Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T01:30:34.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood temperament: Passive gene–environment correlation, gene–environment interaction, and the hidden importance of the family environment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2013

Kathryn Lemery-Chalfant*
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Karen Kao
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Gregory Swann
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
H. Hill Goldsmith
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin–Madison
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Kathryn Lemery-Chalfant, Department of Psychology, P.O. Box 871104, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104; E-mail: klemery@asu.edu.

Abstract

Biological parents pass on genotypes to their children, as well as provide home environments that correlate with their genotypes; thus, the association between the home environment and children's temperament can be genetically (i.e., passive gene–environment correlation) or environmentally mediated. Furthermore, family environments may suppress or facilitate the heritability of children's temperament (i.e., gene–environment interaction). The sample comprised 807 twin pairs (mean age = 7.93 years) from the longitudinal Wisconsin Twin Project. Important passive gene–environment correlations emerged, such that home environments were less chaotic for children with high effortful control, and this association was genetically mediated. Children with high extraversion/surgency experienced more chaotic home environments, and this correlation was also genetically mediated. In addition, heritability of children's temperament was moderated by home environments, such that effortful control and extraversion/surgency were more heritable in chaotic homes, and negative affectivity was more heritable under crowded or unsafe home conditions. Modeling multiple types of gene–environment interplay uncovered the complex role of genetic factors and the hidden importance of the family environment for children's temperament and development more generally.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, A., & Bartlett, D. (2001). Infant motor development and equipment use in the home. Child, Care, and Development, 27, 295306.Google Scholar
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.Google Scholar
Angold, A., Erkanli, A., Silberg, J., Eaves, L., & Costello, E. J. (2002). Depression scale scores in 8–17-year-olds: Effects of age and gender. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 10521063.Google Scholar
Baumrind, D. (1993). The average respectable environment is not good enough: A response to Scarr. Child Development, 64, 12991317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boivin, M., Pérusse, D., Dionne, G., Saysset, V., Zoccolillo, M., Tarabulsy, G. M., et al. (2005). The genetic–environmental etiology of parents' perceptions and self-assessed behaviours toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin and singleton sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 612630.Google Scholar
Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J. C., van Baal, G. C. M., & Koopmans, J. R. (1999). A religious upbringing reduces the influence of genetic factors on disinhibition: Evidence for interaction between genotype and environment on personality. Twin Research, 2, 115125.Google Scholar
Borkenau, P., Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Spinath, F. M. (2002). Similarity of childhood experiences and personality resemblance in monozygotic and dizygotic twins: A test of the equal environments assumption. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 261269.Google Scholar
Bouchard, T. Jr. (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits: A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148151.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. H. (1993). Children's home environments, health, behavior, and intervention efforts: A review using the home inventory as a maker measure. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 119, 439480.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. H. (2006). Home environment. In Watt, N., Ayoub, C., Bradley, R. H., Puma, J., & LaBoeuf, W. (Eds.), The crisis in youth mental health: Vol. 4. Early intervention programs and policies (pp. 89120). Westport, CT: Greenwood.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. (2005). Caring for children around the world: A view from HOME. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 468478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. (1994). Nature–nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568586.Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P. K., & Duncan, G. J. (1996). Ethnic differences in children's intelligence test scores: Role of economic deprivation, home environment, and maternal characteristics. Child Development, 67, 396408.Google Scholar
Button, T. M. M., Scourfield, J., Martin, N., Purcell, S., & McGuffin, P. (2005). Family dysfunction interacts with genes in the causation of antisocial symptoms. Behavior Genetics, 35, 115120.Google Scholar
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock: University of Arkansas Press.Google Scholar
Coldwell, J., Pike, A., & Dunn, J. (2006). Household chaos—Links with parenting and child behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 11161122.Google Scholar
Cronk, N. J., Slutske, W. S., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Reich, W., & Heath, A. C. (2002). Emotional and behavioral problems among female twins: An evaluation of the equal environments assumption. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 829837.Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., & O'Connor, T. G. (2000). Parent–child mutuality in early childhood: Two behavioral genetic studies. Developmental Psychology, 36, 561570.Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., & Petrill, S. A. (2004). Parent–child dyadic mutuality and child behavior problems: An investigation of gene–environment processes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 11711179.Google Scholar
D'Onofrio, B. M., Turkheimer, E., Emery, R. E., Slutske, W. S., & Martin, N. G. (2005). A genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 570586.Google Scholar
Dumas, J., Nissley, J., Nordstrom, A., Smith, E., Prinz, R., & Levine, D. (2005). Home chaos: Sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 93104.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T. L., Liew, J., Zhou, Q., Losoya, S. H., et al. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children's effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45, 9881008.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Developmental Psychopathology, 23, 728.Google Scholar
Evans, G. (2006). Child development and the physical environment. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 423451.Google Scholar
Evans, G., Gonnella, C., Marcynyszyn, L., Gentile, L., & Salpekar, N. (2005). The role of chaos in poverty and children's socioemotinoal adjustment. Psychological Science, 16, 560565.Google Scholar
Evans, G., Wells, N., & Moch, A. (2003). Housing and mental health: A review of the evidence and a methodological and conceptual critique. Journal of Social Issues, 59, 475500.Google Scholar
Forget-Dubois, N., Pérusse, D., Turecki, G., Girard, A., Billette, J.-M., Rouleau, G., et al. (2003). Diagnosing zygosity in infant twins: Physical similarity, genotyping, and chorionicity. Twin Research, 6, 479485.Google Scholar
Gagne, J. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2011). A longitudinal analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from 12 to 36 months of age. Developmental Science, 14, 112124.Google Scholar
Gagne, J. R., Van Hulle, C. A., Aksan, N., Essex, M. J., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2011). Deriving childhood temperament measures from emotion-eliciting behavioral episodes: Scale construction and initial validation. Psychological Assessment, 23, 337353.Google Scholar
Gjone, H., & Stevenson, J. (1997). A longitudinal twin study of temperament and behavior problems: Common genetic or environmental influences? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 14481456.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H. (1988). Human developmental behavioral genetics: Mapping the effects of genes and environments. Annals of Child Development, 5, 187227.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H. (1991). A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: A research note. Behavior Genetics, 21, 257269.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, K. A., & Lemery, K. S. (1997). Toddler and childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Developmental Psychology, 33, 891905.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, K. A., & Campos, J. J. (1999). Genetic analyses of focal aspects of infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 35, 972985.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, H. H., Reilly, J., Lemery, K. S., Longley, S., & Prescott, A. (1993). Preliminary manual for the Preschool Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Technical Report Version 1.0). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin–Madison, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hur, Y. (2009). Genetic and environmental contributions to childhood temperament in South Korean Twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12, 549554.Google Scholar
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 4455.Google Scholar
Johnson, W. (2007). Genetic and environmental influences on behavior: Capturing all the interplay. Psychological Review, 114, 423440.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Baker, J. H. (2007). Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 37, 615626.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Karkowski-Shuman, L. (1997). Stressful life events and genetic liability to major depression: Genetic control of exposure to the environment. Psychological Medicine, 27, 539547.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., Prescott, C. A., Kessler, R. C., Heath, A. C., Corey, L. A., et al. (1996). Childhood parental loss and alcoholism in women: A causal analysis using a twin-family design. Psychological Medicine, 26, 7995.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Pederson, N. L. (2000). Tobacco consumption in Swedish twins reared apart and reared together. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 886892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R., South, S., Johnson, W., & Iacono, W. (2008). The heritability of personality is not always 50%: Gene–environment interactions and correlations between personality and parenting. Journal of Personality, 76, 14851521.Google Scholar
Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2010). How genes and environments work together to promote resilience. In Reich, J., Zautra, A. J., & Hall, J. S. (Eds.), Handbook of adult resilience. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lemery-Chalfant, K., Clifford, S., & Swann, G. (in press). Temperament and child psychopathology: Specificity in shared genetic effects. In Kim, Y. (Series Ed.) & Saudino, K. & Ganiban, J. (Vol. Eds.). Behavior genetics book series: Behavior genetics of temperament and personality. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Lemery-Chalfant, K., Doelger, L., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2008). Genetic relations between effortful and attentional control and symptoms of psychopathology in middle childhood. Infant and Child Development, 17, 365385.Google Scholar
Lemery-Chalfant, K., Goldsmith, H. H., Schmidt, N. L., Arneson, C. L., & Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Wisconsin Twin Project: Current directions and findings. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 10301037.Google Scholar
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 309337.Google Scholar
Lichtenstein, P., & Pedersen, N. L. (1997). Does genetic variance for cognitive abilities account for genetic variance in educational achievement and occupational status? A study of twins reared apart and twins reared together. Biodemography and Social Biology, 44, 7790.Google Scholar
Liddell, C., & Kruger, P. (1989). Activity and social behaviour in a crowded South African township nursery—A follow-up study on the effects of crowding at home. Merrill–Parmer Quarterly, 35, 209226.Google Scholar
Lytton, H., & Gallagher, L. (2002). Parenting twins and the genetics of parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 227253). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Matheny, A., & Phillips, K. (2001). Temperament and context: Correlates of home environment with temperament continuity and change, newborn to 30 months. In Wachs, T. D. & Kohnstamm, G. A. (Eds.), Temperament in context (pp. 81101). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Matheny, A., Wachs, T., Ludwig, J., & Phillips, K. (1995). Bring order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the confusion, hubbub, and order scale. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 429444.Google Scholar
Matheny, A., Wilson, R., & Thoben, A. (1987). Home and mother: Relations with infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 23, 323331.Google Scholar
McCall, R. (1991). So many interactions, so little evidence: Why? In Wachs, T. & Plomin, R. (Eds.), Conceptualization and measurement of organism environment interaction (pp. 142161). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325343.Google Scholar
Moberg, T., Lichtenstein, P., Forsman, M., & Larsson, H. (2011). Internalizing behavior in adolescent girls affects parental emotional overinvolvement: A cross-lagged twin study. Behavior Genetics, 41, 223233.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Rutter, M. (2006). Measured gene–environment interactions in psychopathology: Concepts, research strategies, and implications for research, intervention, and public understanding of genetics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 527.Google Scholar
Moilanen, I., Linna, S. L., Ebeling, H., Kumpulainen, K., Tamminen, T., Piha, J., et al. (1999). Are twins' behavioral/emotional problems different from singletons'? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, IV/62IV/67.Google Scholar
Mullineaux, P., Deater-Deckard, K., Petrill, S., Thompson, L., & DeThorne, L. (2009). Temperament in middle childhood: A behavioral genetic analysis of fathers' and mothers' reports. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 737746.Google Scholar
Murray, R. (1974). The influence of crowding on children's behavior. In Canter, D. & Lee, T. (Eds.), Psychology and the built environment (pp. 112117). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical modeling (6th ed.). Richmond, VA: Virginia Commonwealth University, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Novak, G., & Pelaez, M. (2003). Child and adolescent development: A behavioral systems approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Pettit, G., Bates, J., & Dodge, K. (1997). Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children's adjustment: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 68, 908923.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2001). Behavioral genetics. New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2010). Differential susceptibility to parenting and quality child care. Developmental Psychology, 46, 379390.Google Scholar
Prevatt, F. (2003). The contribution of parenting practices in a risk and resiliency model of children's adjustment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 469480.Google Scholar
Price, T. S., Freeman, B., Craig, I. W., Petrill, S. A., Ebersole, L., & Plomin, R. (2000). Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Research, 3, 129133.Google Scholar
Price, T., & Jaffee, S. (2008). Effects of the family environment: Gene–environment interaction and passive gene–environment correlation. Developmental Psychology, 44, 305315.Google Scholar
Purcell, S. (2002). Variance components models for gene–environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research, 5, 554571.Google Scholar
Rathouz, P., Van Hulle, C., Rodgers, J., Waldman, I., & Lahey, B. (2008). Specification, testing, and interpretation of gene-by-measured-environment interaction models in the presence of gene–environment correlation. Behavior Genetics, 38, 301315.Google Scholar
Reiss, D., & Leve, L. D. (2007). Genetic expression outside the skin: Clues to mechanisms of genotype × environment interaction. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 10051027.Google Scholar
Reiss, D., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2000). The interplay of genetic influences and social processes in developmental theory: Specific mechanisms are coming into view. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 357374.Google Scholar
Rhee, S. H., Cosgrove, V. E., Schmitz, S., Haberstick, B. C., Corley, R. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2007). Early childhood temperament and the covariation between internalizing and externalizing behavior in school-aged children. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 3344.Google Scholar
Riggins-Caspers, K. M., Cadoret, R. J., Knutson, J. F., & Langbehn, D. (2003). Biology–environment interaction and evocative biology–environment correlation: Contributions of harsh discipline and parental psychopathology to problem adolescent behaviors. Behavior Genetics, 33, 205220.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at 3–7 years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105176). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99166). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rowe, David (1994). The limits of family influence. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rubin, K., Coplan, R., & Bowker, J. (2009). Social withdrawal in childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 141171.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and behavior: Nature–nurture interplay explained. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Moffitt, T., & Caspi, A. (2006). Gene–environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 226261.Google Scholar
Saudino, K. (2005). Behavior genetics and child temperament. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 214223.Google Scholar
Saudino, K. J., & Cherny, S. S. (2001). Parent ratings of temperament in twins. In Emde, R. N. & Hewitt, J. K. (Eds.), The transition from infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences in the MacArthur Longitudinal Twin Study (pp. 7388). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Saudino, K. J., Pedersen, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., McClearn, G. E., & Plomin, R. (1997). Can personality explain genetic influences on life events? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 196206.Google Scholar
Saudino, K. J., & Plomin, R. (1997). Cognitive and temperamental mediators of genetic contributions to the home environment during infancy. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 43, 123.Google Scholar
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 119.Google Scholar
Schmitz, S., & Saudino, K. J. (2003). Links between temperament and behavior problems in early adolescence. In Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and the transition to early adolescence (pp. 185198). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schmitz, S., Saudino, K. J., Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J. C. (1996). Genetic and environmental influences on temperament in middle childhood: Analyses of teacher and tester ratings. Child Development, 67, 409422.Google Scholar
Schwartz, C., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1996). Early childhood temperament as determinant of externalizing behavior in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 527537.Google Scholar
Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene–environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60B, 6576.Google Scholar
Silventoinen, K., Kaprio, J., Lahelma, E., & Koskenvuo, M. (2000). Relative effect of genetic and environmental factors on body height: Differences across birth cohorts among Finnish men and women. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 627630.Google Scholar
Singh, A., & Waldman, I. (2010). The etiology of associations between negative emotionality and childhood externalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 376388.Google Scholar
Slutske, W. C., Heath, A. C., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A. R., Bucholz, K. K., Dunne, M. P., et al. (1997). Modeling genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of conduct disorder: A study of 2,682 adult twin pairs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 266279.Google Scholar
South, S. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2011). Socioeconomic status and health problems moderate genetic and environmental influences on internalizing pathology. Psychological Medicine, 41, 107117.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 251268.Google Scholar
Supplee, L., Unikel, E., & Shaw, D. (2007). Physical environmental adversity and the protective role of maternal monitoring in relation to early child conduct problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 166183.Google Scholar
Thapar, A., Harold, G., & McGuffin, P. (1998). Life events and depressive symptoms in childhood—Shared genes or shared adversity: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 11531158.Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 160164.Google Scholar
Valiente, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., & Reiser, M. (2007). Pathways to problem behaviors: Chaotic homes, parent and child effortful control, and parenting. Social Development, 16, 249267.Google Scholar
Wachs, T. D. (1988). Relevance of physical environment influences for toddler temperament. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 431445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wachs, T. D. (2000). Necessary but not sufficient: The respective roles of single and multiple influences on individual development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Wachs, T. D. (2010). Viewing microsystem chaos through a Bronfenbrenner bioecological lens. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Waldman, I. D. (2007). Gene–environment interactions reexamined: Does mother's marital stability interact with the dopamine receptor D2 gene in the etiology of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 11171128.Google Scholar
Wichers, M., Purcell, S., Danckaerts, M., Derom, C., Derom, R., Vlietinck, R., et al. (2002). Prenatal life and postnatal psychopathology: Evidence for negative gene–birth weight interaction. Psychological Medicine, 32, 11651174.Google Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Marceau, K. (2008). Disorders of childhood and adolescence: Gender and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 275303.Google Scholar