Hostname: page-component-588bc86c8c-schgg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2023-12-01T03:45:06.773Z Has data issue: false Feature Flags: { "corePageComponentGetUserInfoFromSharedSession": true, "coreDisableEcommerce": false, "useRatesEcommerce": true } hasContentIssue false

Development of fear and guilt in young children: Stability over time and relations with psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 July 2012

Erika Baker
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Eugenia Baibazarova
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Georgia Ktistaki
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Katherine H. Shelton
Affiliation:
Cardiff University
Stephanie H. M. van Goozen*
Affiliation:
Cardiff University Leiden University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Stephanie van Goozen, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, Park Place, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK; E-mail: vangoozens@cardiff.ac.uk.

Abstract

Extremes in fearful temperament have long been associated with later psychopathology and risk pathways. Whereas fearful children are inhibited and anxious and avoid novel events, fearless individuals are disinhibited and more likely to engage in aggressive behavior. However, very few studies have examined fear in infants from a multimethod and prospective longitudinal perspective. This study had the following objectives: to examine behavioral, maternal reported, and physiological indices of fearful temperament in infancy, together with their relations and stability over time; and to establish whether early indices of fear predict fear later in toddlerhood. We also examined the association between behavioral and physiological measures of fear and guilt and whether fear in infancy predicts guilt in toddlers. Finally, we investigated infant risk factors for later psychopathology. We recorded skin conductance level (SCL) and heart rate (HR) and observed children's responses during a Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery fear paradigm across the first 3 years of life and during a guilt induction procedure at age 3 (N = 70). The results indicate that different measures of infant fear were associated across time. Observed fearlessness in infancy predicted observed fearlessness and low levels of SCL arousal to fear and guilt in toddlers. Low levels of HR and SCL to fear in infancy predicted low levels of physiological arousal to the same situation and to guilt 2 years later. Fear and guilt were significantly associated across measures. Finally, toddlers with clinically significant internalizing problems at age 3 were already notably more fearful in Year 1 as reflected by their significantly higher HR levels. The results indicated that assessments of children in infancy are predictive of how these children react 2 years later and therefore lend support to the idea that the emotional thermostat is set in the first 3 years of life. They also showed, for the first time, that infant fear is a predictor of guilt, which is an emotion that develops later. The implications of these findings for our understanding of developmental psychopathology are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Watrers, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barker, E. D., Oliver, B. R., Viding, E., Salekin, R. T., & Maughan, B. (2011). The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on adolescent callous–unemotional traits: A 14-year longitudinal investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 878888.Google Scholar
Barrett, K. C. (1995). The origins of guilt in early childhood. In Bybee, J. (Ed.), Guilt and children (pp. 7590). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, K. C., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Cole, P. M. (1993). Avoiders vs. amenders: Implications for the investigation of guilt and shame during toddlerhood? Cognition & Emotion, 7, 481505.Google Scholar
Bauer, A. M., Quas, J. A., & Boyce, W. T. (2002). Associations between physiological reactivity and children's behavior: Advantages of a multisystem approach. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23, 102113.Google Scholar
Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Bolduc-Murphy, E. A., Faraone, S. V., Chaloff, J., Hirshfield, M. A., et al. (1993). A 3-year follow-up of children with and without behavioral inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 814821.Google Scholar
Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Hirshfield, D. R., & Faraone, S. V. (1990). Psychiatric correlates of behavioral inhibition in young children of parents with and without psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 2126.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (1997). Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 731739.Google Scholar
Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O'Brien, M. (2010). Contributions of child's physiology and maternal behavior to children's trajectories of temperamental reactivity. Developmental Psychology, 46, 10891102.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Burgess, K. B., Marshall, P. J., Rubin, K. H., & Fox, N. A. (2003). Infant attachment and temperament as predictors of subsequent externalizing problems and cardiac physiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 819831.Google Scholar
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Buss, K. A., Goldsmith, H. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Cardiac reactivity is associated with changes in negative emotion in 24-month-olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 118132.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Dedmon, S. E. (2000). Physiological and behavioral regulation in two-year-old children with aggressive/destructive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 103127.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J. (1976). Heart rate: A sensitive tool for the study of emotional development in the infant. In Lipsitt, L. P. (Ed.), Developmental psychobiology: The significance of infancy (pp. 134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2008). Integrating biological measures into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 737743.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 50, 1625.Google Scholar
Colder, C. R., Mott, J. A., & Berman, A. S. (2002). The interactive effects of infant activity level and fear on growth trajectories of early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 123.Google Scholar
Cole, P. M., Barrett, K. C., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Emotion displays in two-year-olds during mishaps. Child Development, 63, 314324.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 351, 14131420.Google Scholar
Denham, S., & Weissberg, R. P. (2004). Social–emotional learning in early childhood: What we know and where to go from here? In Chesebrough, E., King, P., Bloom, M., & Gulotta, T. P. (Eds.), A blueprint for prosocial behavior in early childhood. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Early temperament and emotional development. In Kalverboer, A. F. & Gramsbergen, A. (Eds.), Handbook of brain and behavior in human development (pp. 967988). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. (1984). The role of emotion in moral socialization. In Izard, C., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. B. (Eds.), Emotions, cognitions and behavior (pp. 484513). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Edelberg, R. (1967). Methods in psychophysiology. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Ferguson, T. J., Stegge, H., & Damhuis, I. (1991). Children's understanding of guilt and shame. Child Development, 62, 827839.Google Scholar
Ferguson, T. J., Stegge, H., Miller, E. R., & Olsen, M. E. (1999). Guilt, shame, and symptoms in children. Developmental Psychology, 35, 347357.Google Scholar
Fowles, D. C. (1994). A motivational theory of psychopathology. In Spaulding, W. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 41. Integrated views of motivation and emotion (pp. 181228). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Fowles, D. C., & Kochanska, G. (2000). Temperament as a moderator of pathways to conscience in children: The contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 37, 788795.Google Scholar
Fowles, D. C., Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. (2000). Electrodermal activity and temperament in preschool children. Psychophysiology, 37, 777787.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A. (1989). Psychophysiological correlates of emotional reactivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 25, 364372.Google Scholar
Fracasso, M. P., Porges, S. W., Lamb, M. E., & Rosenberg, A. A. (1994). Cardiac activity in infancy: Reliability and stability of individual differences. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 277284.Google Scholar
Frick, P. J. (2004). Integrating research on temperament and childhood psychopathology: Its pitfalls and promise. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 27.Google Scholar
Gao, Y., Raine, A., Venables, P. H., Dawson, M. E., & Mednick, S. A. (2010). Association of poor childhood fear conditioning and adult crime. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 5660.Google Scholar
Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infants temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 26, 6486.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1999). The Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery; Description of procedures. Locomotor version. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harder, D. W. (1995). Shame and guilt assessment, and relationships of shame- and guilt-proneness to psychopathology. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W. (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions the psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 368392). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hernes, K. G., Mørkrid, L., Fremming, A., Ødegården, S., Martinsen, Ø. G., & Storm, H. (2002). Skin conductance changes during the first year of life in full-term infants. Pediatric Research, 52, 837843.Google Scholar
Hirshfield, D. R., Biederman, J., Brody, L., Faraone, S. V., & Rosenbaum, F. (1997). Expressed emotion toward children with behavioral inhibition: Association with maternal anxiety disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 910917.Google Scholar
Hirshfield-Becker, D. R., Biederman, J., Calltharp, S., Rosenbaum, E. D., Faraone, S. V., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (2003). Behavioral inhibition and disinhibition as hypothesized precursors to psychopathology: Implications for paediatric bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 985999.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral internalization. In Higgins, E. T., Ruble, D., & Hartup, W. (Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A socio-cultural perspective (pp. 236274). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1992). Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion–cognition relations. Psychological Review, 99, 561566.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1982). Heart rate and heart rate variability as signs of temperamental dimension in infancy. In Izard, C. E. (Ed.), Measuring emotions in infants and children (pp. 3866). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen's prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child Development, 68, 139143.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1998). Biology and the child. In Damon, W. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 117235). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 22122225.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 14591473.Google Scholar
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 51, 706716.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the development of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62, 13791392.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialisation and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 325347.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (2001). Emotional development in children with different attachment histories: The first three years. Child Development, 72, 474490.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Aksan, N., & Boldt, L. J. (2008). A developmental model of maternal and child contributions to disruptive conduct: The first six years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 49, 12201227.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Jimenez, N. B., Hollatz, A. L., & Woodard, J. (2009). Guilt and effortful control: Two mechanisms that prevent disruptive developmental trajectories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 322333.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., Tjebkes, T. L., & Husarek, S. J. (1998). Individual differences in emotionality in infancy. Child Development, 69, 375390.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., DeVet, K., Goldman, M., Murray, K., & Putnam, S. P. (1994). Maternal reports of conscience development and temperament in young children. Child Development, 65, 852868.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Gross, J. N., Lin, M. H., & Nichols, K. E. (2002). Guilt in young children: Development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards. Child Development, 73, 461482.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1987). Emotion. In Plum, F. (Ed.), Handbook of physiology—The nervous system (Vol. 5, pp. 419459). Washington, DC: American Physiological Society.Google Scholar
Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament. Developmental Psychology, 35, 189204.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ramsey, D. S. (1999). Effect of maternal soothing on infant stress response. Child Development, 70, 1120.Google Scholar
Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing data (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley Interscience.Google Scholar
Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1997). Development of juvenile aggression and violence. Some common misconceptions and controversies. American Psychologist, 53, 242259.Google Scholar
Lovallo, W. R. (1997). Stress and health: Biological and psychological interactions. Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 395422.Google Scholar
O'Connor, L. E., Berry, J. W., Weiss, J., & Gilbert, P. (2002). Guilt, fear, submission, and empathy in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 71, 1927.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, M., Goldsmith, H. H., Davidson, R. J., & Rickman, M. (2002). Continuity and change in inhibited and uninhibited children. Child Development, 73, 14741485.Google Scholar
Provost, M. A., & Gouin-Decarie, T. (1979). Heart rate reactivity of 9- and 12-months old infants showing specific emotions in natural setting. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2, 109120.Google Scholar
PsyLab. (2007). PSYLAB program. Retrieved from http://www.psylab.comGoogle Scholar
Quay, H. C. (1965). Psychopathic personality as pathological stimulation seeking. American Journal of Psychiatry, 122, 180183.Google Scholar
Raine, A. (1993). The psychopathology of crime: Criminal behavior as a clinical disorder. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (1997). Low resting heart rate at age 3 years predisposes to aggression at age 11 years: Evidence from the Mauritius Child Health Project. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 14571464.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569578.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 40, 2139.Google Scholar
Scarpa, A., Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (1997). Heart rate and skin conductance in behaviorally inhibited Mauritian children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 182190.Google Scholar
Schwartz, C. E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 10081015.Google Scholar
Snoek, H., van Goozen, S. H. M., Matthys, W., Buitelaar, J. K., & van Engeland, H. (2004). Stress responsivity in children with externalizing behavior disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 389406.Google Scholar
Strelau, J. (1998). Temperament: A psychological perspective. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Stuewig, J., Tangney, J. P., Heigel, C., Harty, L., & McCloskey, L. (2010). Shaming, blaming, and maiming: Functional links among the moral emotions, externalization of blame, and aggression. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 91102.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tilghman-Osborne, C., Cole, D. A., & Felton, J. W. (2010). Definition and measurement of guilt: Implications for clinical research and practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 536546.Google Scholar
Van Bokhoven, I., Matthys, W., van Goozen, S. H. M., & van Engeland, H. (2005). Prediction of adolescent outcome in children with disruptive behavior disorders: A study of neurobiological, psychological and family factors. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 153163.Google Scholar
van Goozen, S. H. M., Fairchild, G., Snoek, H., & Harold, G. T. (2007). The evidence for a neurobiological model of childhood antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 149182.Google Scholar
van Goozen, S. H. M., Matthys, W., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Buitelaar, J. K., & van Engeland, H. (2000). Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and autonomic nervous system activity in disruptive children and matched controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 14381445.Google Scholar
Weiss, G., & Hechtman, L. (1993). Hyperactive children grown up: ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Kochanska, G., Krupnick, J., & McKnew, D. (1990). Patterns of guilt in children of depressed and well mothers. Developmental Psychology 26, 5159.Google Scholar