Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T05:47:18.685Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Normal Bias Toward a Pictorially Defined Top in Line Bisection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2015

Cory Toth*
Affiliation:
Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon
Andrew Kirk*
Affiliation:
Division of Neurology, Department of Medicine, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon
*
Department of Medicine, Royal University Hospital, 103 Hospital Drive, Saskatoon. Saskatchewan, Canada S7N 0W8
Department of Medicine, Royal University Hospital, 103 Hospital Drive, Saskatoon. Saskatchewan, Canada S7N 0W8
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract:

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Background: We set out to determine whether separable visual and representational components underlie normal subjects’ upward and distal biases in bisecting vertical and radial lines under visual guidance. Methods: Thirty-four normal subjects were asked to bisect lines oriented horizontally, vertically, and radially. Human silhouette figures were placed at either end of each line. These figures were presented upright or upside down in order to pictorially define a “top” to each line independent of the actual top of the visual field. Results: Although subjects erred toward the top of the visual field, they also demonstrated a significant bias toward the heads of the figures for lines in all spatial orientations. Conclusions: This result supports the existence of two biases: one toward the upper visual field, and another toward an internally represented “top” as suggested pictorially. These findings provide further support for the hypothesis that normal subjects’ upward and distal biases on bisection of vertical and radial lines under visual guidance have both representational and visual-based components.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Neurological Sciences Federation 1996

References

1.Shelton, PA, Bowers, D, Heilman, KM.Personal and vertical neglect Brain 1990; 113: 191205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Mennemeier, M, Wertman, E, Heilman, KM.Evidence for multidirectional attentional systems in humans Brain 1992; 115: 3750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Geldmacher, DS, Heilman, KM.Differences in radial line bisection above and below eye level [abstract]. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1992; 14:35.Google Scholar
4.Jeerakathil, TJ, Kirk, A.A representational vertical bias. Neurology 1994; 44:703706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Kirk, A, Boyle, C.“Radial neglect” is caused by vertical neglect of an internal representation [abstract]. Can J Neurol Sci 1994; 21: 519.Google Scholar
6.Heilman, KM, Watson, RT, Valenstein, E.Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman, KM, Valenstein, E, eds. Clinical Neuropsychology, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University, 1993; 248298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7.Bradshaw, JL, Nettleton, NC, Nathan, G, Wilson, L.Bisecting rods and lines: effects of horizontal and vertical posture on left-side underestimation by normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 1985; 23:421425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Halligan, PW, Marshall, JC.Line bisection in visuo-spatial neglect: disproof of a conjecture. Cortex 1989; 25: 517522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Shepard, RN, Metzler, J.Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science; 171: 701703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar