Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T10:42:30.173Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Newly Identified Impairment in Both Vision and Hearing Increases the Risk of Deterioration in Both Communication and Cognitive Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 September 2021

Dawn M. Guthrie*
Affiliation:
Department of Kinesiology & Physical Education, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario Department of Health Sciences, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario
Nicole Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Kinesiology & Physical Education, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario
Jennifer Campos
Affiliation:
Toronto Rehabilitation Institute – University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario
Paul Mick
Affiliation:
Department of Surgery, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan
Joseph B. Orange
Affiliation:
School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Western University, London, Ontario
M. Kathleen Pichora-Fuller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario
Marie Y. Savundranayagam
Affiliation:
School of Health Studies, Western University, London, Ontario
Walter Wittich
Affiliation:
School of Optometry, University of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec Lethbridge-Layton-Mackay Rehabilitation Centre of West-Central Montreal, Montreal, Quebec Institut Nazareth et Louis-Braille du Centre intégré de santé et de services sociaux de la Montérégie-Centre, Longueuil, Québec
Natalie A. Phillips
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology/Centre for Research in Human Development, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec
*
Corresponding author: La correspondance et les demandes de tirés-à-part doivent être adressées à : / Correspondence and requests for offprints should be sent to: Dawn M. Guthrie, Ph.D. Department of Kinesiology & Physical Education 75 University Ave. W. Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, Ontario Canada N2L 3C5 (dguthrie@wlu.ca)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Vision and hearing impairments are highly prevalent in adults 65 years of age and older. There is a need to understand their association with multiple health-related outcomes. We analyzed data from the Resident Assessment Instrument for Home Care (RAI-HC). Home care clients were followed for up to 5 years and categorized into seven unique cohorts based on whether or not they developed new vision and/or hearing impairments. An absolute standardized difference (stdiff) of at least 0.2 was considered statistically meaningful. Most clients (at least 60%) were female and 34.9 per cent developed a new sensory impairment. Those with a new concurrent vison and hearing impairment were more likely than those with no sensory impairments to experience a deterioration in receptive communication (stdiff = 0.68) and in cognitive performance (stdiff = 0.49). After multivariate adjustment, they had a twofold increased odds (adjusted odds ratio [OR] = 2.1; 95% confidence interval [CI]:1,87, 2.35) of deterioration in cognitive performance. Changes in sensory functioning are common and have important effects on multiple health-related outcomes.

Résumé

Résumé

Les déficiences visuelles et auditives présentent un fort taux de prévalence chez les personnes âgées. Il est nécessaire de comprendre comment ces déficiences sont associées avec plusieurs autres données liées à la santé. Nous avons analysé les résultats obtenus au test Resident Assessment Instrument for Home Care (RAI-HC). Les clients recevant des soins à domicile ont été suivis pendant une période allant jusqu’à cinq ans et ont été classés selon sept cohortes, en fonction de l’apparition ou non de nouvelles déficiences visuelles ou auditives. Une différence absolue standardisée (diffst) d’au moins 0,2 était considérée comme statistiquement significative. La plupart des clients (au moins 60 %) étaient des femmes et 34,9 % ont développé une nouvelle déficience sensorielle. Ceux qui ont développé une double déficience sensorielle (visuelle et auditive) étaient plus susceptibles de présenter une détérioration de la communication réceptive (diffst = 0,68) et de la performance cognitive (diffst = 0,49) que ceux qui n’avaient aucune déficience sensorielle. Le risque de détérioration des performances cognitives dans le cas des doubles déficiences sensorielles était deux fois plus élevé (RR ajusté = 2,1 ; IC 95 % : 1,87-2,35) suivant l’ajustement multivarié. Les altérations des fonctions sensorielles sont courantes et leurs effets sur de multiples indicateurs liés à la santé sont importants.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Canadian Association on Gerontology 2021

Sensory impairments increase with age and are very common among older adults (≥ 65 years of age). The prevalence rates of these impairments are expected to increase over time because the population is aging (Mathers & Loncar, Reference Mathers and Loncar2006). Approximately 65 per cent of Canadians 70 years of age and older have a hearing impairment (HI), with both incidence and prevalence rates rising with each decade of life (Feder, Michaud, Ramage-Morin, McNamee, & Beauregard, Reference Feder, Michaud, Ramage-Morin, McNamee and Beauregard2015). In 2016, 1,500,000 Canadian males 45–85 years of age had at least mild hearing loss, 1,800,000 had at least mild vision loss, and 570,000 had both. Among females, 1,200,000 had at least mild hearing loss, 2,200,000 had at least mild vision loss, and 450,000 had both (Mick et al., Reference Mick, Hämäläinen, Kolisang, Pichora-Fuller, Phillips and Guthrie2020). It has been estimated that HI and vision impairment (VI), respectively, are the second- and third-most common impairments worldwide (Vos, Reference Vos2016).

HI and VI in older adults are particularly important to understand given their influence on multiple health-related outcomes. For example, HI is associated with poor self-rated health (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Betz, Deal, Contrera, Genther and Chen2015) and difficulties with activities of daily living (ADLs) and instrumental ADLs (IADLs) (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Betz, Yaffe, Ayonayon, Kritchevsky and Martin2015; Choi et al., Reference Choi, Betz, Deal, Contrera, Genther and Chen2015; Slaughter, Hopper, Ickert, & Erin, Reference Slaughter, Hopper, Ickert and Erin2014), falls (Campos, Ramkhalawansingh, & Pichora-Fuller, Reference Campos, Ramkhalawansingh and Pichora-Fuller2018; Jiam, Li, & Agrawal, Reference Jiam, Li and Agrawal2016; Lin & Ferrucci, Reference Lin and Ferrucci2012), and is the top potentially modifiable risk factor for dementia (Davies, Cadar, Herbert, Orrell, & Steptoe, Reference Davies, Cadar, Herbert, Orrell and Steptoe2017; Deal et al., Reference Deal, Betz, Yaffe, Harris, Purchase-Helzner and Satterfield2017; Fritze et al., Reference Fritze, Teipel, Ovari, Kilimann, Witt and Doblhammer2016). HI is also associated with lower social support and loneliness (Mick, Parfyonov, Wittich, Phillips, & Pichora-Fuller, Reference Mick, Parfyonov, Wittich, Phillips and Pichora-Fuller2018).

Similarly, VI is associated with an increased risk of mortality (Reuben, Mui, Damesyn, Moore, & Greendale, Reference Reuben, Mui, Damesyn, Moore and Greendale1999; Wang, Mitchell, Simpson, Cumming, & Smith, Reference Wang, Mitchell, Simpson, Cumming and Smith2001), difficulties with ADLs and IADLs (Grue et al., Reference Grue, Finne-Soveri, Stolee, Poss, Sorbye and Noro2009; Reuben et al., Reference Reuben, Mui, Damesyn, Moore and Greendale1999), and difficulties caused by reduced mobility (Wang, Mitchell, Smith, Cumming, & Attebo, Reference Wang, Mitchell, Smith, Cumming and Attebo1999). Individuals with VI also have lower social support, increased loneliness, reduced social participation, and smaller social networks (Grue et al., Reference Grue, Finne-Soveri, Stolee, Poss, Sorbye and Noro2009; Laliberte Rudman et al., Reference Laliberte Rudman, Gold, McGrath, Zuvela, Spafford and Renwick2016). Compared with those who have no VI, they are also more likely to receive community-based supports such as home care and meals-on-wheels (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Mitchell, Smith, Cumming and Attebo1999).

Older people with a combination of VI and HI, known as dual sensory impairment (DSI), are a particularly vulnerable group (Simcock, Reference Simcock2017; Simcock & Wittich, Reference Simcock and Wittich2019; Wittich & Simcock, Reference Wittich, Simcock and Ravenscroft2019). They experience limitations in completing ADLs and IADLs (Smith, Bennett, & Wilson, Reference Smith, Bennett and Wilson2008), are at increased risk for depression (Capella-McDonnall, Reference Capella-McDonnall2009; Fletcher & Guthrie, Reference Fletcher and Guthrie2013; Guthrie, Theriault, & Davidson, Reference Guthrie, Theriault and Davidson2015; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Gopinath, McMahon, Leeder, Mitchell and Wang2011) and mortality (Reuben et al., Reference Reuben, Mui, Damesyn, Moore and Greendale1999), have significantly impaired communication function (McDonnall, Crudden, LeJeune, Steverson, & O’Donnell, Reference McDonnall, Crudden, LeJeune, Steverson and O’Donnell2016) and have reduced social participation (Mick et al., Reference Mick, Parfyonov, Wittich, Phillips and Pichora-Fuller2018). Despite these findings, there is very limited research globally on older adults with DSI (Heine & Browning, Reference Heine and Browning2015), and this is also true in Canada (Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Theriault and Davidson2015; Wittich, Watanabe, & Gagné, Reference Wittich, Watanabe and Gagné2011).

Previous research in Canada points to differential outcomes for individuals with VI, HI, or DSI. For example, in cross-sectional analyses, individuals with DSI and cognitive challenges were the most likely to experience communication difficulties, deterioration in communication over time, or a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s dementia, and to have a primary caregiver feeling distressed (Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018). HI has also been shown to interact with impaired cognitive functioning to influence the risk of admission to long-term care. Individuals with an HI, but no cognitive challenges, had a faster time to admission versus clients with both HI and cognitive difficulties (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Phillips, Wittich, Campos, Mick and Orange2020). Home care clients who experienced significant deterioration in their hearing are more likely than those with no changes in their hearing to experience communication difficulties and to have a caregiver who is distressed (Williams, Guthrie, Davidson, Fisher, & Griffith, Reference Williams, Guthrie, Davidson, Fisher and Griffith2018).

The current literature linking sensory impairments and outcomes such as communication difficulties, cognitive decline, and caregiver burden has several gaps. The research designs have been mainly cross-sectional, few studies have included adults with DSI, and little research has been completed in Canada. Previous studies have primarily investigated community-dwelling older adults (Alattar et al., Reference Alattar, Bergstrom, Laughlin, Kritz-Silverstein, Richard and Reas2020; Amieva et al., Reference Amieva, Ouvrard, Giulioli, Meillon, Rullier and Dartigues2015; Davies et al., Reference Davies, Cadar, Herbert, Orrell and Steptoe2017; Deal et al., Reference Deal, Betz, Yaffe, Harris, Purchase-Helzner and Satterfield2017; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Yaffe, Xia, Xue, Harris and Purchase-Helzner2013) or long-term care residents (Yamada et al., Reference Yamada, Vlachova, Richter, Finne-Soveri, Gindin and van der Roest2014), with very few focused on home care recipients (Vengnes Grue et al., Reference Vengnes Grue, Hylen Ranhoff, Noro, Finne-Soveri, Birna Jensdottir and Ljunggren2009).

Home care is an important sector to study for several reasons. Roughly 2,000,000 Canadians receive home care annually, of whom approximately 40 per cent are 65 years of age and older (Sinha & Bleakney, Reference Sinha and Bleakney2014). The federal, provincial, and territorial governments recently endorsed A Common Statement of Principles on Shared Health Priorities, accompanied by an $11 billion federal investment to improve Canadians’ access to home and community care (Government of Canada, 2019). Evidence to better understand this sector is important, because the majority of older adults prefer to “age in place” and remain in their own homes for as long as possible (Wiles, Leibing, Guberman, Reeve, & Allen, Reference Wiles, Leibing, Guberman, Reeve and Allen2012). Finally, older persons receiving home care are typically more impaired in their cognitive and physical functioning than community-dwelling older adults. The prevalence of cognitive difficulties is approximately 20–30 per cent (de Almeida Mello et al., Reference de Almeida Mello, Ces, Vanneste, Van Durme, Van Audenhove and Macq2020; Garms-Homolova et al., Reference Garms-Homolova, Notthoff, Declercq, van der Roest, Onder and Jonsson2017; Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018) in home care and roughly 3 per cent among physically healthy, community-dwelling older persons (Mery, Wodchis, & Laporte, Reference Mery, Wodchis and Laporte2016). The same is true for functional abilities, with prevalence of ADL impairment as high as 32 per cent in home care (Brown, McAvay, Raue, Moses, & Bruce, Reference Brown, McAvay, Raue, Moses and Bruce2003; Foebel et al., Reference Foebel, van Hout, van der Roest, Topinkova, Garms-Homolova and Frijters2015) versus roughly 10 per cent among those living in the community (Mery et al., Reference Mery, Wodchis and Laporte2016; Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland, & Griffith, 2010–Reference Raina, Wolfson, Kirkland and Griffith2015). This group is important to study because they are under-represented in health services research in Canada.

The present study aimed to address several of the gaps in the current literature. A longitudinal design was utilized to enable the identification of the onset of new sensory impairments. Assessing home care clients over time allowed for an evaluation of risk versus simply understanding cross-sectional relationships. We created multiple unique cohorts to explore how the identification of the onset of new sensory impairments influences the risk of several important outcomes including communication difficulties, a deterioration in cognitive performance, and the risk of caregiver burden. We chose to focus on individuals with a newly identified sensory impairment, because this group may be in an adjustment phase and may not have had time to develop compensatory strategies (e.g., seeking advice and looking into and purchasing assistive devices), whereas people with longer-term impairments may have had time to do so. Developing these compensatory strategies may also be more difficult among home care recipients who are also dealing with multiple co-morbidities.

Based on our previous work and other literature, we anticipated that older adults with a newly identified DSI would show a greater risk for multiple negative outcomes than individuals with VI or HI only. For example, we expected that this group would be more likely to experience communication difficulties, worsening cognitive performance, and caregiver burden, even after adjusting for multiple control variables.

Design and Methods

Data Source

We conducted secondary analyses of data collected using the Resident Assessment Instrument for Home Care (RAI-HC) in Ontario. All data included in this article came from the RAI-HC, and no other supplementary assessments or data were used. The RAI-HC is a standardized assessment that is routinely used to assess clients who are expected to receive at least 60 days of service (Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care, 2007). The assessment has established validity and reliability and includes roughly 300 items to capture domains including, but not limited to communication, sensory impairments, cognitive status, and functional ability (Hirdes et al., Reference Hirdes, Ljunggren, Morris, Frijters, Finne-Soveri and Gray2008). Assessments are completed by trained care coordinators (typically registered nurses) through an interview with the client and their informal caregivers, and in consultation with other health professionals, as needed. Re-assessments are completed every 6–12 months or following a clinically important change in health status. Because the assessment is mandated in Ontario, the software used to capture the completed assessments does not allow the assessor to leave fields blank. Therefore, there are virtually no missing data in this data set. In Ontario, all assessments are shared with the Canadian Institute for Health Information (CIHI). Researchers can apply to the CIHI for a copy of the anonymized data, as was done for the present study. The Research Ethics Board at Wilfrid Laurier University reviewed and approved the design of this study (REB #4184).

Sample

The sample included all home care clients who were 65 years of age and older and who had completed at least two RAI-HC assessments between 2009 and 2014 (n = 106,920). Each pair of assessments that was completed within 60 months was examined to find the first point in time when a sensory impairment was newly identified for a client. The time frame between assessments was chosen in order to maximize the length of follow-up within the data set. For example, if an individual had three assessments, and at their first assessment had no sensory impairment, but were identified as having a new HI at their third assessment (i.e., no new impairment at their second), then their third assessment would be considered T2 and their second assessment would be considered T1. We followed each person forward to the first instance when a new sensory impairment occurred, which was deemed their T2 assessment, whereas the assessment just previous to this one was T1. For those who did not develop a new impairment at any of their assessments, we used their two most recent assessments in the database as T1 and T2.

We defined seven mutually exclusive cohorts of individuals based on their sensory status at baseline (T1) and how this status may have changed by follow-up (T2) (Table 1). The seven cohorts included those with (1) no sensory impairments at either T1 or T2 (n = 47,475), (2) a newly identified HI at T2 (n = 12,771), (3) a newly identified VI at T2 (n = 7,485), (4) “persistent” DSI that was present at both T1 and T2 (n = 22,148), (5) an existing HI and a newly identified VI at T2 (n = 9,087), (6) an existing VI and a newly identified HI at T2 (n = 4,683), and (7) no baseline impairments and a newly identified DSI at T2 (n = 3,271).

Table 1. Summary of the seven different cohorts and their status at baseline and at follow-up

Note. HI = hearing impairment; VI = vision impairment; DSI = dual sensory impairment; SD = standard deviation

Sensory Measures

Care coordinators complete the RAI-HC based on a standardized manual developed by interRAI, the group who holds the copyright for the instrument (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Bernabei, Ikegami, Gilgen, Frijters and Hirdes1999). They determine a client’s hearing and vision status based on a combination of self-assessment, discussions with caregivers, consultations with other care providers, and review of any available medical records, as appropriate. For the measures related to vision and hearing (one item each on the RAI-HC), assessors are instructed to evaluate sensory impairments when the client is using their existing aids or devices (i.e., while using hearing aids or glasses).

The presence of HI was defined as a score of one or higher on a single item on the RAI-HC. This item scores hearing ability from zero (no impairment) to three (highly impaired). Similarly, VI was defined as a score of one or higher on a single item that scores vision from zero (no impairment) to four (severely impaired). Finally, DSI was defined as a score of three or higher on the Deafblind Severity Index (DbSI). The DbSI uses the two items that measure vision and hearing to identify clients with at least minimal losses in both senses (i.e., a score of one or higher on both items) (Dalby et al., Reference Dalby, Hirdes, Stolee, Strong, Poss and Tjam2009). The hearing and vision items have good test–retest reliability (hearing: κ = 0.83; vision: κ = 0.85) (Dalby et al., Reference Dalby, Hirdes, Stolee, Strong, Poss and Tjam2009) and correlate well with gold standard objective measures (Urqueta Alfaro et al., Reference Urqueta Alfaro, Guthrie, Phillips, Pichora-Fuller, Mick and McGraw2019).

Outcome Measures

The RAI-HC includes two items on communication, one to capture expressive and the other to capture receptive communication. The first item, on expressive communication, scores the person’s ability to express themselves orally, using sign language, in writing, or some combination of these techniques. The other item targets the person’s capacity to understand information communicated with the person orally, in writing, through sign language, or in braille. In both cases, the items are scored from zero to four, where a score of one or higher was used to define difficulties with communication. A deterioration in communication was defined as at least a one-point increase on the item over time.

The Caregiver Risk Evaluation (CaRE) algorithm is a decision-support tool that differentiates the risk of caregiver burden among informal caregivers (Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Williams, Beach, Maxwell, Mills and Mitchell2021). The algorithm is created using items from within the RAI-HC and assigns caregivers to one of four unique groups, ranging from low risk (score of zero) to very high risk (score of four) of experiencing burden. Individuals were grouped into two categories, including those with low or moderate risk (scores of 1 and 2; roughly 60% of individuals across the seven cohorts) and compared with those with a high or very high risk of experiencing caregiver burden (scores of 3 and 4; roughly 40%).

The Cognitive Performance Scale (CPS) is a hierarchical scale which includes two items found on traditional cognitive assessments (e.g., short-term memory, daily decision making) and two items reflecting functional status (e.g., expressive communication, independence in eating). The scale ranges from zero to six (0 = no cognitive impairment, 1 = borderline intact, 2 = mild impairment, 3 = moderate impairment, 4 = moderately severe impairment, 5 = severe impairment, and 6 = very severe impairment), has excellent inter-rater reliability (average κ = 0.85), and is correlated with performance on two cognitive screening measures; namely, the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (Jones, Perlman, Hirdes, & Scott, Reference Jones, Perlman, Hirdes and Scott2010) and the Mini Mental State Exam (Gruber-Baldini, Zimmerman, Mortimore, & Magaziner, Reference Gruber-Baldini, Zimmerman, Mortimore and Magaziner2000). It was designed to be a functional measure and to act as a brief screen for impaired cognitive performance.

We focused on four main dependent measures; namely, a deterioration in: (1) cognitive performance (measured with the CPS scale), (2) receptive communication, (3) expressive communication, and (4) the CaRE algorithm. In all cases, a minimum of a one-point change on the item or scale was considered to be a meaningful decline. These four outcomes were chosen because they are highly relevant to the functioning of individuals who are experiencing sensory changes over time. Furthermore, they enabled us to build upon our earlier cross-sectional analyses which showed associations among these outcomes(Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018).

Control Variables

There are five health index scales embedded within the RAI-HC. For all scales, a higher score indicates worse functioning (i.e., greater impairment). The scales have been described in detail previously (Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018) and are summarized.here.

  1. 1. The Activities of Daily Living (ADL) Self-Performance Hierarchy Scale (ADL-H) includes items on bathing and dressing with scores ranging from a score of zero (independent) to six (total dependence) (Morris, Fries, & Morris, Reference Morris, Fries and Morris1999). A score of two or higher, representing the point at which an individual can no longer complete all of their ADLs independently, was used to define moderate/severe ADL impairment, in line with previous research (Davidson & Guthrie, Reference Davidson and Guthrie2017; Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018; Williams, Jamal, & Guthrie, Reference Williams, Jamal and Guthrie2018).

  2. 2. The Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) Involvement Scale has scores ranging from 0 to 21 and includes items on telephone use, managing medications, and meal preparation. A cut-point of 14 or higher was used to indicate at least moderate impairment in performing these tasks, in line with previous studies (Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018; Williams, Jamal, & Guthrie, Reference Williams, Jamal and Guthrie2018).

  3. 3. The Depression Rating Scale (DRS) is a summative scale with scores ranging from 0 to 14. A score of three or higher, the cut-point we have chosen, is predictive of a clinical diagnosis of depression (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Poss, Hirdes, Jones, Stones and Fries2008).

  4. 4. The Pain Scale includes two items which capture the frequency and intensity of pain. The scale ranges from zero to four and has been validated against the vertical version of the Visual Analog Scale (VAS). A score of two or higher was used to indicate severe/daily pain, because research has shown an important increase in the VAS score among those with a score of two or higher (Fries, Simon, Morris, Flodstrom, & Bookstein, Reference Fries, Simon, Morris, Flodstrom and Bookstein2001).

  5. 5. The Changes in Health, End-Stage Disease and Signs and Symptoms (CHESS) Scale is scored from zero to five and is a measure of health instability. For every one-point increase on the scale, there is a nearly two-fold increased risk of mortality. A cut-point of two or higher was used to determine health instability based on previous research showing a marked increase in the hazard ratio for mortality among those scoring two or higher compared with those scoring less than two (Hirdes, Poss, Mitchell, Korngut, & Heckman, Reference Hirdes, Poss, Mitchell, Korngut and Heckman2014).

Several other dichotomous variables (measured as yes/no) were examined, including self-reported loneliness, and items related to diagnoses (Alzheimer’s dementia/another type of dementia, stroke, diabetes, hypertension, coronary artery disease, congestive heart failure, Parkinson’s disease, irregularly irregular pulse, peripheral vascular disease, cataracts, and glaucoma). A co-morbidity count was developed (having at least one diagnosis vs. none) from the preceding list.

Univariate Analysis

The preliminary descriptive analysis examined the absolute percent change between T1 and T2 across multiple outcomes and across the seven cohorts. We focused on change over time, as we have previously explored similar outcomes, but using cross-sectional data (Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Davidson, Williams, Campos, Hunter and Mick2018). Given the large sample size and potential for type I error, we used an absolute standardized difference (stdiff, similar to a z-score) of 0.2 or higher to identify statistically meaningful differences between individuals with and those without impairments (Yang & Dalton, Reference Yang and Dalton2012). Standardized differences are one metric by which to understand the effect size when comparing two proportions (Azuero, Reference Azuero2016). Our chosen cut-point identifies a difference representing at least a small effect size. The standardized difference is defined as the difference in two proportions divided by an estimate of the prevalence of the covariate in each of the two groups (Austin, Reference Austin2009). Given that we considered the raw baseline data (T1) to be supplementary to our main question, we did not calculate any standardized differences to compare the seven cohorts at T1.

For each of the four outcomes (described previously), the analysis was then stratified by both the type of sensory change (i.e., the seven unique cohorts), as well as by age (5-year increments). We did a similar analysis, stratified by sensory cohort and by sex, which failed to show any significant differences (data not shown); therefore, all results presented here are collapsed across sex.

Multivariate Analysis

Logistic regression was used to examine the association between the identification of a new impairment (i.e., new HI-only, new VI-only, or a new DSI) across the four different outcomes of interest. A total of four multivariate models were constructed, in which each model represented at least a one-point decline (i.e., worsening) in the outcome of interest between T1 and T2: (1) a decline in receptive communication, (2) a decline in expressive communication, (3) a decline on the CaRE algorithm, and (4) a decline on the CPS score. The reference group for each outcome was the absence of the event (e.g., for a decline on the CPS score, the reference group represented individuals who did not experience a decline in their score between T1 and T2). Each model was adjusted for the following control variables: age (continuous variable), sex (male/female), marital status (married/never married/widowed/separated or divorced), level of education (post-secondary/less than high school/high school), a score of three or higher on the DRS, a co-morbidity count of one or higher, and a score of one or higher on the CPS scale (only applicable for a decline in receptive communication outcome). Because we were interested in examining the association between a new impairment across the four outcomes, all of the control variables were based on data from T1, whereas the outcomes of interest were the change in scores (decline vs. no decline) between T1 and T2. All analyses were completed using SAS Enterprise Guide, version 7.1, and the study followed the STrengthening the Reporting of OBservational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guidelines (von Elm et al., Reference von Elm, Egger, Altman, Pocock, Gotzsche and Vandenbroucke2007).

Results

In this group of home care clients, 33.6% (n = 35,918) had a sensory impairment at T1 and an additional 34.9% (n = 37,297) were identified as having the onset of a new sensory impairment between T1 and T2. A small proportion (3.1%) were identified as having developed a new impairment in both their vision and hearing. The average time between T1 and T2 was very similar across the cohorts, ranging from a mean 9.7 months (cohort 5: existing HI/new VI) to 12.8 months (cohort 7: new DSI; Table 1).

At T1, the cohorts identified as having sensory challenges tended to be at least 85 years of age. The proportion in this age group varied by cohort, ranging from 26.7 per cent (new VI) to 64.1 per cent (persistent DSI), compared with 27 per cent in those without any sensory impairments. At least 60 per cent in each of the seven cohorts were female, with little variation across these groups (ranging from 61.7% to 71.1%). Between 33.5 per cent (persistent DSI) and 43.2 per cent (no impairments) were married. Based on the proportions alone (i.e., without any statistical testing), clients with existing sensory impairments, and in particular the persistent DSI cohort, were relatively more likely to experience impaired ADLs and IADLs, to experience health instability, to have difficulties in both receptive and expressive communication, and to experience some level of difficulty with cognitive performance, measured with the CPS scale (Table 2). We opted not to conduct any formal testing of statistical significance between these proportions, because these analyses were not meant to address our primary research question, which focused on change between T1 and T2.

Table 2. Comparison of demographic and clinical characteristics across the seven cohorts at baseline (T1)

Note. HI = hearing impairment; VI = vision impairment; DSI = dual sensory impairment.

In examining change over time, a clear pattern emerged whereby the new DSI cohort had the highest proportion of individuals who showed a deterioration over time across multiple outcomes. For this reason, we calculated the standardized difference comparing the new DSI cohort and those with no sensory impairments, to highlight which of these differences were statistically meaningful. Those in the new DSI cohort were significantly more likely to experience a deterioration over time in terms of receptive communication (stdiff = 0.68), expressive communication (stdiff = 0.61), cognitive performance (stdiff = 0.49), ADLs (stdiff = 0.36), IADLs (stdiff = 0.39), and symptoms of depression (stdiff = 0.36). Those with a newly identified DSI were also more likely to have caregivers who, over time, were in the highest risk groups for caregiver burden (stdiff = 0.38). Across the nine disease categories, only two met our threshold for statistical significance; namely, the presence of Alzheimer’s dementia/another type of dementia (stdiff = 0.25) and cataracts (stdiff = 0.26; Table 3).

Table 3. Percent of participants who experienced a change between baseline (T1) and onset of a new impairment (T2) across the seven cohortsa

Note. Negative values in the table indicate a decrease in the proportion in that group over time.

a Standardized difference between Cohort 1 and Cohort 7 of at least 0.2 (absolute value).

HI = hearing impairment; VI = vision impairment; DSI = dual sensory impairment.

Across all seven cohorts, the prevalence of experiencing a deterioration on the CPS score was nearly 20 per cent, regardless of age, and the rate tended to increase with age within each cohort. When assessing each individual cohort, very little difference was seen with age. The exception to this was the cohort with newly identified DSI. Within this cohort, the percent experiencing a deterioration on the CPS score ranged from 41.7 per cent in the youngest group (65–69 years of age) to 64.0% in the oldest age group (≥ 95 years of age; Figure 1).

Figure 1. Individuals who experienced any deterioration on the Cognitive Performance Scale (CPS) (any one-point increase) between time 1 and time 2

A very similar pattern was observed for both expressive and receptive communication. In both types, the newly identified DSI cohort had the highest prevalence, of declines in communication, in each of the age groups. There was an absolute difference, between proportions, of roughly 10 per cent when comparing the youngest with the oldest age groups. Across most age groups, the rates were next highest in the existing HI/new VI cohort. This cohort also showed the largest difference across the age groups at 13.8 per cent for deterioration in expressive communication, and a difference of 15.1 per cent for deterioration in receptive communication. In this cohort, there was an increase in the proportion across each age cohort (Figures 2 and 3). A newly identified VI in those with an existing HI exhibited roughly the same effect on communication as a newly identified DSI.

Figure 2. Comparison of the clients with and without sensory impairments who experienced a deterioration in expressive communication

Figure 3. Comparison of clients with and without sensory impairments who experienced a deterioration in their receptive communication

The final outcome, stratified by age, was deterioration on the CaRE algorithm. The results were unremarkable except for the newly identified DSI cohort. This cohort had the highest risk of caregiver burden in all but one of the age groups. Like the other outcomes we studied, those with newly identified DSI showed the most difference when comparing the two extreme age groups, with a difference of 8.5 per cent (65–69 years of age: 16.7% vs. ≥ 95 years of age: 25.2%; Figure 4).

Figure 4. Comparison of clients with and without sensory impairments with a caregiver who experienced a deterioration on the Caregiver Risk Evaluation (CaRE) algorithm

Given that there was an influence of age across several of these outcomes and given the known association among age, cognitive status, and sensory functioning, we explored these outcomes using multivariate regression. After adjusting for multiple control variables, individuals with a newly identified DSI (compared with those without DSI) were significantly more likely to experience each outcome. This was particularly true for both types of communication in which the newly identified DSI cohort had at least a four-fold increase in the odds of experiencing a decline in expressive communication (odds ratio [OR] = 4.13; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 3.69, 4.62) as well as a decline in receptive communication (OR = 5.22; CI: 4.63, 5.87). The newly identified HI only cohort also had a significant increase in the odds of having communication difficulties, with a two-fold increase for a decline in receptive communication (OR = 2.61; CI: 2.43, 2.80), compared with those without a new HI. The odds were lower, but still significant, for difficulties with expressive communication (OR = 2.00; CI: 1.86, 2.15). Similar, but lower odds were also observed for the newly identified VI cohort for both a decline in receptive (OR = 1.53; CI: 1.39, 1.68) and expressive communication (OR = 1.52; CI: 1.39, 1.67). The newly identified DSI cohort also had a roughly 80 per cent increase in the risk of caregiver burden (OR = 1.81; CI: 1.60, 2.06), compared with roughly 40 per cent in the newly identified HI cohort (OR = 1.43; CI: 1.32, 1.53) and newly identified VI cohorts (OR = 1.38; CI: 1.26, 1.51). Finally, the newly identified DSI cohort was approximately three times as likely to experience a worsening on the CPS score (OR = 3.11; CI: 2.80, 3.46), and again, the odds ratios were lower in the two other single-impairment cohorts (Table 4).

Table 4. Logistic regression models for the association between newly acquired sensory impairments and the odds of the event across four unique outcomes

Note. All models were adjusted for age, sex, level of education, marital status, Depression Rating Scale (DRS) score of ≥ 3, co-morbidity (sum of disease diagnoses listed in Table 1) and Cognitive Performance Scale (CPS) score (only for decline in receptive communication outcome).

OR = odds ratio comparing those with the impairment to those without it; CI = confidence interval

Overall, the newly identified DSI cohort stands out as being more likely to experience multiple negative outcomes related to declines in communication, worsening on the CPS score, and an increased risk of caregiver burden, even after adjusting for many other covariates that may be potential confounders in these associations.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to describe how a new onset of sensory impairments may influence the risk of negative outcomes such as communication difficulties, changes in cognitive performance, and caregiver burden in a group of older home care clients. In this sample of roughly 100,000 older clients who were receiving home care, 35 per cent were newly identified as having a sensory impairment between the baseline assessment and a re-assessment. The average time frame between the two assessments was approximately 12 months. This is not a trivial proportion of older adults for whom sensory challenges emerge during the delivery of home care. To our knowledge, this is one of the first Canadian studies to explore the influence of single and dual sensory impairments on multiple health-related outcomes over time. The fact that these impairments took roughly a year to be identified implies that there is a window of opportunity within which to assess the person for changes in sensory functioning and to implement strategies and provide rehabilitative services to maximize their ability to function using their residual vision and hearing. This window of opportunity also applies to family caregivers. These caregivers also require access to supportive services and strategies to maintain their own well-being while helping their loved one with the emerging sensory impairments that could exacerbate many aspects of that person’s health and wellness.

At baseline, clients with sensory impairments were more likely to experience functional impairment, health instability, to have difficulties in communication, and to experience cognitive challenges. This is in line with multiple longitudinal studies supporting HI as an important risk factor for the onset of cognitive impairment (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Cadar, Herbert, Orrell and Steptoe2017; Deal et al., Reference Deal, Betz, Yaffe, Harris, Purchase-Helzner and Satterfield2017; Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Cruickshanks, Schubert, Pinto, Carlsson and Klein2016; Fritze et al., Reference Fritze, Teipel, Ovari, Kilimann, Witt and Doblhammer2016; Gurgel et al., Reference Gurgel, Ward, Schwartz, Norton, Foster and Tschanz2014; Lin et al., Reference Lin, Metter, O’Brien, Resnick, Zonderman and Ferrucci2011, Reference Lin, Yaffe, Xia, Xue, Harris and Purchase-Helzner2013) or a deterioration in cognitive performance (Alattar et al., Reference Alattar, Bergstrom, Laughlin, Kritz-Silverstein, Richard and Reas2020; Amieva et al., Reference Amieva, Ouvrard, Giulioli, Meillon, Rullier and Dartigues2015). A smaller number of studies likewise support VI as a risk factor for dementia or cognitive decline (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Cauley, Lui, Fink, McCulloch and Stone2007; Reyes‐Ortiz et al., Reference Reyes‐Ortiz, Kuo, DiNuzzo, Ray, Raji and Markides2005; Rogers & Langa, Reference Rogers and Langa2010; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Swenor, Christ, West, Lam and Lee2018). A limited number of studies have been published that examine DSI as a factor for cognitive decline, and all reported a positive association (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Gutierrez, Stone, Yaffe, Ensrud and Fink2004; Liu, Cohen, Fillenbaum, Burchett, & Whitson, Reference Liu, Cohen, Fillenbaum, Burchett and Whitson2016; Maharani et al., Reference Maharani, Dawes, Nazroo, Tampubolon and Pendleton2018; Yamada et al., Reference Yamada, Denkinger, Onder, Henrard, van der Roest and Finne-Soveri2015).

Regardless of the sensory cohort, and irrespective of age, roughly 20 per cent of the home care clients in the present study had a worsening in their CPS score over time. The actual change in the proportion showing this deterioration (i.e., comparing the proportion at T1 with the proportion at T2) was relatively flat, except in the newly identified DSI cohort. Although this cohort represents a relatively small proportion of the sample (3%), it represents a sample of just over 3,200 unique individuals. To our knowledge, this is larger than in most recent publications, which tend to focus on more clinical and rehabilitation populations (Roets-Merken, Zuidema, Vernooij-Dassen, & Kempen, Reference Roets-Merken, Zuidema, Vernooij-Dassen and Kempen2014; Roets-Merken et al., Reference Roets-Merken, Zuidema, Vernooij-Dassen, Teerenstra, Hermsen and Kempen2017; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, McMahon, Gopinath, Kifley, Barton and Mitchell2014; Shakarchi et al., Reference Shakarchi, Assi, Ehrlich, Deal, Reed and Swenor2020; Urqueta Alfaro, Guthrie, McGraw, & Wittich, Reference Urqueta Alfaro, Guthrie, McGraw and Wittich2020).

Those in the new DSI cohort represent a uniquely impaired group of older adults. More so than any other cohort, they were significantly more likely to experience a deterioration in multiple areas, including communication, cognitive performance, functional abilities, and symptoms of depression. Even after adjusting for multiple control variables, this cohort had an increased risk across all four of the key outcomes measured, including the risk for caregiver burden. These results are in line with previous research showing individuals with DSI to be at increased risk of functional impairment (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Bennett and Wilson2008), depression (Capella-McDonnall, Reference Capella-McDonnall2009; Fletcher & Guthrie, Reference Fletcher and Guthrie2013; Guthrie et al., Reference Guthrie, Theriault and Davidson2015; Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Gopinath, McMahon, Leeder, Mitchell and Wang2011) and communication difficulties (McDonnall et al., Reference McDonnall, Crudden, LeJeune, Steverson and O’Donnell2016). However, what we cannot tell from our data is exactly when the DSI was first evident, because we only know that it was identified within a specific time frame between two assessments. This is a limitation to our work, because we cannot differentiate individuals who had DSI caused by a single event (e.g., a stroke) from those for whom the onset was more gradual.

The consequences of newly identified sensory changes on caregiver burden is particularly noteworthy given the evidence that the degree of vision loss has been shown to be positively correlated with the caregivers’ perceived level of burden (Braich, Lal, Hollands, & Almeida, Reference Braich, Lal, Hollands and Almeida2012; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Braich, Rahim, Rayat, Xing and Iqbal2016). Three other studies also found a relationship between more severe vision loss and the risk of depression in the caregiver (Bambara et al., Reference Bambara, Owsley, Wadley, Martin, Porter and Dreer2009; Braich et al., Reference Braich, Lal, Hollands and Almeida2012, Reference Braich, Jackson, Knohl, Bhoiwala, Gandham and Almeida2016). A single Canadian study, the only one we found looking at hearing loss, reported that higher caregiver burden was experienced by caregivers who cared for someone without HI than by caregivers of someone with difficulties in their hearing (Westaway, Wittich, & Overbury, Reference Westaway, Wittich and Overbury2010). The authors suggest that this finding may reflect the fact that all participants came from a rehabilitation centre, and participants with a hearing loss were all receiving rehabilitation services. The receipt of these services would likely have benefited both the individuals and their caregivers, thus reducing the level of stress or burden experienced by these caregivers. We did not find any research looking at DSI and caregiver well-being, one of the many areas in need of further research (Wittich, Jarry, Groulx, Southall, & Gagné, Reference Wittich, Jarry, Groulx, Southall and Gagné2016).

This work has several limitations. The RAI-HC is based on subjective evaluations of vision, hearing and cognition. The assessment can draw on behavioral test results if they are included in the medical chart. The RAI-HC also captures information about functioning in these domains; however, it cannot quantify these impairments objectively. The assessment does not capture the use of assistive devices (e.g., glasses or hearing aids) or the history of rehabilitation services that have been accessed, thereby limiting our ability to understand how sensory rehabilitation and accommodation for sensory disabilities might influence the rate of change in cognition or communication. In addition, we cannot determine the temporal sequence between the onset of cognitive challenges and the onset of sensory impairments. It is possible that some individuals with a new sensory challenge had a change in their cognitive functioning that preceded the changes in sensory functioning. Given that there was a year, on average, between the initial assessment and the identification of the sensory impairment, changes in cognition may have taken place during this time, which could have influenced some of our study outcomes. We attempted to adjust for this in our multivariate models, but there remains the possibility of residual confounding based on other unmeasured factors.

Despite these limitations, a standardized assessment, like the RAI-HC, is vital to create real-time information with which clinicians can develop a person-centred care plan and respond quickly to ongoing changes for the person and their family. Approximately one third of home care clients in Ontario are likely to be identified as having new sensory challenges over time, with an associated deterioration in their communication and cognitive performance. Although our results focus on data from Ontario, multiple other provinces (e.g., Alberta, British Columbia, Nova Scotia, Manitoba) also use the RAI-HC, making our findings relevant to these other regions as well.

Clinicians within this sector are clearly working with a complex group of individuals who have multiple health conditions and unique needs. Hearing impairment is an important risk factor in the development of cognitive challenges (Lin & Albert, Reference Lin and Albert2014). Although screening for hearing and vision impairment is important, it is recognized that effective sensory interventions (e.g., assistive technologies, vision/hearing rehabilitation) and contact with health care professionals are often under-utilized strategies (Davis et al., Reference Davis, McMahon, Pichora-Fuller, Russ, Lin and Olusanya2016; Klaver, Wolfs, Vingerling, Hofman, & de Jong, Reference Klaver, Wolfs, Vingerling, Hofman and de Jong1998; Lin, Thorpe, Gordon-Salant, & Ferrucci, Reference Lin, Thorpe, Gordon-Salant and Ferrucci2011; Overbury & Wittich, Reference Overbury and Wittich2011). This may be partly explained by a lack of knowledge surrounding screening among those with cognitive challenges (McGilton et al., Reference McGilton, Hobler, Campos, Dupuis, Labreche and Guthrie2016). Consequently, there is an ongoing requirement for better training and education for home care clinicians to be able to effectively screen for these impairments and put in place a plan of care and/or referrals to maximize the person’s functional abilities and use of their residual vision and hearing. Earlier identification and care planning to accommodate sensory impairments would enable these clients to maximize their independence and quality of life.

Funding

This work was supported by the Canadian Consortium on Neurodegeneration in Aging (CCNA) (http://ccna-ccnv.ca/en) through funding to all authors. The CCNA is supported by a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) (http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/193.html) with funding from several partners including the Alzheimer Society of Canada, Alberta Innovates, Brain Canada Foundation, and Canadian Nurses Foundation. W.W. is supported by a Junior 1 Career Grant from the FRQ-S (# 28881 & 30620; http://www.frqs.gouv.qc.ca/en/).

Footnotes

*

All authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Alattar, A. A., Bergstrom, J., Laughlin, G. A., Kritz-Silverstein, D., Richard, E. L., Reas, E. T., et al. (2020). Hearing impairment and cognitive decline in older, community-dwelling adults. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 75(3), 567573. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glz035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amieva, H., Ouvrard, C., Giulioli, C., Meillon, C., Rullier, L., & Dartigues, J. F. (2015). Self-reported hearing loss, hearing aids, and cognitive decline in elderly adults: A 25-year study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 63(10), 20992104. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.13649CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, P. C. (2009). Using the standardized difference to compare the prevalence of a binary variable between two groups in observational research. Communications in Statistics - Simulation and Computation, 38(6), 12281234. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610910902859574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azuero, A. (2016). A note on the magnitude of hazard ratios. Cancer, 122(8), 12981299. https://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.29924Google Scholar
Bambara, J. K., Owsley, C., Wadley, V., Martin, R., Porter, C., & Dreer, L. E. (2009). Family caregiver social problem-solving abilities and adjustment to caring for a relative with vision loss. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 50(4), 15851592. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2744Google Scholar
Barnes, D. E., Cauley, J. A., Lui, L. Y., Fink, H. A., McCulloch, C., Stone, K. L., et al. (2007). Women who maintain optimal cognitive function into old age. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 55(2), 259264. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01040.xGoogle Scholar
Braich, P. S., Jackson, M., Knohl, S. J., Bhoiwala, D., Gandham, S. B., & Almeida, D. (2016). Burden and depression in caregivers of blind patients in New York State. Ophthalmic Epidemiology, 23(3), 162170. https://doi.org/10.3109/09286586.2015.1099684CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braich, P. S., Lal, V., Hollands, S., & Almeida, D. R. (2012). Burden and depression in the caregivers of blind patients in India. Ophthalmology, 119(2), 221226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2011.07.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, E. L., McAvay, G., Raue, P. J., Moses, S., & Bruce, M. L. (2003). Recognition of depression among elderly recipients of home care services. Psychiatric Services, 54(2), 208213.Google Scholar
Campos, J., Ramkhalawansingh, R., & Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2018). Hearing, self-motion perception, mobility, and aging. Hearing Research, 369, 4255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2018.03.025Google Scholar
Capella-McDonnall, M. (2009). The effects of developing a dual sensory loss on depression in older adults: A longitudinal study. Journal of Aging and Health, 21(8), 11791199. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264309350077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, D. S., Betz, J., Yaffe, K., Ayonayon, H. N., Kritchevsky, S., Martin, K. R., et al. (2015). Association of hearing impairment with declines in physical functioning and the risk of disability in older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 70(5), 654661. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glu207Google ScholarPubMed
Choi, J. S., Betz, J., Deal, J., Contrera, K. J., Genther, D. J., Chen, D. S., et al. (2015). A comparison of self-report and audiometric measures of hearing and their associations with functional outcomes in older adults. Journal of Aging and Health, 28(5), 890910. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264315614006Google Scholar
Dalby, D., Hirdes, J. P., Stolee, P., Strong, J. G., Poss, J., Tjam, E. Y., et al. (2009). Development and psychometric properties of a standardized assessment for adults who are deaf-blind. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 103(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145482X0910300103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, J. G. S., & Guthrie, D. M. (2017). Older adults with a combination of vision and hearing impairment experience higher rates of cognitive impairment, functional dependence, and worse outcomes across a set of quality indicators. Journal of Aging and Health, 31(1), 85108. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264317723407Google Scholar
Davies, H. R., Cadar, D., Herbert, A., Orrell, M., & Steptoe, A. (2017). Hearing impairment and incident dementia: Findings from the English longitudinal study of ageing. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 65(9), 20742081. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.14986Google Scholar
Davis, A., McMahon, C. M., Pichora-Fuller, K. M., Russ, S., Lin, F., Olusanya, B. O., et al. (2016). Aging and hearing health: The life-course approach. Gerontologist, 56(Suppl 2), S256267. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnw033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Almeida Mello, J., Ces, S., Vanneste, D., Van Durme, T., Van Audenhove, C., Macq, J., et al. (2020). Comparing the case-mix of frail older people at home and of those being admitted into residential care: A longitudinal study. BMC Geriatrics, 20(1), 195. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-020-01593-wGoogle Scholar
Deal, J. A., Betz, J., Yaffe, K., Harris, T., Purchase-Helzner, E., Satterfield, S., et al. (2017). Hearing impairment and incident dementia and cognitive decline in older adults: The Health ABC Study. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 72(5), 703709. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glw069Google Scholar
Feder, K., Michaud, D., Ramage-Morin, P., McNamee, J., & Beauregard, Y. (2015). Prevalence of hearing loss among Canadians aged 20 to 79: Audiometric results from the 2012/2013 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Reports, 26(7), 1825.Google Scholar
Fischer, M. E., Cruickshanks, K. J., Schubert, C. R., Pinto, A. A., Carlsson, C. M., Klein, B. E., et al. (2016). Age-related sensory impairments and risk of cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 64(10), 19811987. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.14308CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P. C., & Guthrie, D. M. (2013). The lived experiences of individuals with acquired deafblindness: Challenges and the future. International Journal of Disability, Community and Rehabilitation, 12, 111.Google Scholar
Foebel, A. D., van Hout, H. P., van der Roest, H. G., Topinkova, E., Garms-Homolova, V., Frijters, D., et al. (2015). Quality of care in European home care programs using the second generation interRAI Home Care Quality Indicators (HCQIs). BMC Geriatrics, 15, 148. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-015-0146-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, B. E., Simon, S. E., Morris, J. N., Flodstrom, C., & Bookstein, F. L. (2001). Pain in US nursing homes: Validating a pain scale for the minimum data set. The Gerontologist, 41(2), 173179. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/41.2.173Google Scholar
Fritze, T., Teipel, S., Ovari, A., Kilimann, I., Witt, G., & Doblhammer, G. (2016). Hearing impairment affects dementia incidence. An analysis based on longitudinal health claims data in Germany. PLoS One, 11(7), e0156876. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0156876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garms-Homolova, V., Notthoff, N., Declercq, A., van der Roest, H. G., Onder, G., Jonsson, P., et al. (2017). Social and functional health of home care clients with different levels of cognitive impairments. Aging and Mental Health, 21(1), 1823. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2016.1247426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Government of Canada. (2019). Shared health priorities. Ottawa, ON: Author. Retrieved 15 October 2020 from https://www.canada.ca/en/health-canada/corporate/transparency/health-agreements/shared-health-priorities.html.Google Scholar
Gruber-Baldini, A. L., Zimmerman, S. I., Mortimore, A. M., & Magaziner, J. (2000). The validity of the minimum data set in measuring the cognitive impairment of persons admitted to nursing homes. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48(12), 16011606. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb03870.xGoogle Scholar
Grue, E. V., Finne-Soveri, H., Stolee, P., Poss, J., Sorbye, L. W., Noro, A., et al. (2009). Recent visual decline – A health hazard with consequences for social life: A study of home care clients in 12 countries. Current Gerontology and Geriatrics Research, 2010(8), 19. https://doi.org/10.1155/2010/503817CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurgel, R. K., Ward, P. D., Schwartz, S., Norton, M. C., Foster, N. L., & Tschanz, J. T. (2014). Relationship of hearing loss and dementia: A prospective, population-based study. Otology & Neurotology, 35(5), 775781. https://doi.org/10.1097/MAO.0000000000000313Google Scholar
Guthrie, D. M., Davidson, J. G. S., Williams, N., Campos, J., Hunter, K., Mick, P., et al. (2018). Combined impairments in vision, hearing and cognition are associated with greater levels of functional and communication difficulties than cognitive impairment alone: Analysis of interRAI data for home care and long-term care recipients in Ontario. PLoS One, 13(2), e0192971. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0192971CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, D. M., Theriault, E., & Davidson, J. G. S. (2015). Self-rated health, cognition, and dual sensory impairment are important predictors of depression among home care clients in Ontario. Home Health Care Management & Practice, 28(1), 35–34.Google Scholar
Guthrie, D. M., Williams, N., Beach, C., Maxwell, C. J., Mills, D., Mitchell, L., et al. (2021). Development and validation of caregiver risk evaluation (CaRE): A new algorithm to screen for caregiver burden. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 40(7), 731741. https://doi.org/10.1177/073346482092010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, C., & Browning, C. (2015). Dual sensory loss in older adults: A systematic review. Gerontologist, 55(5), 913928. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnv074CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirdes, J. P., Ljunggren, G., Morris, J. N., Frijters, D. H., Finne-Soveri, H., Gray, L., et al. (2008). Reliability of the interRAI suite of assessment instruments: A 12-country study of an integrated health information system. BMC Health Services Research, 8, 277. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6963-8-277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirdes, J. P., Poss, J. W., Mitchell, L., Korngut, L., & Heckman, G. (2014). Use of the interRAI CHESS scale to predict mortality among persons with neurological conditions in three care settings. PLoS One, 9(6), e99066. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099066Google Scholar
Jiam, N. T., Li, C., & Agrawal, Y. (2016). Hearing loss and falls: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Laryngoscope, 126(11), 25872596. https://doi.org/10.1002/lary.25927Google Scholar
Jones, K., Perlman, C. M., Hirdes, J., & Scott, T. (2010). Screening cognitive performance with the resident assessment instrument for mental health cognitive performance scale. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 55(11), 736740. https://doi.org/10.1177/070674371005501108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, Z., Braich, P. S., Rahim, K., Rayat, J. S., Xing, L., Iqbal, M., et al. (2016). Burden and depression among caregivers of visually impaired patients in a Canadian population. Advances in Medicine, 2016, 4683427. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/4683427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaver, C., Wolfs, R., Vingerling, J., Hofman, A., & de Jong, P. (1998). Age-specific prevalence and causes of blindness and visual impairment in an older population. Archives of Ophthalmology, 116(5), 653658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laliberte Rudman, D., Gold, D., McGrath, C., Zuvela, B., Spafford, M. M., & Renwick, R. (2016). “Why would I want to go out?”: Age-related vision loss and social participation. Canadian Journal on Aging, 35(4), 465478. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980816000490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, F. R., & Albert, M. (2014). Hearing loss and dementia – Who is listening? Aging and Mental Health, 18(6), 671673. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2014.915924CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, F. R., & Ferrucci, L. (2012). Hearing loss and falls among older adults in the United States. Archives of Internal Medicine, 172(4), 369371. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinternmed.2011.728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, F. R., Metter, E. J., O’Brien, R. J., Resnick, S. M., Zonderman, A. B., & Ferrucci, L. (2011). Hearing loss and incident dementia. Archives of Neurology, 68(2), 214220. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurol.2010.362Google Scholar
Lin, F. R., Thorpe, R., Gordon-Salant, S., & Ferrucci, L. (2011). Hearing loss prevalence and risk factors among older adults in the United States. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 66A(5), 582590.Google Scholar
Lin, F. R., Yaffe, K., Xia, J., Xue, Q., Harris, T. B., Purchase-Helzner, E., et al. (2013). Hearing loss and cognitive decline among older adults. JAMA Internal Medicine, 173(4), 114. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868Google Scholar
Lin, M. Y., Gutierrez, P. R., Stone, K. L., Yaffe, K., Ensrud, K. E., Fink, H. A. et al. (2004). Vision impairment and combined vision and hearing impairment predict cognitive and functional decline in older women. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52(12), 19962002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, P. L., Cohen, H. J., Fillenbaum, G. G., Burchett, B. M., & Whitson, H. E. (2016). Association of co-existing impairments in cognition and self-rated vision and hearing with health outcomes in older adults. Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, 2, 19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2333721415623495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maharani, A., Dawes, P., Nazroo, J., Tampubolon, G., Pendleton, N., & Sense-Cog WPI Group. (2018). Visual and hearing impairments are associated with cognitive decline in older people. Age Ageing, 47, 575581. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afy061Google Scholar
Martin, L., Poss, J. W., Hirdes, J. P., Jones, R. N., Stones, M. J., & Fries, B. E. (2008). Predictors of a new depression diagnosis among older adults admitted to complex continuing care: Implications for the Depression Rating Scale (DRS). Age and Ageing, 37(1), 5156. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afm162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathers, C. D., & Loncar, D. (2006). Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002–2030. PLoS Medicine, 3(11), 20112030. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0030442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonnall, M. C., Crudden, A., LeJeune, B. J., Steverson, A., & O’Donnell, N. (2016). Needs and challenges of seniors with combined hearing and vision loss. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 110, 399411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGilton, K. S., Hobler, F., Campos, J., Dupuis, K., Labreche, T., Guthrie, D. M., et al. (2016). Hearing and vision screening tools for long-term care residents with dementia: Protocol for a scoping review. British Medical Journal, 6(7), 19. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011945Google Scholar
Mery, G., Wodchis, W. P., & Laporte, A. (2016). The determinants of the propensity to receive publicly funded home care services for the elderly in Canada: A panel two-stage residual inclusion approach. Health Economics Review, 6(1), 8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13561-016-0086-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mick, P., Hämäläinen, A., Kolisang, L., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Phillips, N., Guthrie, D. M., et al. (2020). The prevalence of hearing and vision loss in older Canadians: An analysis of data from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Canadian Journal on Aging, 40, 122. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980820000070Google Scholar
Mick, P., Parfyonov, M., Wittich, W., Phillips, N., & Pichora-Fuller, K. M. (2018). Associations between sensory loss and social networks, participation support and loneliness. Analysis of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Canadian Family Physician, 64(1), e33e41. https://www.cfp.ca/content/64/1/e33.longGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. N., Bernabei, R., Ikegami, N., Gilgen, R., Frijters, D., Hirdes, J. P., et al. (1999). RAI-Home Care (RAI-HC) assessment manual for version 2.0. Washington, DC: interRAI Corporation.Google Scholar
Morris, J. N., Fries, B. E., & Morris, S. A. (1999). Scaling ADLs within the MDS. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 54A(11), M546M553. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/54.11.m546Google Scholar
Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care. (2007). Community care access centres: Client services policy manual. Ottawa, ON: Author. Retrieved 4 May 2020 from http://www.health.gov.on.ca/english/providers/pub/manuals/ccac/ccac_mn.html.Google Scholar
Overbury, O., & Wittich, W. (2011). Barriers to low vision rehabilitation: The Montreal Barriers Study. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 52(12), 89338938. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.11-8116Google Scholar
Raina, P., Wolfson, C., Kirkland, S., & Griffith, L. (2010–2015). The Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA) report on health and aging in Canada. Hamilton, ON: The Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging.Google Scholar
Reuben, D. B., Mui, S., Damesyn, M., Moore, A. A., & Greendale, G. A. (1999). The prognostic value of sensory impairment in older persons. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 47(8), 930935.Google Scholar
Reyes‐Ortiz, C. A., Kuo, Y. F., DiNuzzo, A. R., Ray, L. A., Raji, M. A., & Markides, K. S. (2005). Near vision impairment predicts cognitive decline: Data from the Hispanic established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 681686. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53219.xGoogle Scholar
Roets-Merken, L. M., Zuidema, S. U., Vernooij-Dassen, M. J. F. J., & Kempen, G. I. J. M. (2014). Screening for hearing, visual and dual sensory impairment in older adults using behaviour cues: A validation study. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 51, 14341440.Google Scholar
Roets-Merken, L. M., Zuidema, S. U., Vernooij-Dassen, M. J. F. J., Teerenstra, S., Hermsen, P. G. J. M., Kempen, G. J. M., et al. (2017). Effectiveness of a nurse-supported self-management programme for dual sensory impaired older adults in long-term care: A cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open, 8, e016674. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2017-016674Google Scholar
Rogers, M. A., & Langa, K. M. (2010). Untreated poor vision: A contributing factor to late-life dementia. American Journal of Epidemiology, 171(6), 728735. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwp453Google Scholar
Schneider, J. M., Gopinath, B., McMahon, C. M., Leeder, S. R., Mitchell, P., & Wang, J. J. (2011). Dual sensory impairment in older age. Journal of Aging and Health, 23(8), 13091324. https://doi.org/10.1177/0898264311408418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, J. M., McMahon, C., Gopinath, B., Kifley, A., Barton, R., Mitchell, P., et al. (2014). Dual sensory impairment and hearing aid use among clients attending low-vision services in Australia: The vision-hearing project. Journal of Aging and Health, 26(2), 231249.Google Scholar
Shakarchi, A. F., Assi, L., Ehrlich, J. R., Deal, J. A., Reed, N. S., & Swenor, B. K. (2020). Dual sensory impairment and perceived everyday discrimination in the United States. JAMA Ophthalmology, 138(12), 12271233. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2020.3982Google Scholar
Simcock, P. (2017). One of society’s most vulnerable groups? A systematically conducted literature review exploring the vulnerability of deafblind people. Health and Social Care in the Community, 25(3), 813839. https://doi.org/10.1111/hsc.12317Google Scholar
Simcock, P., & Wittich, W. (2019). Are older deafblind people being left behind? A narrative review of literature on deafblindness through the lens of the United Nations principles for older people. Journal of Social Welfare and Family Law, 41, 119. https://doi.org/10.1080/09649069.2019.1627088Google Scholar
Sinha, M., & Bleakney, A. (2014). Receiving care at home. Ottawa, ON: Statistics Canada. Retrieved 11 December 2019 from https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/pub/89-652-x/89-652-x2014002-eng.htm.Google Scholar
Slaughter, S. E., Hopper, T., Ickert, C., & Erin, D. F. (2014). Identification of hearing loss among residents with dementia: Perceptions of health care aides. Geriatric Nursing, 35, 434440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gerinurse.2014.07.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. L., Bennett, L. W., & Wilson, R. H. (2008). Prevalence and characteristics of dual sensory impairment (hearing and vision) in a veteran population. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 45(4), 597610. https://doi.org/10.1682/JRRD.2007.02.0023Google Scholar
Urqueta Alfaro, A., Guthrie, D. M., McGraw, C., & Wittich, W. (2020). Older adults with dual sensory loss in rehabilitation show high functioning and may fare better than those with single sensory loss. PLoS One, 15(8), e0237152. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237152Google Scholar
Urqueta Alfaro, A., Guthrie, D. M., Phillips, N. A., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Mick, P., McGraw, C., et al. (2019). Detection of vision and/or hearing loss using the interRAI Community Health Assessment aligns well with common behavioral vision/hearing measurements. PLoS One, 14(10), e0223123. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223123Google Scholar
Vengnes Grue, E. V., Hylen Ranhoff, A. H., Noro, A., Finne-Soveri, H., Birna Jensdottir, A. B., Ljunggren, G., et al. (2009). Vision and hearing impairments and their associations with falling and loss of instrumental activities in daily living in acute hospitalized older persons in five Nordic hospitals. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Science, 23(4), 635643. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6712.2008.00654.xGoogle Scholar
von Elm, E., Egger, M., Altman, D. G., Pocock, S. J., Gotzsche, P. C., & Vandenbroucke, J. P. (2007). Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. British Medical Journal, 335, 806808. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.11.008Google Scholar
Vos, T. (2016). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 310 disease and injuries, 1990–2015: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. The Lancet, 338(10053), 15451602. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31678-6Google Scholar
Wang, J. J., Mitchell, P., Simpson, J. M., Cumming, R. G., & Smith, W. (2001). Visual impairment, age-related cataract, and mortality. Archives of Ophthalmology, 119(8), 11861190. https://doi.org/10.1001/archopht.119.8.1186Google Scholar
Wang, J. J., Mitchell, P., Smith, W., Cumming, R. G., & Attebo, K. (1999). Impact of visual impairment of use of community support services by elderly persons: The Blue Mountains Eye Study. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 40(1), 1219.Google ScholarPubMed
Westaway, L., Wittich, W., & Overbury, O. (2010). Depression and burden in spouses of individuals with sensory impairment. Insight: Research and Practice in Visual Impairment and Blindness, 4(1), 2936.Google Scholar
Wiles, J. L., Leibing, A., Guberman, N., Reeve, J., & Allen, R. E. (2012). The meaning of “aging in place” to older people. The Gerontologist, 52(3), 357366. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnr098Google Scholar
Williams, N., Guthrie, D. M., Davidson, J. G. S., Fisher, K., & Griffith, L. E. (2018). A deterioration in hearing is associated with functional and cognitive impairments, difficulty with communication, and greater health instability. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 39, 159171. https://doi.org/10.1177/0733464818755312Google Scholar
Williams, N., Jamal, S., & Guthrie, D. (2018). The relationship between caregiver burden and depressive symptoms in Ontario home care clients. Home Health Care Services Quarterly, 37(1), 6076. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621424.2018.1425647Google Scholar
Williams, N., Phillips, N. A., Wittich, W., Campos, J. L., Mick, P., Orange, J. B., et al. (2020). Hearing and cognitive impairments increase the risk of long-term care admissions. Innovation in Aging, 4(2), 112. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igz053Google Scholar
Wittich, W., Jarry, J., Groulx, G., Southall, K., & Gagné, J. (2016). Rehabilitation and research priorities for deafblindness for the next decade. JVIB, 110(4), 219.Google Scholar
Wittich, W., & Simcock, P. (2019). Aging and combined vision and hearing loss. In Ravenscroft, J. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of visual impairment: Social and cultural research (pp. 438456). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wittich, W., Watanabe, D. H., & Gagné, J. (2011). Sensory and demographic characteristics of deafblindness rehabilitation clients in Montreal, Canada. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 32, 242251. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-1313.2012.00897.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamada, Y., Denkinger, M., Onder, G., Henrard, J.-C., van der Roest, H., Finne-Soveri, H., et al. (2015). Dual sensory impairment and cognitive decline: The results from the Shelter study. Journals of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 71(1), 117123. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glv036Google Scholar
Yamada, Y., Vlachova, M., Richter, T., Finne-Soveri, H., Gindin, J., van der Roest, H., et al. (2014). Prevalence and correlates of hearing and visual impairments in European nursing homes: Results from the SHELTER study. Journal of American Medical Directors Association, 15(10), 738–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2014.05.012Google Scholar
Yang, D., & Dalton, J. E. (2012). A unified approach to measuring the effect size between two groups using SAS. Retrieved 4 May 2020 from http://www.lerner.ccf.org/qhs/software/lib/stddiff.pdf.Google Scholar
Zheng, D. D., Swenor, B. K., Christ, S. L., West, S. K., Lam, B. L., & Lee, D. J. (2018). Longitudinal associations between visual impairment and cognitive functioning: The Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study. JAMA Ophthalmology, 136(9), 989995. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.2493Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of the seven different cohorts and their status at baseline and at follow-up

Figure 1

Table 2. Comparison of demographic and clinical characteristics across the seven cohorts at baseline (T1)

Figure 2

Table 3. Percent of participants who experienced a change between baseline (T1) and onset of a new impairment (T2) across the seven cohortsa

Figure 3

Figure 1. Individuals who experienced any deterioration on the Cognitive Performance Scale (CPS) (any one-point increase) between time 1 and time 2

Figure 4

Figure 2. Comparison of the clients with and without sensory impairments who experienced a deterioration in expressive communication

Figure 5

Figure 3. Comparison of clients with and without sensory impairments who experienced a deterioration in their receptive communication

Figure 6

Figure 4. Comparison of clients with and without sensory impairments with a caregiver who experienced a deterioration on the Caregiver Risk Evaluation (CaRE) algorithm

Figure 7

Table 4. Logistic regression models for the association between newly acquired sensory impairments and the odds of the event across four unique outcomes