. Feelings of exclusion from others (item 2) were the most reduced in all analyses, whereas scores on the Isolation subscale (item 3) decreased in the overall sample but were unchanged in people living alone. These findings support the notion that loneliness is not a unitary, isolated construct but rather represents a cluster of subjective and objective experiences of social integration and socioemotional states.Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo10

The observation that the UCLA scale item ‘feelings of exclusion’ exhibited the greatest fall in score during a hard lockdown, which included an enforced reduction of physical social contacts, may be related to the general contextual component, namely, that people knew that there was nothing to be done about the limitation on social contacts, and that everybody else was in the same situation. This interpretation may be aligned with findings of a recent longitudinal study during COVID-19 lockdown conducted among university students who, during the confinement, reported worrying less about the possibility that others were having more rewarding experiences (i.e. fear of missing out) and perceived less competition.Reference Elmer, Mepham and Stadtfeld6 In our study, during lockdown, contact with close family members and friends may have been more easily maintained either within a shared household or using online methods, even though wider community life stopped, and this may have contributed to the decrease in feelings of exclusion.

In addition to this effect, there are other plausible sociological and psychological causes that might explain why feelings of exclusion suddenly dropped during the first weeks of lockdown in Spain. Some may appear obvious to many of us who lived through the experience, such as a sudden surge of patriotism, both spontaneous and fostered by the government's communication campaign, or a shift from a focus on personal problems towards a shared problem affecting the entire community. Our intriguing findings should invite replication with data from other countries, where strict lockdowns were imposed, or even building on social identity theory propounding that times of uncertainty and crisis predispose individuals to powerful identification with groups,Reference Hogg13 and should encourage further research into the psychological and political predictors which, despite their observational nature, may shed light on certain psychological and sociopolitical causal mechanisms.

The main implications of our findings are that loneliness, and in particular feelings of inclusion and social belonging, can be improved without the need to enlarge the individual's physical social network. Therefore, programmes designed to address loneliness should not solely aim to ‘physically’ reconnect people (e.g., Shared Lives UK; see https://sharedlivesplus.org.uk/news-campaigns-and-jobs/growing-shared-lives/) but should consider incorporating or strengthening resources such as digital tools and the use of internet and social media to engage people creatively in new forms of social support, improving access to information about local community groups and promoting social integration. This is aligned with initiatives that aim to enhance collective creativity and increase the sense of ‘making things together’ which have been implemented during the quarantine in charitable campaigns such as the Campaign to End Loneliness (see https://bemoreus.org.uk/the-quarantine-quilt-connecting-through-creativity/).

Pending future assessments to determine whether feelings of loneliness fluctuate further during more extended periods of confinement or in the face of changes in the social context, the present findings provide new evidence of the psychological responses to a state of confinement, and may also offer valuable insights for future campaigns and interventions to reduce loneliness.Reference Cacioppo, Grippo, London, Goossens and Cacioppo14

Funding

The research leading to these results received funding from ‘la Caixa’ Foundation (LCF/PR/PR16/11110004), and also from the Guttmann Institute and Fundació Abertis. D.B.-F. was funded by an ICREA Academia 2019 Award and by a Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (RTI2018-095181-B-C21) research grant.

Author contributions

G.C., J.S. and D.M. had full data access to all data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the analyses. R.B. provided expert statistical analysis support. Concept and design was undertaken by all authors. D.B.-F., A.P.-L. and D.M. drafted the manuscript. D.B.-F., A.P.-L. and J.M.T.carried out critical revision and supervision.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, D.B.-F., upon reasonable request.

Declaration of interest

None.

Footnotes

1

These authors contributed equally to this article.

References

Worldometer, . COVID-19 Coronavirus Pandemic. Worldometer, 2020 (https://www.worldometers.info/coronavirus/?utm_campaign=homeAdvegas1?).Google Scholar
Spanish Official Government Bulletin (Boletin Oficial del Estado). National Alarm State Order, March, 18, 2020. Official State Gazette Agency, 2020 (https://www.boe.es/eli/es/rd/2020/03/14/463/con).Google Scholar
Brooks, SK, Webster, RK, Smith, LE, Woodland, L, Wessely, S, Greenberg, N, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 2020; 395(10227): 912–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tso, IF, Park, S. Alarming levels of psychiatric symptoms and the role of loneliness during the COVID-19 epidemic: a case study of Hong Kong. Psychiatry Res 2020; 293: 113423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killgore, WDS, Cloonan, SA, Taylor, EC, Miller, MA, Dailey, NS. Three months of loneliness during the COVID-19 lockdown. Psychiatry Res 2020; 293: 113392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elmer, T, Mepham, K, Stadtfeld, C. Students under lockdown: comparisons of students’ social networks and mental health before and during the COVID-19 crisis in Switzerland. PLoS One 2020; 15(7): e0236337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkley, LC, Cacioppo, JT. Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann Behav Med 2010; 40: 218–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peplau, LA, Perlman, D. Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research, and Therapy. Wiley, 1982.Google Scholar
Cattaneo, G, Bartrés-Faz, D, Morris, TP, Sánchez, JS, Macià, D, Tarrero C, , et al. The Barcelona brain health initiative: a cohort study to define and promote determinants of brain health. Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10: 321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, ME, Waite, LJ, Hawkley, LC, Cacioppo, JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two population-based studies. Res Aging 2004; 26(6): 655–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Mächler, M, Bolker, B, Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 2015; 67(1): 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. R package version 1.9.12. Northwestern University, 2019. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.Google Scholar
Hogg, MA. Subjective uncertainty reduction through self-categorization: a motivational theory of social identity processes. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 2000; 11(1): 223–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, S, Grippo, AJ, London, S, Goossens, L, Cacioppo, JT. Loneliness: clinical import and interventions. Perspect Psychol Sci 2015; 10(2): 238–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Frequency of loneliness responses before and during COVID-19 confinement.

Figure 1

Table 1 Odds ratios of loneliness items before and during COVID-19 confinement

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.