Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T08:04:50.134Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Investigating the relationship between mindfulness, stress and creativity in introductory engineering design

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2023

Hannah Nolte
Affiliation:
Department of Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA
Nicolás F. Soria Zurita
Affiliation:
School of Engineering Design and Innovation, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA College of Sciences and Engineering, University of San Francisco, Quito, Campus Cumbayá, Quito EC170901, Ecuador
Elizabeth Starkey
Affiliation:
School of Engineering Design and Innovation, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA
Christopher McComb*
Affiliation:
Department of Mechanical Engineering, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA
*
Corresponding author C. McComb ccm@cmu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) for engineering design are promising for their stress management, cognition and well-being benefits. Prior work concluded that engineering design is stressful and that each engineering design stage has unique stressors. This non-randomized study investigated the effect of an MBI on students’ cognitive stress and final design creativity during a multistage, hands-on design assessment. Data were collected using surveys, project deliverables and follow-up interviews. While no significant increase was found in students’ measured state mindfulness due to the intervention, students in the MBI condition were more likely to perceive the intervention positively compared to students in the control condition (alternative use tasks). Students in both conditions were found to have similar levels of state stress, which indicates that the MBI had no observable effect on students’ measured stress during design. Although students in the MBI condition were found to produce higher-quality final designs, there were no differences in design creativity or novelty. When data were clustered to identify types of student experiences, state mindfulness was found to meaningfully contribute, but state stress was not. Future research should continue to investigate MBIs in engineering design as a potential approach to improve design education and outcomes.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is used to distribute the re-used or adapted article and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) for college students have been increasingly implemented within universities due to their ability to support students’ well-being. There is evidence that MBIs impact stress management (Mohan, Sharma & Bijlani Reference Mohan, Sharma and Bijlani2011; Bamber & Kraenzle Schneider Reference Bamber and Kraenzle Schneider2016), cognition (Zeidan et al. Reference Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, David and Goolkasian2010) and well-being (Brown & Ryan Reference Brown and Ryan2003) – making them a promising intervention for engineering design education. However, there is a lack of research investigating the applicability of MBIs for engineering or engineering design.

Prior research has determined that mental stress is induced during engineering design (Zhu, Yao & Zeng Reference Zhu, Yao and Zeng2007; Petkar et al. Reference Petkar, Dande, Yadav, Zeng and Nguyen2009; Nguyen, Xu & Zeng Reference Nguyen, Xu and Zeng2013; Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2014, Reference Nguyen and Zeng2017a; Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2020, Reference Nolte and McComb2021). Understanding design stress is critical because high levels of stress are known to decrease performance during problem-solving tasks (Beilock et al. Reference Beilock, Kulp, Holt and Carr2004; Beilock & DeCaro Reference Beilock and DeCaro2007). Alternatively, a moderate level of stress is theorized to be ideal for encouraging the most creative engineering design outcomes (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012). Practicing mindfulness has been shown to reduce students’ general perceived stress and anxiety (Bamber & Kraenzle Schneider Reference Bamber and Kraenzle Schneider2016), and MBIs have been used to manage task-induced stress during stressful tasks like videogames (Mohan et al. Reference Mohan, Sharma and Bijlani2011). Therefore, an MBI is promising for helping engineering students manage the task-induced stress of engineering design. Additionally, a relationship between mindfulness and creativity has also been observed, and it is suggested that practicing mindfulness can support creativity (Baas, Nevicka & Ten Velden Reference Baas, Nevicka and Ten Velden2014; Lebuda, Zabelina & Karwowski Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016; Henriksen, Richardson & Shack Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). However, there is a lack of research exploring mindfulness and creativity in engineering design, which is important because design creativity is a critical metric of design success in engineering coursework (Chiu & Salustri Reference Chiu and Salustri2010).

Incorporating MBIs into engineering design courses has the potential to help students manage the stress of design and support students’ well-being. This research is crucial to determining effective stress-management interventions for design and contributes unique insights into the effect of MBIs on introductory engineering students. The study presented here will investigate the impact of a longitudinal MBI on students’ stress and design creativity during a hands-on multistage design assessment.

2. Relevant literature

2.1. Overview of mindfulness research

The current work will use an accepted definition of mindfulness, specifically a “process of regulating attention in order to bring a quality of nonelaborative awareness to current experience and a quality of relating to one’s experience within an orientation of curiosity, experiential openness, and acceptance” (Bishop et al. Reference Bishop, Lau, Shapiro, Carlson, Anderson, Carmody, Segal, Abbey, Speca, Velting and Devins2004, p. 234). Originally, MBIs, like the prevalent Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction program (Kabat-Zinn Reference Kabat-Zinn1982, Reference Kabat-Zinn2003), were used to treat adults in clinical settings and have been found to improve many conditions, including chronic pain (Kabat-Zinn Reference Kabat-Zinn1982; Creswell Reference Creswell2017; Creswell et al. Reference Creswell, Lindsay, Villalba and Chin2019), anxiety (Creswell Reference Creswell2017) and depression (Creswell Reference Creswell2017). After demonstrating success in clinical settings, MBIs were extended to other populations like healthy adults (e.g., Khoury et al. Reference Khoury, Sharma, Rush and Fournier2015) and additional settings like education (Renshaw Reference Renshaw2019).

However, mindfulness-based research within engineering is limited. A majority of this research in engineering has focused on investigating engineers’ level of dispositional or trait mindfulness, which is defined as an individual’s enduring level of mindfulness that is consistent over time. Dispositional or trait mindfulness in engineering students is associated with higher innovation self-efficacy (Rieken et al. Reference Rieken, Schar, Shapiro and Sheppard2017); greater business self-efficacy (Rieken, Schar & Sheppard Reference Rieken, Schar and Sheppard2016); decreased perceived stress (Lal et al. Reference Lal, Pathak, Chaturvedi and Talukdar2019); increased mathematical test scores through reduced test anxiety (Bellinger, DeCaro & Ralston Reference Bellinger, DeCaro and Ralston2015) and improved academic outcomes (Estrada & Dalton Reference Estrada and Dalton2019). Additionally, recent research found that engineering students who practiced mindfulness-based activities during the COVID-19 pandemic had healthier responses to survey questions indicating clinical health concerns (Beddoes & Danowitz Reference Beddoes and Danowitz2021). These results suggest that engineering students’ trait mindfulness is associated with many positive outcomes.

Further research has started to investigate MBIs for engineers. First-year students were receptive to a short MBI for stress and resilience (Huerta Reference Huerta2018), and upper-level engineering students were receptive to incorporating brief daily mindfulness practices into an online engineering course during the COVID-19 pandemic (Miller & Jensen Reference Miller and Jensen2020). A one-credit course for engineering students also found that non-cognitive competencies like mindfulness can be learned and developed (Ge et al. Reference Ge, Berger, Major and Froiland2019), and other mindfulness interventions led to improved software development (Bernárdez et al. Reference Bernárdez, Durán, Parejo and Ruiz-Cortés2014) and conceptual modeling (Bernárdez et al. Reference Bernárdez, Durán, Parejo and Ruiz-Cortés2018). Additionally, a four-workshop MBI for first-year engineering students was found to increase their mindfulness and enhance their intrapersonal and interpersonal competencies (Huerta et al. Reference Huerta, Carberry, Pipe and McKenna2021). This prior research indicates that engineering students are receptive to MBIs and that they have a variety of benefits.

While many of the previous research findings support that MBIs would be beneficial to engineering design, mindfulness research in this field is almost non-existent. Yet, prior mindfulness research in other fields has found many improvements in aspects critical to engineering design like improved executive function (Zeidan et al. Reference Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, David and Goolkasian2010), lower cognitive rigidity (Greenberg, Reiner & Meiran Reference Greenberg, Reiner and Meiran2012) and greater attention (Zeidan et al. Reference Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, David and Goolkasian2010; Norris et al. Reference Norris, Creem, Hendler and Kober2018). Specifically, in engineering design, students perceived benefits from a brief mindfulness intervention that induced a small increase in students’ decentering, which is one aspect of students’ state mindfulness (Nolte, Huff & McComb Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). However, the intervention had no observable impact on students’ stress during three short design tasks (Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). Therefore, further mindfulness research in engineering design is warranted.

Additional mindfulness research with college students indicates that engineering students would further benefit from incorporating MBIs through improved well-being. For example, MBIs could be used to help students transition to college (Dvořáková et al. Reference Dvořáková, Kishida, Li, Elavsky, Broderick, Agrusti and Greenberg2017). Also, a narrative review of the literature has found that mindfulness meditation can reduce college students’ stress and anxiety (Bamber & Kraenzle Schneider Reference Bamber and Kraenzle Schneider2016). MBIs are promising for introductory engineering design students as previous research has shown many benefits related to developing important engineering skills and improving students’ well-being.

2.2. Stress

Engineering design has been found to induce stress for designers (Zhu et al. Reference Zhu, Yao and Zeng2007; Petkar et al. Reference Petkar, Dande, Yadav, Zeng and Nguyen2009; Nguyen et al. Reference Nguyen, Xu and Zeng2013; Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2014, Reference Nguyen and Zeng2017a; Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2020, Reference Nolte and McComb2021; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). The stress of engineering design is predominantly due to the cognitive abilities required to do engineering design and the inherent complexity of design problems (Dym et al. Reference Dym, Agogino, Eris, Frey and Leifer2005). Understanding stress across the engineering design process is critical because different stress levels can affect a designer’s cognitive experience during design and the quality of design outcomes. High levels of stress can reduce a designer’s effort (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2014, Reference Nguyen and Zeng2017a), can decrease task performance (Sandi Reference Sandi2013), and are likely to result in less creative designs (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012). However, a more moderate stress experience is theorized to lead to the most creative designs (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012), better design performance (Yang, Liu & Zeng Reference Yang, Liu and Zeng2018) and improved concentration (Degroote et al. Reference Degroote, Schwaninger, Heimgartner, Hedinger, Ehlert and Wirtz2020).

Previous research indicates that the acute stress (i.e., stress lasting only short periods of time) brought on by participating in engineering design fluctuates across the design process (Zhu et al. Reference Zhu, Yao and Zeng2007; Nguyen et al. Reference Nguyen, Xu and Zeng2013; Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2014; Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2021; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). When the design process was separated into principal stages, it was found that physical modeling was the most stressful for first-year engineers, followed by concept generation and concept selection (Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2021; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). Correspondingly, prior research with engineering graduate students identified that low to moderate stress is experienced during most of the conceptual design process with less time spent at high stress levels (Nguyen et al. Reference Nguyen, Xu and Zeng2013; Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2014). Therefore, stress management strategies to help designers manage periods of high stress could be beneficial. However, prior work identified that each stage of the design process has unique sources of stress (Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2020, Reference Nolte and McComb2021; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a), which would make it incredibly difficult for a stress management intervention to directly target sources of stress throughout the design process. Moreover, many additional factors are likely to affect stress during the design process, like the design prompt (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012), time constraints (Yerkes & Dodson Reference Yerkes and Dodson1908), problem-solving strategy (Wang et al. Reference Wang, Nguyen and Zeng2015; Zhao & Zeng Reference Zhao and Zeng2019) and individual differences (García-García et al. Reference García-García, Chulvi, Royo, Gual and Felip2019). Consequently, a stress management intervention targeting high stress generally would likely be the most effective. An MBI was primarily chosen for this study because it can target stress generally, and prior research has shown that MBIs can reduce the adverse effects of stress without reducing task performance (e.g., Mohan et al. Reference Mohan, Sharma and Bijlani2011).

Furthermore, engineering students are already experiencing substantial stress (Foster & Spencer Reference Foster and Spencer2003) and in recent years, the stress of college students has been rising (American College Health Association 2020). This indicates that engineering students struggle with a considerable amount of chronic stress (i.e., stress lasting extended periods of time), which can adversely impact their health and well-being. In addition, students’ well-being has been found to drop significantly during their first year, and while it rebounds by the end of the year, it does not return to pre-college levels (Conley et al. Reference Conley, Kirsch, Dickson and Bryant2014). The ability of MBIs to support student well-being (Brown & Ryan Reference Brown and Ryan2003; Shapiro et al. Reference Shapiro, Oman, Thoresen, Plante and Flinders2008) also encourages investigating their applicability for introductory engineering design courses. MBIs for engineering students have the potential to improve their engineering design experience and help institutions develop more holistically sound engineers.

2.3. Creativity

Creativity in engineering design is often a measure of both novelty and appropriateness (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Hunter, Starkey, Ramachandran, Ahmed and Fuge2020) and is considered a critical metric of design success (Sarkar & Chakrabarti Reference Sarkar and Chakrabarti2011), especially in engineering courses (Chiu & Salustri Reference Chiu and Salustri2010). Creativity is influenced by many factors (Sarkar & Chakrabarti Reference Sarkar and Chakrabarti2011) and is difficult to measure (Chiu & Salustri Reference Chiu and Salustri2010; Sarkar & Chakrabarti Reference Sarkar and Chakrabarti2011; Denson et al. Reference Denson, Buelin, Lammi and D’Amico2015; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Hunter, Starkey, Ramachandran, Ahmed and Fuge2020).

Prior work has determined a link between creativity and mindfulness (Baas et al. Reference Baas, Nevicka and Ten Velden2014; Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016; Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). A recent meta-analysis determined a statistically significant but relatively weak relationship between mindfulness and creativity that was moderated by the type of mindfulness or aspect focus (e.g., mindfulness focused on open-monitoring aspects may be beneficial to creativity) (Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016). Another qualitative review found that practicing mindfulness can support the skills for creativity (e.g., awareness, capacity for nonjudgmental description, and restraint from immediate evaluation), and practicing mindfulness could be beneficial in an educational setting (Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). Still, more research is needed in this area (Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). Therefore, while there is likely a relationship between mindfulness and creativity, more research is required to characterize this connection. For example, each aspect of mindfulness is likely to contribute to creativity differently (Baas et al. Reference Baas, Nevicka and Ten Velden2014; Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016; Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). There is a lack of research investigating the relationship between mindfulness and creativity in engineering design.

Stress is theorized to have an inverse U-shaped relationship with creativity according to the Mental-Stress Creativity Relation of Nguyen & Zeng (Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012). This theory postulates that mental stress is positively related to mental workload and negatively related to mental capacity (Tang & Zeng Reference Tang and Zeng2009; Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012). Mental capacity is a combination of a designer’s knowledge, affect and skill (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012). The theory states that “workload can be defined as an external load assigned to a person whereas mental capacity is the person’s ability to handle the external load” (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012, p. 76). Specifically, this theory predicts that a moderate amount of stress will lead to the most creative design, while low or high stress will result in designs of lower creativity. This theory is supported by a meta-analysis that determined a curvilinear relationship between stress and creativity where low stress supported creativity, and high stress decreased creativity (Byron, Khazanchi & Nazarian Reference Byron, Khazanchi and Nazarian2010). Also congruent with this theory, recent work identified that mental workload was predictive of creative performance during an engineering design activity (Chen, Chang & Chuang Reference Chen, Chang and Chuang2022). It is predicted that students who participate in an MBI will produce more creative designs due to the MBI helping them manage the stress of design.

3. Research aims and significance

This work will investigate the effect of a longitudinal MBI on students’ cognitive experience and design outcome creativity during a hands-on engineering design assessment. An MBI is promising for engineering design to help manage the stress induced during engineering design (e.g., Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2021), promoting the development of critical engineering design skills (e.g., Rieken et al. Reference Rieken, Schar, Shapiro and Sheppard2017) and supporting first-year engineering students’ well-being (e.g., Huerta et al. Reference Huerta, Carberry, Pipe and McKenna2021). Additionally, this work will be the first to investigate stress, mindfulness and design creativity in engineering design. Specifically, the following research questions (RQs) will be addressed:

  1. 1. What effect does an MBI have on introductory engineering students’ stress during a multistage, hands-on engineering design assessment?

  2. 2. What effect does students’ state mindfulness have on the creativity of their design outcomes?

  3. 3. What effect does students’ state stress have on the creativity of their design outcomes?

  4. 4. How do introductory engineering students perceive the inclusion of MBI to affect their mindfulness, stress and creativity during a multistage, hands-on engineering design assessment?

It is hypothesized that the MBI will help students better manage their stress during design, which will lead to a more moderate stress experience during design and more creative final designs. This study will help educators and researchers understand the effect of a longitudinal MBI on students’ experience during an engineering design challenge. Therefore, the applicability of the intervention for engineering design education and other disciplines with similar characteristics can be determined. This intervention could also contribute to the improved well-being of engineering students regardless of its effect on the engineering design outcomes.

4. Methodology

This work investigated the effect of an MBI on students’ stress and design creativity during multistage, hands-on design assessment for first-year engineers. Students in the MBI condition practiced mindfulness-based meditation (MBM) in-class during the first half of the semester and had two MBM breaks during their assessment. Students in the control condition had class as normal and experienced two control breaks during the assessment where they completed one alternative use task (AUT) during each break. Data were collected during the first in-person semester following online instruction due to the global COVID-19 pandemic.

4.1. Experimental design

In this non-randomized study, students completed a two-part hands-on design assessment consisting of an in-class design challenge and a written project report. Quantitative data were collected during the assessment using multiple surveys and project deliverables. Follow-up interviews were also conducted with a subset of the students who participated in the assessment (i.e., qualitative data). This study puts an emphasis on the quantitative data and results and uses the qualitative data and results to provide a broader perspective of the quantitative results.

4.1.1. Quantitative experimental design

Students from four course sections of a required first-year engineering design course participated in this institutional review board–approved study at a large mid-Atlantic university. This study followed best practices for research within the domain; however, it was not pre-registered. First-year engineering students were chosen as the population because they are learning fundamental engineering design skills for the first time. Therefore, they will rely on the habits they form during this course in the future. Additionally, first-year students are learning how to manage the demands of college and developing behavioral habits that will likely persist into their careers. Students received no compensation for this portion of the study.

Participants were asked to report demographic information after completing the in-class portion of the assessment. Of the students who reported demographics, the average age of participants was 18.35 years (SD = 0.59 years, Range: 18–20 years), 34.2% identified as women and 64.4% identified as men, and 75.4% identified as white/Caucasian and 23.3% as non-white or a minoritized race or ethnicity. Students were also asked, “Within the six months before the start of the Fall 2021 semester, how often did you intentionally participate in mindfulness activities? Examples may include mediation, yoga, Qigong, Tai Chi, etc.” to assess their previous experience with mindfulness activities. Many students reported having never participated in mindfulness activities (45.2% of students), 15.1% reported once a month or less, 19.2% selected once a week, and 17.8% chose more than once a week or daily. When asked to report their mindfulness activities (if they had previous experience), 17 students listed exercising (e.g., running, yoga), 18 reported deep breathing or meditation, and 14 described other activities (e.g., prayer, journaling). This indicates that a majority of students had limited prior mindfulness experience and did not participate in mindfulness-based practices regularly.

During the first half of the semester, students in the MBI condition completed short MBM practices in class, while students in the control condition had class as usual. Students were assigned to their experimental condition according to the course section they were enrolled in. During the first week of the semester, students in the MBI condition were informed that they would be completing these meditations in class and received access to an educational module briefly explaining mindfulness and its relevance to college students. Students completed 11 MBM practices during the first half of the semester (before the assessment) from the Foundations portion of the Healthy Minds Program App (Healthy Minds Innovations 2021). The Healthy Minds Program App is intended to promote human flourishing by training four core dimensions of well-being including awareness, connection, insight and purpose (Dahl, Wilson-Mendenhall & Davidson Reference Dahl, Wilson-Mendenhall and Davidson2020), and was previously found to reduce distress and improve markers of well-being (Goldberg et al. Reference Goldberg, Imhoff-Smith, Bolt, Wilson-Mendenhall, Dahl, Davidson and Rosenkranz2020). Each practice was formatted as a 5-minute guided sitting meditation and was completed in-class. Students listened to the meditation as a group but participated individually. Only one meditation was completed per class period and students did 1–2 meditations a week. Narrators for each practice were randomly selected to create an equal number of meditations narrated by a male and female voice as previous research has shown that voice quality can impact perceptions of the speaker like trustworthiness (O’Connor & Barclay Reference O’Connor and Barclay2017).

During the multistage, hands-on assessment, students experienced two scheduled breaks. Students in the MBI condition completed the Mindfulness of Sound and Counting the Breath meditations from the Awareness section of the Healthy Minds App (Healthy Minds Innovations 2021), which were formatted to match the in-class practices. Students in the control condition completed two 5-minute AUTs, one for a brick and one for a pencil, in which students were asked to brainstorm as many uses as they could for each object different from its normal use. Typically, AUTs (Torrance Reference Torrance1966) are used in engineering design research to measure divergent thinking and, consequently, creativity (Alhashim et al. Reference Alhashim, Marshall, Hartog, Jonczyk, Dickson, van Hell, Okudan-Kremer and Siddique2020). In this study, the AUTs were used as an active control as task-switching during design has been shown to reduce design fixation and improve design performance (Sio, Kotovsky & Cagan Reference Sio, Kotovsky and Cagan2017). Break one occurred 35 minutes into the assessment during the conceptual design phase, and break two occurred 75 minutes into the assessment during the prototyping phase. A complete experimental methodology can be seen in Table 1.

Table 1. Experimental methodology for the design challenge

The assessment included an in-class hands-on design challenge and an out-of-class written project report. The design challenge was 2-hours in duration and was completed individually in-class. The prompt of the design challenge instructed students to design a solution to help elderly individuals maintain their yard (e.g., mow the lawn, rake leaves or pull weeds). Students were asked to understand the problem, conduct background research, identify user-needs, ideate at least four concepts, complete a concept selection matrix and build an alpha prototype. At the end of the design challenge, students were asked to submit a written deliverable detailing their work during the design challenge. After the design challenge, students were given 24 hours to write and submit an individual project report detailing their final solution, explaining their design process, and reflecting on their design process.

The primary data for this study was collected using four surveys during the in-class portion of the assessment. Students took a pre-assessment survey containing the pre-task version of the Short Stress State Questionnaire (SSSQ; Helton Reference Helton2004; Helton & Nöswall Reference Helton and Nöswall2010). The students in the MBI took the second survey after completing their first MBM during break one. This survey queried their thoughts on the break and contained the Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS; Lau et al. Reference Lau, Bishop, Segal, Buis, Anderson, Carlson, Shapiro, Carmody, Abbey and Devins2006). The second survey for the control group contained the AUT and the TMS. The third survey for each condition had the same format as the second survey, except the TMS was removed due to concerns about survey fatigue. The post-assessment survey was the same for both conditions and was taken after completing the in-class design challenge. The post-assessment survey included a modified version (Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2020, Reference Nolte and McComb2021; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a) of the NASA-Raw Task Load Index (Hart & Staveland Reference Hart and Staveland1988, Reference Hart and Staveland2006), the post-task version of the SSSQ, and a few questions querying students’ sources of stress and coping during the design challenge. A detailed description of each survey measure can be seen in Table 2. Students’ written project reports were also collected after the design challenge.

Table 2. Survey measures

4.1.2. Qualitative experimental design

Only students who indicated that they were willing to be contacted about a follow-up interview were eligible for this portion of the study. Interviewees were originally recruited using a maximum variation sampling technique aimed to create a diverse sample in terms of students’ reported stress during the assessment and their demographic characteristics, including age, gender and ethnicity. The selected students were recruited via email after submitting their written project reports.

However, the response rate for students selected for this group was low. Consequently, recruitment was expanded to invite all students who were willing to be contacted for a follow-up interview. However, only students in the original sample were sent reminder emails. A total of 24 interviews were conducted using an online video conferencing platform. All interviews were conducted within a week of students completing the in-class design challenge. Interviewees from the MBI condition (N = 12) were all 18 years old; eight identified as men and four identified as women; and 10 identified as white/Caucasian and two identified as non-white or a minoritized race. Students from the control condition (N = 12) included nine 18-year-olds, two 19-year-olds and one 20-year-old; six men and six women; and eight students who identified as white/Caucasian and four who identified as non-white or a minoritized race or ethnicity. Students in both conditions had a range of previous mindfulness experience.

All interviews were conducted by one interviewer, recorded and transcribed for analysis. Interview questions were semi-structured (Hove & Anda Reference Hove and Anda2005; Rubin & Rubin Reference Rubin and Rubin2012) to allow the interviewer to ask each interviewee follow-up questions. The interview inquired about students’ design process, cognitive experience during design, attitudes toward the scheduled breaks, and thoughts on the inherent difficulty or challenge of engineering. Students in the MBI were also asked about their thoughts on the in-class MBM practices. All interview questions can be seen in the Appendix. Transcripts were created using a secure automatic transcription software and manually verified by one of the researchers to ensure accuracy. Students who participated in the follow-up interviews received a $20 gift card as compensation.

4.2. Procedure

Students consented to this study at least 1 week before completing the assessment. All students individually completed the in-class design challenge portion of the assessment on the same day during their assigned class time. The design challenge was 2 hours in duration, and all students were given the same design prompt. During the design challenge, students were asked to complete four surveys and two breaks. The pre-assessment survey was taken before starting the design challenge. The first break was 35 minutes into the design challenge, and the second survey was taken immediately after the first break. The second break was 75 minutes into the design challenge, and the third survey was completed immediately following the second break. The last survey was taken after completing the design challenge. All breaks were 5 minutes in duration, and the format was dependent on the course section students were enrolled in. At the end of the class period, students submitted a document detailing their work during the design challenge.

Students were then given 24 hours to individually write a project report detailing their final design, their design process, and a reflection on their experience during the design challenge. After submitting their project reports, a diverse subset of students were recruited via email to participate in a 30-minute follow-up interview. After a couple of days, interview recruitment was expanded to invite any student willing to be contacted about a follow-up to participate. All interviews were conducted within a week of completing the in-class design challenge. The entire experimental design can be seen in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Generalized experimental procedure.

5. Results

The results of this study will be presented by measure or analysis for clarity. Quantitative results will be presented before the qualitative results and the relationships between the various results will be detailed in the discussion. While the sample size of the current non-randomized study was similar to others in the domain, the study was unpowered. Therefore, effect sizes are reported with all results to provide additional context. The quantitative analysis was conducted using R Studio and R version 4.0.3, and the qualitative analysis was conducted using NVivo 12. All assumptions for statistical testing were met and significance was assessed using an alpha level of 0.05.

5.1. Toronto Mindfulness Scale

Students completed the TMS to evaluate their level of state mindfulness after their first design challenge break. Total TMS scores were calculated for each student by averaging their responses for all 13 TMS questions. Students’ scores for each factor of the TMS were calculated by averaging each student’s response to the questions associated with each of the factors, curiosity (six questions) and decentering (seven questions). Cronbach’s alpha computed across all TMS responses is 0.926, indicating acceptable internal consistency. Results for the mindfulness and control conditions can be seen in Figure 2.

Figure 2. TMS scores for the MBI and the control condition.

The total TMS, curiosity and decentering scores for the students in the MBI were compared to the scores of students in the control condition using multiple Mann–Whitney U tests to determine if the MBM break increased students’ level of state mindfulness. Students in the MBI were not found to have significantly different levels of total TMS (Z = −0.474, p = 0.635, r = 0.055), curiosity (Z = −1.025, p = 0.306, r = 0.120) or decentering (Z = −0.108, p = 0.914, r = 0.013) when compared to students in the control condition. Students in the MBI had an average total TMS of 2.22 (SD = 0.95), curiosity of 2.40 (SD = 1.12) and decentering of 2.07 (SD = 0.85), while students in the control condition had an average total TMS of 2.17 (SD = 0.84), curiosity of 2.23 (SD = 1.06) and decentering of 2.09 (SD = 0.87). Future research could include the TMS before and after break one to assess the effect of the breaks more accurately.

It can be seen in Figure 2 that students in the MBI had scores that were highly concentrated around the median even though the range of scores was similar to students in the control condition. This trend likely reflects that a subset of students did not fully participate in the first MBM break. While a majority of students did participate in the breaks as instructed, interviews with the students revealed that some students would not fully participate in the break so that they could continue making progress on the design challenge. The scores centered on the median likely reflect students who fully participated in the break, while the large variability in the first quartile represents students who did not participate in the break. Research in the future should ask students to rank their engagement with the MBMs.

5.2. Short Stress State Questionnaire

Students completed the SSSQ during the pre- and post-assessment surveys. The SSSQ was used to evaluate each student’s level of multidimensional state stress. For each student, a score was calculated for each factor of the SSSQ by averaging the student’s response to each of the questions corresponding to that factor (eight questions per factor). Then each student’s change score for each factor of the SSSQ was calculated by subtracting the student’s pre-assessment score from their post-assessment score for each factor. Cronbach’s alpha computed across all SSSQ responses is 0.790 (pre-assessment) and 0.790 (post-assessment), indicating acceptable internal consistency.

The change scores for students in the MBI were compared to the change scores for students in the control condition using multiple independent t-tests to determine if the MBM breaks had an effect on students’ stress during the design challenge. Students in the MBI condition were not found to have different distress (t(68) = 0.035, p = 0.973, d = 0.008), worry (t(67) = 1.241, p = 0.219, d = 0.230), or engagement (t(69) = 0.061, p = 0.952, d = 0.014) scores compared to students in the control condition. Students in the MBI had an average change in distress of −0.13 (SD = 0.59), worry of −0.12 (SD = 0.73) and engagement of 0.15 (SD = 0.53), while students in the control condition had an average change in distress of −0.14 (SD = 0.80), worry of −0.34 (SD = 0.67) and engagement of 0.16 (SD = 0.47). There is likely no difference between conditions for students’ SSSQ scores. SSSQ scores by factor and condition can be seen in Figure 3.

Figure 3. Comparison of SSSQ change scores.

5.3. Modified NASA-RTLX

Students completed the modified NASA-RTLX during the post-assessment survey. Cronbach’s alpha computed across all NASA-RTLX responses is 0.816, indicating acceptable internal consistency. Each student’s modified NASA-RTLX score was determined by averaging their responses for the nine questions. To determine if there was a difference between the modified NASA-RTLX scores by condition, students’ scores were compared using an independent t-test. As can be seen in Figure 4, no significant difference was found between the modified NASA-RTLX scores for students in the MBI condition compared to students in the control condition (t(70) = −0.401, p = 0.690, d = 0.095). Students in the MBI condition had an average modified NASA-RTLX score of 54.38 (SD = 15.43), and students in the control condition had an average score of 55.96 (SD = 17.93). Statistical results were similar when the same test was run using comparing students’ average NASA-RTLX scores with only the original six measures included. This indicates that the design challenge produced a similar mental workload for students in both conditions that was not significantly impacted by the intervention. Additionally, these results support the conclusion that there were no differences in students’ stress experience by condition.

Figure 4. Modified NASA-RTLX scores compared by condition. Error bars represent ± 1 standard error.

5.4. Final design creativity

Students submitted a written project report detailing their final design within 24 hours of completing the in-class design challenge. These designs were rated for creativity using the Consensual Assessment Technique (CAT; Amabile Reference Amabile1982, Reference Amabile1996; Amabil Reference Amabil1983). CAT is a commonly used technique for measuring creativity in many disciplines that relies on the ratings of subject-matter experts (Baer & Kaufman Reference Baer, Kaufman, Lebuda and Glăveanu2019; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Hunter, Starkey, Ramachandran, Ahmed and Fuge2021) and does not depend on a specific definition of creativity but instead asserts that creativity is subjective and can be recognized and agreed upon by domain experts (Baer & Kaufman Reference Baer, Kaufman, Lebuda and Glăveanu2019).

For this study, two expert raters were used to rate the designs. The experts had completed at least one graduate degree in engineering and had significant experience in engineering design research and assessment, including multiple engineering design publications. The raters received no information on who made the design or which condition the student was in. Each design was rated for novelty and quality on a Likert-type scale that ranged from one (low novelty or quality) to seven (high novelty or quality) (Besemer Reference Besemer1998; Besemer & O’Quin Reference Besemer and O’Quin1999; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Hunter, Starkey, Ramachandran, Ahmed and Fuge2020). Novelty was defined as “original and surprising,” and quality was defined as “value, logic, utility, and how understandable the ideas were” (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Hunter, Starkey, Ramachandran, Ahmed and Fuge2021, p. 031404-3). To determine a creativity score for each design, the average was taken between the novelty and quality ratings. An inter-rater reliability of κ = 0.675 was achieved between the two raters, which is considered as moderate agreement among raters (McHugh Reference McHugh2012).

To determine if the creativity of students’ final designs depended on their experimental condition, three Mann–Whitney U tests were used to compare total creativity, novelty and quality scores by condition (Figure 5). Students in the MBI condition (M = 4.43, SD = 1.06) were not found to have final designs with significantly different total creativity scores (Z = −0.912, p = 0.362, r = 0.107) compared to the final designs of students in the control group (M = 4.04, SD = 1.51). There were also no significant differences found for final design novelty by condition (Z = −0.074, p = 0.941, r = 0.009). Final designs from students in the MBI had a novelty of 3.42 (SD = 1.87) compared to the final designs from students in the control group 3.53 (SD = 1.93).

Figure 5. Creativity scores by condition. The asterisk on Plot C indicates a significant difference.

However, there was a significant difference found for final design quality by condition (Z = −2.479, p = 0.013, r = 0.290). Students in the MBI condition had final designs of higher average quality (M = 5.43, SD = 1.62) compared to the final designs of students in the control group (M = 4.55, SD = 1.79). These results do not directly align with previous research that found that mindfulness contributes to improved creativity (Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016) because students in the MBI did not have significantly higher state mindfulness compared to students in the control group as measured by the TMS. It is posited that the time the MBI students had during the breaks to reflect on design decision-making may have led to higher-quality final design.

5.5. Student experience categorization

Finally, all quantitative data were clustered using the k-means clustering algorithm to determine if there were different types of student experiences during the design challenge regardless of experimental condition. The k-means clustering algorithm was chosen because of its versatility and ability to easily discover hidden groups within datasets (Shraddha & Naganna Reference Shraddha and Naganna2014). The data for each measure were normalized using a z-score transformation, which modifies the variable to have a mean of zero and a standard deviation equal to one. The optimal number of clusters for this data was determined to be three using the elbow method (Ketchen & Shook Reference Ketchen and Shook1996; Makwana, Kodinariya & Makwana Reference Makwana, Kodinariya and Makwana2013), where the amount of explained variance is graphed corresponding to the cluster number and the optimal number of clusters is visually determined as the cluster number that explains the most variance without wasting computational resources (Ketchen & Shook Reference Ketchen and Shook1996). The ideal number of clusters was confirmed using the silhouette method (Rousseeuw Reference Rousseeuw1987), where the number of clusters is chosen to maximize cohesion (Rousseeuw Reference Rousseeuw1987). Visualizations for each measure by cluster can be seen in Figure 6.

Figure 6. Quantitative data by cluster.

Cluster 2 likely represents a typical student experience as this cluster includes a majority of the students in the study (N = 41). This cluster is defined by high state mindfulness scores, slightly higher increases in state stress, average mental workload scores and highly creative final designs. Cluster 2 demonstrates a relationship between high state mindfulness and high final design creativity as these two metrics best characterize Cluster 2. This result likely aligns with prior research that found that mindfulness can support creativity (Baas et al. Reference Baas, Nevicka and Ten Velden2014; Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016; Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). Alternatively, mindfulness may support students choosing to proceed with more creative designs, which leads to higher final design creativity. Previous research has found that the creativity of students’ design reduces throughout the design process (Starkey, Toh & Miller Reference Starkey, Toh and Miller2016). While students in Cluster 2 have high state mindfulness, they also have no decrease or slight increases in state stress. This contradicts previous research, which determined that practicing mindfulness can induce a relaxation effect (Mohan et al. Reference Mohan, Sharma and Bijlani2011). It is likely that students in this cluster were easily able to focus on the task because their high state mindfulness helped them focus on the present moment. They are also able to produce final designs with high creativity but will not experience a decrease in their state stress due to participating in the design task.

The first alternative student experience, Cluster 3 (N = 15), is characterized by low state mindfulness, moderate change stress scores, average mental workload scores and highly creative final designs. Cluster 2 and Cluster 3 have similar mental workload and final design creativity. However, Cluster 3 has lower state mindfulness and slightly greater decreases in state stress than Cluster 2. Cluster 2 likely represents an alternative relationship between state mindfulness and final design creativity. It is likely that mindfulness supports creativity during design but is not the only route for producing creative designs. It is also possible that mindfulness has a U-shaped relationship with creativity. Alternatively, a third factor could have contributed to these students proceeding with highly creative designs like their level of risk aversion (Toh & Miller Reference Toh and Miller2016). It is likely that while students in Cluster 3 do not have the same focus on the design task as those in Cluster 2, as demonstrated by their lower state mindfulness, they can still accomplish the design task and produce equally creative final designs. Students in Cluster 3 also experience slight decreases in state stress due to participating in the task.

Cluster 1 (N = 7) is distinguished by an experience of moderate state mindfulness, slightly greater decreases in state stress (except for engagement), lower mental workload scores and low creativity final designs. Cluster 1 appears to be the opposite of Cluster 2; whereas Cluster 2 has high mindfulness and highly creative final designs, Cluster 1 has average mindfulness and final designs with low creativity. Additionally, Cluster 1 has greater decreases in distress and worry while also having increased engagement. This indicates that students were engaged and concentrated on the design challenge but could not produce final designs of high creativity. It may be that these students struggled with an aspect of the design challenge or may have been experiencing fixation during the design process. Previous research has shown that design fixation can lead to less novel designs (Jansson & Smith Reference Jansson and Smith1991). However, stress is theorized to contribute to design fixation (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2017b), which does not match the experience of this cluster. Cluster 1 likely indicates students that were engaged in the task but struggled to produce a creative final design. Students in Cluster 1 are expected to need additional help or instruction.

5.6. Design challenge intervention

The interview transcripts were analyzed using a directed content analysis approach (Hsieh & Shannon Reference Hsieh and Shannon2005) to qualitative analysis through a post-positivist lens (Guba & Lincoln Reference Guba, Lincoln, Denzin and Lincoln1994). The focus of directed content analysis is to validate or extend current theory or research findings by identifying how they are represented in the data (Hsieh & Shannon Reference Hsieh and Shannon2005). The work sought to extend the Mental Stress-Creativity Relation and the findings of relevant literature for a hands-on engineering design assessment with a MBI through the experiences of the participants. This analysis uses a post-positivist paradigm (Guba & Lincoln Reference Guba, Lincoln, Denzin and Lincoln1994), which contends that the results of analysis indicate context-dependent generalizations but not fully generalizable results (Charney Reference Charney1996; Cooper Reference Cooper1997). Interview responses detailing students’ perception of the design challenge intervention were first reviewed to identify any references to perceived effects of the intervention (i.e., scheduled breaks), and these references were then compared to the Mental Stress-Creativity Relation (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012) to develop operational code definitions. While the transcripts were exclusively coded by the first author, codebook development and iteration were discussed through a collaborative process with other authors and colleagues, and therefore, did not warrant inter-rater reliability.

Students had mixed responses when asked about the intervention they experienced during the design challenge. All students experienced two scheduled 5-minute breaks during the design challenge where they either did an AUT or MBM. Overall, students in the MBI perceived the breaks more positively compared to students in the control condition. Five themes were prevalent when students described their thoughts and feelings on the scheduled breaks, (1) breaks as a distraction, (2) breaks caused a change in emotion, (3) breaks impacted design time, (4) breaks encouraged thinking and reflection and (5) breaks improved creativity.

Many students reported that the breaks were a distraction from the design challenge. While some students described the breaks as a positive distraction from the design challenge (MBI = 7, C = 6), others described the breaks as a negative interruption to their work (MBI = 8, C = 8). Students who described the breaks as positive often reported them being a good opportunity to think about a topic other than the design challenge and how the breaks allowed them to clear their minds and refocus on the design challenge. However, students who described the breaks as a negative distraction from their work expressed that the breaks interrupted their focus and train of thought. These students also described having trouble reorienting themselves to the design challenge after completing the break. Participant 42 describes why they thought the breaks were a negative distraction:

Me personally, when I’m doing a task, I like to stay focused on that task. I don’t take breaks normally when I’m studying or doing homework. I have to sit down in one fell swoop and do it all in a session. So taking “brain breaks” doesn’t really help me, it more derails my train of thought and then I have to do more work to get back on whatever thing I was doing before.

Students also reported that the breaks caused them to experience different emotions or emotional states. When discussing the effect of the breaks on their emotions, students often used terms such as stressed, calm and relaxed to represent emotion or change in emotion. Some students experienced increased positive emotions or an improved emotional state in response to the breaks (MBI = 5, C = 1). In contrast, others reported experiencing new or increased negative emotions in response to the breaks (MBI = 5, C = 5). Students who described a positive emotional experience in response to the breaks often stated that it helped them be calmer and reduced their stress. For example, Participant 3 describes how the MBM break allowed them to manage the stress of the design challenge:

I think that having the mindfulness activity was positive, because it was able to help me relax for a few minutes and it helped with the stress that I had during the design process.

Students who described negative emotions due to the breaks often reported that the interruption to their work caused the negative emotions rather than the break activity itself.

In addition, students perceived an effect on their design time due to the breaks. In this theme, most students reported that the breaks were a waste of design time (MBI = 3, C = 4). However, a couple of students reported that the breaks helped them better manage their time during the design challenge (MBI = 1, C = 1). Students who described the breaks as a waste of design time often stated that the breaks were 5 minutes they would have preferred to use for design. Participant 72 describes how they would have rather been designing than doing the breaks:

When I heard about like the five minute breaks and even when I started, like the first one, I was like “wow, this like looks like a waste of time” just because its five minutes taken out of what I could be using for like designing or something else.

Conversely, a couple of students discussed how the breaks helped them better manage their time during the design challenge. Participant 7 described that the breaks helped them make a timeline for the design challenge, and Participant 42 explained how the breaks helped them evaluate the appropriateness of their pace during the design challenge.

Students, mainly in the MBI condition, described that the breaks encouraged them to do more thinking and reflection (MBI = 5, C = 2). A couple of students explained that the breaks helped them be more self-reflective; however, it was more common for students to report using the breaks to reflect on and think about their design process up to that point. Participant 37 explains how the breaks encouraged them to think about their design decisions:

I think [the breaks] worked out pretty well. They definitely let you like have time to sit there and gather your thoughts and reflect on the decisions that you made, like the past half hour or so. They also helped you think about what you will do moving forward.

Lastly, some students, largely in the control condition, reported a perceived increase in their creativity due to the breaks (MBI = 2, C = 6). Students who described an increase in their creativity typically stated that the breaks helped them think outside the box. For example, Participant 72 describes how the breaks helped them improve their design:

I specifically, I remember after the break, I immediately was thinking of the uses for the brick and then I immediately thought of something for my own design. I thought of my fourth design during the five minute break, so it definitely impacted my design, because I started off having trouble brainstorming and it kind of helped me out.

While it is possible that students did experience an increase in creativity due to the break activity, students might be recalling their first introduction to this activity in their course when it was presented as a technique for “thinking outside the box” during their idea generation unit. It may also be that they were creating an open goal of creativity, that is, a goal for a task that has yet to be completed (Moss, Kotovsky & Cagan Reference Moss, Kotovsky and Cagan2007).

6. Discussion

Students complete a multistage, hands-on design assessment either with two MBM or AUT breaks. Data were collected using multiple surveys, project deliverables and follow-up interviews. This section will detail results related to mindfulness, stress, creativity and the intervention to answer the proposed research questions.

6.1. Mindfulness

No significant difference was found for total TMS, decentering and curiosity scores by condition. This indicates that students did not experience increased state mindfulness due to the first meditation break as measured by the TMS. This lack of increased state mindfulness does not align with previous research, which found that similar mindfulness-based practices can improve students’ levels of state mindfulness (Mahmood, Hopthrow & Randsley de Moura Reference Mahmood, Hopthrow and Randsley de Moura2016; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). While it is possible that students did not experience a change in their state mindfulness due to the MBM, it is also plausible that the TMS did not accurately measure the change due to its focus on curiosity and decentering aspects of mindfulness.

However, the TMS scores of students in the MBI were more centralized around the median TMS score compared to the scores of students in the control condition. This likely reflects that a majority of the students participated in the MBM as instructed and experienced an effect. However, a portion of students likely did not participate in the MBM during break one and, therefore, did not experience an effect. Students’ interview responses support that not all students fully participated in the mediation breaks. A few students mentioned either ignoring the breaks or multitasking during the breaks by continuing to work on the design problem for the assessment. Participant 22 describes how they did not fully participate in the first break but did participate more in the second break:

I kind of did a mixture, whenever I was like at the beginning, I definitely ignored [the break activity], I was like “What did he say?” Then, towards the end, once my anxiety started to go down a bit, I actually started to focus towards [the break activity]. I know for the last one that we did I actually did follow, along with like the voice person, but the first one I like ignored.

Therefore it is likely that not all participants in the MBI condition fully experienced the intervention. Additionally, it is unknown if students experienced an increase in mindfulness after the second break because students did not retake the TMS due to concerns of survey fatigue. It may be that students benefited more from one break over another.

The qualitative results also support the MBM breaks having an effect on students. Students in the MBI were more likely to report an increase in positive emotions due to the break or thinking and reflection during the break despite a similar number of students in both conditions reporting negative break experiences. Moreover, students in the MBI reported being more relaxed and having less perceived stress during the breaks when compared with students in the control condition. This likely indicates that some students were experiencing the effects of increased mindfulness. Alternatively, students in the MBI condition may have had a more positive experience because they were more familiar with the style of breaks as they had been doing similar activities throughout their course, while the control condition had break activities that they had less experience with.

Regardless of the effect of the intervention, students’ level of state mindfulness contributed to their experience during the design challenge. When the quantitative data were clustered to identify types of student experiences, students’ level of state mindfulness was unique for each cluster, while other quantitative measures showed more overlap between clusters. Students in Cluster 1 reported moderate levels, Cluster 2 had high levels and Cluster 3 had low levels of state mindfulness. This indicates that students’ level of state mindfulness meaningfully contributed to their experience during the design challenge. Therefore, continued research to understand how the mindfulness of designers impacts design is warranted.

6.2. Stress

In response to RQ1, what effect does an MBI have on introductory engineering students’ stress during a multistage, hands-on engineering design assessment, no significant difference was found for students’ change in stress scores when compared by condition. This suggests that there is no difference in students’ net stress when compared by condition. This aligns with previous work by the authors that determined that even though a brief MBM resulted in a small change in students’ decentering, there was no observable impact on students’ stress (Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). However, this contradicts reports from the interviews where students reported a change in emotion and stress due to the breaks. For example, Participant 4 from the MBI condition underscores the impact of the breaks on their perceived stress:

The general meditation practices that we did were very, I could say positive because again it helped calm me down a bit more and reduced any little bit of stress that was there. It definitely helped me focus on myself to realize what I needed to do to continue the process.

The qualitative results indicate that a subset of students were experiencing reduced stress due to the MBMs likely through increased mental capacity by improved affect (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012).

However, students from the MBI who did not report reduced stress often reported an alternative experience with increased negative emotions due to the breaks. For example, Participant 22 describes how their anxiety increased due to the MBM breaks:

I felt like my anxiety actually kind of raised a little bit, which was ironic considering it was meant to, I’m pretty sure, to help calm you down and help you deal with your stress because it was focusing on mindfulness. But I felt like it actually increased my anxiety.

When beginning meditation, it may bring the practitioner’s attention to unpleasant experiences or feelings. The increase in negative emotions reported by students in this study likely contributed to the stress they were experiencing by changing their mental capacity through affect (Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012). Overall, this suggests that at an aggregate level, there was likely no change in net stress because some students were experiencing less perceived stress, while others had experiences that likely contributed to increased stress. However, clustering results also indicate that differences in stress were not a meaningful indicator of students’ experience during the design challenge. Students’ change in SSSQ stress was not the most predictive variable of their experience during the design challenge or there is a discrepancy between students’ perceived stress as described in their interviews compared to their measured stress (i.e., SSSQ scores).

6.3. Design creativity

In response to RQ 2, what effect does students’ state mindfulness have on the creativity of their design outcomes?, the final designs produced by students in the MBI condition had significantly higher quality when compared to the final designs produced by students in the control condition. However, no significant differences were found for final design creativity or novelty by condition, likely because the design prompt used in this study implicitly prioritized quality. Prior work has found that the design prompt can affect design creativity (Starkey et al. Reference Starkey, Toh and Miller2016).

Students in the MBI were not found to have significantly higher state mindfulness as measured by the TMS. Therefore, it is improbable that the higher-quality designs produced by students in this condition were due to the relationship between mindfulness and creativity (Baas et al. Reference Baas, Nevicka and Ten Velden2014; Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016; Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020). However, it may be that students in the MBI did have increased state mindfulness that was not measured by the TMS, which contributed to increased final design quality. Alternatively, another aspect of the MBM could have led to the increased final design quality or students choosing final designs of higher quality. It is posited that the MBM breaks provided students with a better opportunity to think and reflect on their design, as indicated by the qualitative results of the study. This opportunity to reflect on their design decision-making likely led to higher-quality designs.

Intriguingly, some students, largely from the control condition, perceived an increase in their creativity due to the breaks during the design challenge, which contradicts the quantitative creativity results. It is possible that these students experienced an increase in their creativity due to the breaks that was not captured in the rating of their final designs as prior work has shown that students do not always choose to proceed with their most creative ideas (Starkey et al. Reference Starkey, Toh and Miller2016). Three out of the six control condition students who reported increases in their creativity due to the breaks had a final design with a creativity above five (range 1–7). Alternatively, it may be that these students were setting an open goal for creativity (Moss et al. Reference Moss, Kotovsky and Cagan2007) (i.e., a goal that had yet to be achieved for increasing the creativity of their designs) and that completing the AUT (i.e., a measure of divergent thinking and creativity) fulfilled this open goal without increasing the creativity of their final designs.

Alternatively, the clustering results suggest two disparate relationships between students’ level of state mindfulness and their final design creativity. However, there was no discernable relationship between state stress and final design creativity. These results further inform RQ 2 (What effect does students’ state mindfulness have on the creativity of their design outcomes?) and RQ 3 (What effect does students’ state stress have on the creativity of their design outcomes?). Cluster 2 had high levels of state mindfulness and Cluster 3 had low levels of state mindfulness, but both produced highly creative final designs. This suggests that there are either two avenues to final designs of high creativity, one supported by mindfulness and one facilitated by a third factor, or there is a U-shaped relationship between mindfulness and design creativity. While mindfulness can support creativity (Baas et al. Reference Baas, Nevicka and Ten Velden2014; Lebuda et al. Reference Lebuda, Zabelina and Karwowski2016; Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020), it is more likely that it is not the only contributor to highly creative final designs. Students’ state stress levels did not vary meaningfully by cluster, and therefore, no distinguishable relationship could be found between students’ state stress and their design creativity. Interventions to increase designers’ mindfulness may help them produce more creative final designs, but there are also other routes to improving design creativity, like reducing design fixation (Jansson & Smith Reference Jansson and Smith1991).

6.4. Design challenge intervention

This section addresses RQ 4, how do introductory engineering students perceive the inclusion of an MBI to affect their mindfulness, stress, and creativity during a multistage, hands-on engineering design assessment?. In general, students in the MBI were more receptive to the scheduled breaks than students in the control condition. Five themes were present regarding students’ perceptions about the design challenge breaks. These themes included (1) breaks as a distraction, (2) breaks caused a change in emotion, (3) breaks impacted design time, (4) breaks encouraged thinking and reflection and (5) breaks improved creativity.

All five of these themes can be directly related back to the Mental-Stress Creativity theory by Nguyen & Zeng (Reference Nguyen and Zeng2012) and indicate that the breaks likely affected students’ stress during the design challenge. The breaks being a distraction likely impacted stress through a temporary increase or decrease in mental workload depending on how positively or negatively they perceived the breaks. Similarly, students’ change in emotion due to the breaks could have increased or decreased their mental capacity through affect depending on their experience. The effect of the breaks on design time likely contributed to reduced mental capacity through affect as many students in this theme reported that the breaks were a waste of design time. Similarly, the thinking and reflection students did during breaks could have increased mental capacity through affect (i.e., when students were self-reflective) or knowledge (i.e., when students reflected on their design decision-making). Lastly, the perceived increase in creativity due to the breaks could have affected mental capacity through skill (i.e., genuine increase in creativity) or affect (i.e., positive emotion due to fulfilling a goal). These results suggest that students likely experienced an increase or decrease in their stress depending on their experience during the breaks. Future research should measure students’ stress immediately following the breaks.

Additionally, aspects of these themes align with prior MBI research. For example, thinking and reflecting during the breaks was mainly reported by students in the MBI. Increased self-reflection was previously found to be an indication of an improved intrapersonal competency when first-year students experienced a different MBI (Huerta et al. Reference Huerta, Carberry, Pipe and McKenna2021). Prior research has also found that practicing meditation can induce a relaxation response (Mohan et al. Reference Mohan, Sharma and Bijlani2011), which aligns with the experiences of students who described increased relaxation and less perceived stress due to the MBM breaks (i.e., the breaks causing a positive change in emotion). Students in the MBI were more likely to report positive experiences during the breaks than students in the control condition.

Students who had a negative experience with the breaks often described the breaks as negatively interrupting their work or focus, having increased negative emotions due to their work being interrupted, and the breaks wasting time they could be using for design. A similar number of students in both conditions reported having these negative experiences. The intervention breaks in this study were prescheduled and, therefore, may have occurred at inconvenient points in the design process. Students likely would have had better experiences with the breaks if they occurred at natural stopping points in the design process. However, many students reported that they did not need a break during the design challenge because 2 hours was a manageable duration for them to focus. Alternatively, while task-switching has previously been shown to reduce design fixation and improve performance (Sio et al. Reference Sio, Kotovsky and Cagan2017), it may be too difficult for first-year engineers. Instructors could consider allowing students to choose when to take their breaks or make the breaks optional to encourage a better student experience.

Students who described having a positive experience with the breaks frequently reported them as being a positive distraction from the design challenge or experiencing an increase in positive emotion or emotional state due to the break. A couple of students also reported that the breaks helped them manage their design time. Positive emotional experiences were more common among students in the MBI than students in the control group. Positive emotional experiences for students in the MBI were often described as leading to less stress and more relaxation, which likely indicates that the MBMs did help some students to manage their stress. This aligns with previous research, which found that practicing meditation before playing stressful tasks like videogames can reduce the stress response to the task (Mohan et al. Reference Mohan, Sharma and Bijlani2011). Alternatively, students in the MBI condition may have more positive emotional experiences because they were familiar with the MBM practices from their course, while students in the control condition had less experience with the AUTs. To preserve the effects experienced by some students, instructors could consider incorporating an MBM before beginning an activity like the design challenge.

Some students also reported the breaks as an opportunity for thinking and reflection. Students typically reported that their thinking was related to the design process, including their progress up to this point and planning for how they will continue the process going forward. Schön (Reference Schön1983) posits that reflective practice is an essential skill for professionals and argues that engineers will have to develop reflection-in-action skills or the ability to learn and correct during the process. While reflective practice is typically incorporated in engineering using reflective essays (e.g., Turns et al. Reference Turns, Newstetter, Allen and Mistree1997), these breaks may provide better opportunities for engineers to be reflective during the design process.

Lastly, several students stated that the breaks improved their creativity. As described above, while some students might have truly experienced an improvement in their creativity due to the breaks, it is also possible that these students set open goals for creativity that were fulfilled by the break activities. Prior research has determined that some MBMs can be practiced to support creativity (Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Richardson and Shack2020), but alternative break activities to encourage creativity should also be explored and tested for design.

6.5. Limitations and recommendations for future research

This study had many promising, interesting and significant results (summary in Table 3). For additional recommendations for implementing mindfulness-based practices into engineering courses and an analysis of qualitative data focused on mindfulness-based practice in introductory engineering design courses, please see Nolte et al. (Reference Nolte, Soria Zurita, Starkey and McComb2022b). A principal limitation of the current non-randomized study was the power, sample size and participant diversity (i.e., race/ethnic diversity). This study should be replicated at an institution with more diversity to investigate if this MBI impacted minoritized students uniquely. Another limitation is that the MBMs used for this intervention are from one program. While this makes it easy for instructors to implement these practices, it is unknown if these results are unique to this program. Additionally, the MBI was only implemented by one of the two course instructors in this study. However, all four course sections utilized the same core instructional materials, a unique teaching assistant was responsible for delivering a portion of the instruction to each course section and prior research has only identified minimal effects due to the instructor (Nolte & McComb Reference Nolte and McComb2021; Nolte et al. Reference Nolte, Huff and McComb2022a). Therefore, while it is unlikely that the results of this study are due to the instructor, this study should be replicated with more course sections and instructors to ensure that none of the results are dependent on the instructor. Lastly, this data was collected during the first semester of in-person classes after the classes were moved online due to the COVID-19 pandemic. It is unknown how this may have influenced the data in this study.

Table 3. Recommendations to instructors based on the principal results of this study

Future research should be conducted to continue understanding students’ stress during design and explore how MBIs uniquely affect engineering students. Future work investigating mindfulness in engineering should use various methods for measuring mindfulness as the quantitative and qualitative mindfulness results in this study were often incongruent. Measures including neuroimaging, physiological measures or behavioral tests may be more indicative of true changes due to MBIs (Tang & Posner Reference Tang and Posner2013). Additionally, the results of this study could be used to create alternative interventions to help students during design. Promising interventions could include directly targeting challenging design stages or increasing students’ self-efficacy through additional hands-on experiences. Lastly, this study identified a relationship between students’ state mindfulness and the creativity of their final designs. These results suggest a novel line of research in engineering investigating the effect of MBIs on design creativity, which should be further explored. For example, future research should examine the creativity of all brainstormed designs as prior work has shown that the most creative designs are not always chosen as the final design (Starkey et al. Reference Starkey, Toh and Miller2016; Zheng & Miller Reference Zheng and Miller2018).

7. Conclusion

This study investigated the effect of an MBI on first-year engineering students’ stress and final design creativity during a multistage, hands-on design assessment. Data were collected using a mixed methods approach. While no significant increase was found in students’ measured state mindfulness due to the MBI, state mindfulness was a significant contributor to students’ experience during the design challenge. Students’ interview responses indicated that they perceived an impact on their stress during the design challenge due to the breaks; however, no statistical differences were found in students’ stress scores by condition. Students in the MBI condition were found to have higher-quality final designs, and high and low state mindfulness were found to have a relationship with highly creative final designs. Future research should continue to investigate MBIs in engineering design to improve design education and design outcomes.

Financial Support

The first author’s research assistantship is supported by the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency through cooperative agreement N66001-17-4064. Any opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the sponsor.

A. Appendix. Semi-structured interview questions

  1. 1. Can you briefly walk me through your design process during the in-class portion of the super quiz? (How did you develop a solution to the design prompt?)

  2. 2. What was your level of confidence while participating in the in-class portion of the super quiz?

    1. a. Were there any aspects of the in-class portion of the super quiz that you were highly confident about? Why?

    2. b. Were there aspects of the in-class portion of the super quiz that you were not so confident about? Why?

  3. 3. What parts of the in-class portion of the super quiz were easy? Why?

    What parts of the in-class portion of the super quiz were hard? Why?

  4. 4. I would like you to think back to the way you felt during the in-class portion of the super quiz. What emotions did you experience?

    1. a. Did you experience any positive emotions during the super quiz? If so, what caused those emotions?

    2. b. Did you experience any negative emotions during the super quiz? If so, what caused those emotions?

    3. c. Were you worried during the super quiz, or did you experience anxiety? If so what were the sources of your worry or anxiety?

    4. d. Did you find the super quiz to be stressful? If so, what was stressful about the super quiz?

    5. e. How well do you think you were able to regulate your emotions during the super quiz? Why?

  5. 5. How would you define your ideal level of stress when completing a design task like the super quiz? Why do you think this is the best level of stress?

  6. 6. Would you describe yourself as someone who thrives under pressure? Why or why not?

    1. a. How do you think this impacted your design process or experience during the super quiz?

  7. 7. What were your feelings about the scheduled breaks during the in-class portion of the super quiz? Why?

    1. a. Were there other activities you would have rather liked to do during the 5-minute breaks? Why?

    2. b. Were there aspects of the breaks you found to be either positive or negative? Why?

  8. 8. Do you think these scheduled breaks impacted your design process? How?

  9. 9. If you were to do a design project like the super quiz again in the future, would you schedule breaks? Why or why not?

  10. 10. [Mindfulness] I would like to talk about the daily mindfulness exercises that you have been completing in-class since the beginning of the semester. How did you feel about the daily mindfulness practices?

    1. a. [Mindfulness] What were the positive or negative aspects of these practices? Why?

    2. b. [Mindfulness] Do you think practicing mindfulness had any impact on your engineering abilities? If so, can you describe this impact?

    3. c. [Mindfulness] How would you change these daily practices for other classes if you were to do them again in the future?

    4. d. [Mindfulness] Will you continue to practice mindfulness after this class? Why or why not?

  11. 11. Would you describe engineering design as an inherently difficult or challenging topic? Why do you think this?

    1. a. Why do you think engineering design is perceived this way by others? OR Why do you think others might perceive engineering design to be difficult or challenging?

    2. b. What are the positives or negatives of this perception?

    3. c. How well do you think you are able to manage the level of difficulty or challenge in engineering (generally)?

  12. 12. Do you currently practice any methods for managing stress? What methods and how often do you use them?

    1. a. When did you learn that these methods were effective for managing your stress?

    2. b. Would you be interested in learning more methods for managing stress? Are you interested in any specific methods?

  13. 13. Are there other questions you think we should be asking?

  14. 14. Are there any other experiences you had during the super quiz that you would like to share? This could include the in-class or out-of-class portion of the super quiz.

References

Alhashim, A., Marshall, M., Hartog, T., Jonczyk, R., Dickson, D., van Hell, J., Okudan-Kremer, G. & Siddique, Z. 2020 Work in progress: Assessing creativity of alternative uses task responses: A detailed procedure. In ASEE Virtual Annual Conference, pp. 115. American Society of Engineering Education.Google Scholar
Amabil, T. M. 1983 Brilliant but cruel: Perceptions of negative evaluators. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 19 (2), 146156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. 1982 Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43 (5), 9971013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. 1996 Creativity in Context (Vol. 1). Westview Press.Google Scholar
American College Health Association 2020 American College Health Association-National College Health Assessment III: Reference Group Executive Summary Fall 2020, pp. 122. American College Health Association.Google Scholar
Baas, M., Nevicka, B. & Ten Velden, F. S. 2014 Specific mindfulness skills differentially predict creative performance. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 40 (9), 10921106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baer, J. & Kaufman, J. C. 2019 Assessing creativity with the consensual assessment technique. In The Palgrave Handbook of Social Creativity Research (ed. Lebuda, I. & Glăveanu, V.), pp. 2737. Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamber, M. D. & Kraenzle Schneider, J. 2016 Mindfulness-based meditation to decrease stress and anxiety in college students: A narrative synthesis of the research. Educational Research Review 18, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beddoes, K. & Danowitz, A. 2021 Engineering students coping with COVID-19: Yoga, meditation, and mental health. In 2021 ASEE Annual Conference, Virtual Conference, pp. 113. American Society of Engineering Education.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L. & DeCaro, M. S. 2007 From poor performance to success under stress: Working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 33 (6), 983998.Google ScholarPubMed
Beilock, S. L., Kulp, C. A., Holt, L. E. & Carr, T. H. 2004 More on the fragility of performance: Choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology 133 (4), 584600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellinger, D. B., DeCaro, M. S. & Ralston, P. A. S. 2015 Mindfulness, anxiety, and high-stakes mathematics performance in the laboratory and classroom. Consciousness and Cognition 37, 123132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernárdez, B., Durán, A., Parejo, J. A. & Ruiz-Cortés, A. 2014 A controlled experiment to evaluate the effects of mindfulness in software engineering. In International Symposium on Empirical Software Engineering and Measurement, pp. 110. IEEE Computer Society.Google Scholar
Bernárdez, B., Durán, A., Parejo, J. A. & Ruiz-Cortés, A. 2018 An experimental replication on the effect of the practice of mindfulness in conceptual modeling performance. Journal of Systems and Software 136, 153172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besemer, S. P. 1998 Creative product analysis matrix: Testing the model structure and a comparison among products – Three novel chairs. Creativity Research Journal 11 (4), 333346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besemer, S. P. & O’Quin, K. 1999 Confirming the three-factor creative product analysis matrix model in an American sample. Creativity Research Journal 12 (4), 287296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D. & Devins, G. 2004 Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 11 (3), 230241.Google Scholar
Brown, K. W. & Ryan, R. M. 2003 The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 (4), 822848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byron, K., Khazanchi, S. & Nazarian, D. 2010 The relationship between stressors and creativity: A meta-analysis examining competing theoretical models. Journal of Applied Psychology 95 (1), 201212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charney, D. 1996 Empiricism is not a four-letter word. College Composition and Communication 47 (4), 567593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Y. C., Chang, Y. S. & Chuang, M. J. 2022 Virtual reality application influences cognitive load-mediated creativity components and creative performance in engineering design. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 38 (1), 618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiu, I. & Salustri, F. A. 2010 Evaluating design project creativity in engineering design courses. In CEEA Conference, pp. 16. Queen’s University Library.Google Scholar
Conley, C. S., Kirsch, A. C., Dickson, D. A. & Bryant, F. B. 2014 Negotiating the transition to college: Developmental trajectories and gender differences in psychological functioning, cognitive-affective strategies, and social well-being. Emerging Adulthood 2 (3), 195210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, M. M. 1997 Distinguishing critical and post-positivist research. College Composition and Communication 48 (4), 556561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creswell, J. D. 2017 Mindfulness interventions. Annual Review of Psychology 68, 491516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creswell, J. D., Lindsay, E. K., Villalba, D. K. & Chin, B. 2019 Mindfulness training and physical health: Mechanisms and outcomes. Psychosomatic Medicine 81 (3), 224232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, C. J., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D. & Davidson, R. J. 2020 The plasticity of well-being: A training-based framework for the cultivation of human flourishing. National Academy of Sciences 117 (51), 3219732206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degroote, C., Schwaninger, A., Heimgartner, N., Hedinger, P., Ehlert, U. & Wirtz, P. H. 2020 Acute stress improves concentration performance: Opposite effects of anxiety and cortisol. Experimental Psychology 67 (2), 8898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denson, C. D., Buelin, J. K., Lammi, M. D. & D’Amico, S. 2015 Developing instrumentation for assessing creativity in engineering design. Journal of Technology Education 27 (1), 2340.Google Scholar
Dvořáková, K., Kishida, M., Li, J., Elavsky, S., Broderick, P. C., Agrusti, M. R. & Greenberg, M. T. 2017 Promoting healthy transition to college through mindfulness training with first-year college students: Pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of American College Health 65 (4), 259267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dym, C. L., Agogino, A. M., Eris, O., Frey, D. D. & Leifer, L. J. 2005 Engineering design thinking, teaching, and learning. Journal of Engineering Education 94 (1), 103120; doi:10.1109/EMR.2006.1679078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estrada, T. & Dalton, E. 2019 Impact of student mindfulness facets on engineering education outcomes: An initial exploration. In ASEE Annual Conference, pp. 117. American Society of Engineering Education.Google Scholar
Foster, C. & Spencer, L. 2003 Are undergraduate engineering students at greater risk for heart disease than other undergraduate students?. Journal of Engineering Education 92 (1), 7377; doi:10.1002/j.2168-9830.2003.tb00740.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-García, C., Chulvi, V., Royo, M., Gual, J. & Felip, F. 2019 Does the work environment affect designers’ creativity during the creative phase depending on their personality profile?. Thinking Skills and Creativity 33, 100578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ge, J. S., Berger, E. J., Major, J. C. & Froiland, J. M. 2019 Teaching undergraduate engineering students gratitude, meaning, and mindfulness. In ASEE Annual Conference and Exposition, American Society for Engineering Education, pp. 17.Google Scholar
Goldberg, S. B., Imhoff-Smith, T., Bolt, D. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Dahl, C. J., Davidson, R. J. & Rosenkranz, M. A. 2020 Testing the efficacy of a multicomponent, self-guided, smartphone-based meditation app: Three-armed randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mental Health 7 (11), e23825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, J., Reiner, K. & Meiran, N. 2012 ‘Mind the trap’: Mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. PLoS One 7 (5), e36206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guba, E. & Lincoln, Y. 1994 Handbook of Qualitative Research (ed. Denzin, N. K. & Lincoln, Y. S.). Sage.Google Scholar
Hart, S. G. & Staveland, L. E. 1988 Development of NASA-TLX (task load index): Results of emperical and theoretical research. Advances in Psychology 52 (C), 139183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, S. G. & Staveland, L. E. 2006 NASA-task load index (NASA-TLX): 20 years later. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 50 (9), 904908; doi:10.1177/154193120605000909CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healthy Minds Innovations 2021 Healthy Minds Program App, online document (downloadable on November 22nd 2021) https://hminnovations.org/Google Scholar
Helton, W. S. 2004 Validation of a Short Stress State Questionnaire. Human Factors and Ergonomic Society 48, 12381242.Google Scholar
Helton, W. S. & Nöswall, K. 2010 Short stress state questionnaire: Factor structure and state change assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 31 (1), 2030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henriksen, D., Richardson, C. & Shack, K. 2020 Mindfulness and creativity: Implications for thinking and learning. Thinking Skills and Creativity 37, 100689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hove, S. E. & Anda, B. 2005 Experiences from conducting semi-structured interviews in empirical software engineering research. In International Software Metrics Symposium (Vol. 2005), pp. 1023. IEEE Computer Society.Google Scholar
Hsieh, H. F. & Shannon, S. E. 2005 Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qualitative Health Research 15 (9), 12771288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huerta, M. 2018 Inner engineering: A convergent mixed methods study evaluating the use of contemplative practices to promote resilience among freshman engineering students. In ASEE Annual Conference and Exposition, pp. 110. ASEE.Google Scholar
Huerta, M. V., Carberry, A. R., Pipe, T. & McKenna, A. F. 2021 Inner engineering: Evaluating the utility of mindfulness training to cultivate intrapersonal and interpersonal competencies among first-year engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education 110 (3), 636670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jansson, D. G. & Smith, S. M. 1991 Design fixation. Design Studies 12 (1), 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kabat-Zinn, J. 1982 An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry 4 (1), 3347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kabat-Zinn, J. 2003 Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 10 (2), 144156.Google Scholar
Ketchen, D. J. & Shook, C. L. 1996 The application of cluster analysis in ecnomics science. Strategic Management Journal 17 (6), 441458.Google Scholar
Khoury, B., Sharma, M., Rush, S. E. & Fournier, C. 2015 Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 78 (6), 519528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lal, R., Pathak, P., Chaturvedi, K. R. & Talukdar, P. 2019 Effect of dispositional mindfulness on perceived stress scores of engineering students: An empirical study. Indian Journal of Public Health Research and Development 10 (1), 6368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., Shapiro, S., Carmody, J., Abbey, S. & Devins, G. 2006 The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Psychology 62 (12), 14451467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebuda, I., Zabelina, D. L. & Karwowski, M. 2016 Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulness–creativity link. Personality and Individual Differences 93, 2226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahmood, L., Hopthrow, T. & Randsley de Moura, G. 2016 A moment of mindfulness: Computer-mediated mindfulness practice increases state mindfulness . PLOS One 11 (4), e0153923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makwana, P., Kodinariya, T. M. & Makwana, P. R. 2013 Review on determining number of cluster in k-means clustering. International Journal of Advance Research in Computer Science and Management Studies 1 (6), 9095.Google Scholar
McHugh, M. L. 2012 Interrater reliability: The kappa statistic. Biochemia Medica 22 (3), 276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, S. R., Hunter, S. T., Starkey, E., Ramachandran, S., Ahmed, F. & Fuge, M. 2020 How should we measure creativity in design studies? A comparison of social science and engineering approaches. In Proceedings of the ASME Design Engineering Technical Conference, American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection (Vol. 8). American Society of Mechanical Engineers; doi:10.1115/DETC2020-22446Google Scholar
Miller, S. R., Hunter, S. T., Starkey, E., Ramachandran, S., Ahmed, F. & Fuge, M. 2021 How should we measure creativity in engineering design? A comparison between social science and engineering approaches. Journal of Mechanical Design 143 (3), 031404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, I. & Jensen, K. 2020 Introduction of mindfulness in an online engineering core course during the COVID-19 pandemic. Advances in Engineering Education 8 (4), 17.Google Scholar
Mohan, A., Sharma, R. & Bijlani, R. L. 2011 Effect of meditation on stress-induced changes in cognitive functions. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 17 (3), 207212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, J., Kotovsky, K. & Cagan, J. 2007 Understanding the role of open goals in problem solving: Impasses and hints. In Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, p. 1817. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. A., Xu, X. & Zeng, Y. 2013 Distribution of mental stresses during conceptual design activities. In International Conference on Engineering Design, pp. 287296. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2012 A theoretical model of design creativity: Nonlinear design dynamics and mental stress-creativity relation. Transactions of the SDPS: Journal of Integrated Design and Process Science 16 (3), 6588.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2014 A physiological study of relationship between designer’s mental effort and mental stress during conceptual design. Computer-Aided Design 54, 318; doi:10.1016/J.CAD.2013.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2017a Effects of stress and effort on self-rated reports in experimental study of design activities. Journal of Intelligent Manufacturing 28 (7), 16091622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2017b A theoretical model of design fixation. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation 5 (3–4), 185204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolte, H., Huff, J. & McComb, C. 2022a No time for that? An investigation of mindfulness and stress in first-year engineering design. Design Science 8, E9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolte, H. & McComb, C. 2020 Identifying stress signatures across the engineering design process: Perceived stress during concept generation, concept selection, and prototyping. In Design Society: Design Conference, pp. 15051514. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nolte, H. & McComb, C. 2021 The cognitive experience of engineering design: An examination of first-year student stress across principal activities of the engineering design process. Design Science 7, E3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolte, H., Soria Zurita, N. F., Starkey, E. & McComb, C. 2022b Student and instructor reflections on integrating short mindfulness-based meditation practices into a first-year engineering design course. In First-Year Engineering Experience Conference, pp. 17. American Society of Engineering Education.Google Scholar
Norris, C. J., Creem, D., Hendler, R. & Kober, H. 2018 Brief mindfulness meditation improves attention in novices: Evidence from ERPs and moderation by neuroticism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12 (315), 120.Google ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, J. J. M. & Barclay, P. 2017 The influence of voice pitch on perceptions of trustworthiness across social contexts. Evolution and Human Behavior 38 (4), 506512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petkar, H., Dande, S., Yadav, R., Zeng, Y. & Nguyen, T. A. 2009 A pilot study to assess designer’s mental stress using eye gaze system and electroencephalogram. In ASME Design Engineering Technical Conference (Vol. 2), pp. 899909. American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection.Google Scholar
Renshaw, T. L. 2019 Mindfulness-based intervention in schools. In Promoting Mind–Body Health in Schools: Interventions for Mental Health Professionals, pp. 145160. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Rieken, B., Schar, M., Shapiro, S. & Sheppard, S. 2017 Exploring the relationship between mindfulness and innovation in engineering students. In American Society for Engineering Education Annual Conference, p. 19515. Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers.Google Scholar
Rieken, B., Schar, M. & Sheppard, S. 2016 Trait mindfulness in an engineering classroom: An exploration of the relationship between mindfulness, academic skills, and professional skills. In Frontiers in Education Conference, pp. 18. Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers Inc.Google Scholar
Rousseeuw, P. J. 1987 Silhouettes: A graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. Journal of Computational and Applied Mathematics 20, 5365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, H. & Rubin, I. 2012 Qualitative Interviewing: The Art of Hearing Data. Sage; doi:10.4135/9781452226651Google Scholar
Sandi, C. 2013 Stress and cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 4 (3), 245261.Google ScholarPubMed
Sarkar, P. & Chakrabarti, A. 2011 Assessing design creativity. Design Studies 32 (4), 348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schön, D. A. 1983 The Reflective Practitioner: How Professionals Think in Action. Basic Books; doi:10.1177/14733250090080010802Google Scholar
Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G. & Flinders, T. 2008 Cultivating mindfulness: Effects on well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology 64 (7), 840862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shraddha, S. & Naganna, S. 2014 A review on k-means data clustering approach. International Journal of Information & Computation Technology 4 (17), 18471860.Google Scholar
Sio, U. N., Kotovsky, K. & Cagan, J. 2017 The facilitating role of task alternation on group idea generation. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 6 (4), 486495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starkey, E., Toh, C. A. & Miller, S. R. 2016 Abandoning creativity: The evolution of creative ideas in engineering design course projects. Design Studies 47, 4772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, Y.-Y. & Posner, M. I. 2013 Tools of the trade: Theory and method in mindfulness neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1), 118120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, Y. & Zeng, Y. 2009 Quantifying designer’s mental stress during the conceptual design process using kinesics study. In International Conference on Engineering Design, pp. 211219. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Toh, C. A. & Miller, S. R. 2016 Choosing creativity: The role of individual risk and ambiguity aversion on creative concept selection in engineering design. Research in Engineering Design 27 (3), 195219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. 1966 Torrance tests of creative thinking. In Norms-Technical Manual Research. Personnel Press.Google Scholar
Turns, J., Newstetter, W., Allen, J. K. & Mistree, F. 1997 Learning essays and the reflective learner: Supporting reflection in engineering design education. In ASEE Annual Conference Proceedings, pp. 114. ASEE.Google Scholar
Wang, X., Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2015 Influence of information collection strategy in problem formulation on design creativity through mental stress: A theoretical analysis. In International Conference on Engineering Design, pp. 91100; doi:10.1017/dsi.2019.184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, D., Liu, S. & Zeng, Y. 2018 Mental stress-performance model. Emotional Engineering 6, 119139.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. & Dodson, J. D. 1908 The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology 18 (5), 459482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z. & Goolkasian, P. 2010 Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition 19 (2), 597605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, X. & Miller, S. R. 2018 Uncovering ownership bias: The influence of idea goodness and creativity on design professionals’ concept selection practices. In ASME Design Engineering Technical Conference, American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection (Vol. 7) American Society of Mechanical Engineers; doi:10.1115/DETC2018-85964Google Scholar
Zhu, S., Yao, S. & Zeng, Y. 2007 A novel approach to quantifying designer’s mental stress in the conceptual design process. In International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference, pp. 593600. American Society of Mechanical Engineers Digital Collection.Google Scholar
Zhao, M. & Zeng, Y. 2019 Influence of information collection strategy on designer’s mental stress. In International Conference on Engineering Design (Vol. 1), pp. 17831792; doi:10.1017/dsi.2019.184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Experimental methodology for the design challenge

Figure 1

Table 2. Survey measures

Figure 2

Figure 1. Generalized experimental procedure.

Figure 3

Figure 2. TMS scores for the MBI and the control condition.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Comparison of SSSQ change scores.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Modified NASA-RTLX scores compared by condition. Error bars represent ± 1 standard error.

Figure 6

Figure 5. Creativity scores by condition. The asterisk on Plot C indicates a significant difference.

Figure 7

Figure 6. Quantitative data by cluster.

Figure 8

Table 3. Recommendations to instructors based on the principal results of this study