Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology

  • Ruth Feldman (a1)

Abstract

Elucidating the mechanisms by which infant birth conditions shape development across lengthy periods is critical for understanding typical and pathological development and for targeted early interventions. This study examined how newborns' regulatory capacities impact 10-year outcomes via the bidirectional influences of child emotion regulation (ER) and reciprocal parenting across early development. Guided by dynamic systems theory, 125 infants were tested at seven time points: birth, 3, 6, 12, and 24 months and 5 and 10 years. Initial regulatory conditions were measured by respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA; vagal tone) and neurobehavioral regulation (Brazelton, 1973) at birth. At each assessment between 3 months and 5 years, infant ER was microcoded from age-appropriate paradigms and mother–child reciprocity observed during social interactions. Four regulation-related outcomes were measured at 10 years: child RSA, empathy measured by mother–child conflict discussion and a lab paradigm, accident proneness, and behavior problems. An autoregressive cross-lagged structural model indicated that infant birth conditions impacted 10-year outcomes via three mechanisms. First, child ER and reciprocal parenting were individually stable across development and were each predicted by regulatory birth conditions, describing gradual maturation of ER and reciprocity over time. Second, better ER skills at one time point were related to greater reciprocity at the next time point and vice versa, and these cross-time effects defined a field of individual-context mutual influences that mediated the links between neonatal RSA and 10-year outcomes. Third, direct associations emerged between neonatal regulation and outcome, suggesting that birth conditions may establish a neurobiological milieu that promotes a more mature and resilient system. These mechanisms describe distinct “attractor” states that constrain the system's future options, emphasize the importance of defining behavior-based phenotypes of heterotypic continuity, and suggest that infants may shape their development by initiating unique cascades of individual-context bidirectional effects.

  • View HTML
    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology
      Available formats
      ×

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Ruth Feldman, Department of Psychology and the Gonda Brain Sciences Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel 52900; E-mail: Feldman@mail.biu.ac.il.

References

Hide All
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. Unpublished manuscript.
Adolphs, R. (2010). Conceptual challenges and directions for social neuroscience. Neuron, 65, 752767.
Anderson, T. W. (1960). Some stochastic process models for intelligence test scores. In Arrow, A. J., Karlin, S., & Suppes, Patrick (Eds.), Mathematical methods in the social sciences (pp. 205220). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Apter-Levi, Y., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2013). Oxytocin and vasopressin support distinct configurations of social synchrony. Brain Research, 1580, 19. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.10.052
Barrett, L., Blumstein, D. T., Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Kappeler, P. M. (2013). Taking note of Tinbergen: The promise of a biology of behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1618). doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0352
Bazhenova, O. V., Plonskaia, O., & Porges, S. W. (2001). Vagal reactivity and affective adjustment in infants during interaction challenges. Child Development, 72, 13141326.
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 300304. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x
Bernhardt, B. C., & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 123.
Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & O'Brien, M. (2008). Individual differences in trajectories of emotion regulation processes: The effects of maternal depressive symptomatology and children's physiological regulation. Developmental Psychology, 44, 11101123.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Braungart-Rieker, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. (1996). Infants’ responses to frustrating situations: Continuity and change in reactivity and regulation. Child Development, 67, 17671779.
Brazelton, T. B. (1973). Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale (Clinics in Developmental Medicine No. 50) . London: William Heinemann Medical Books.
Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Self-regulatory processes in early personality development: Multilevel approach to the study of childhood social withdrawal and aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 477498.
Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction of experience; A longitudinal study of representation and behavior. Child Development, 75, 6683.
Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 311388). New York: Wiley.
Choe, D. E., Shaw, D. S., Brennan, L. M., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2014). Inhibitory control as a mediator of bidirectional effects between early oppositional behavior and maternal depression. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 11291147.
Cirulli, F., Francia, N., Berry, A., Aloe, L., Alleva, E., & Suomi, S. J. (2009). Early life stress as a risk factor for mental health: Role of neurotrophins from rodents to non-human primates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 573585. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.001
Colombo, J., Moss, M., & Horowitz, F. D. (1989). Neonatal state profiles: Reliability and short-term prediction of neurobehavioral status. Child Development, 60, 11021110.
Daly, M., Delaney, L., Egan, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2015). Childhood self-control and unemployment throughout the life span: Evidence from two British cohort studies. Psychological Science, 26, 709723.
Day, J. M., & Tappan, M. B. (1996). The narrative approach to moral development: From the epistemic subject to dialogical selves. Human Development, 39, 6782.
Decety, J. (2012). Empathy: From bench to bedside. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. New York: Oxford University Press.
Diamantopoulou, R. J., van Meurs, S., Verhulst, F., & van der Ende, J. (2009). Child to adult continuities of psychopathology: A 24-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 120, 230238.
Dollberg, D., Feldman, R., & Keren, M. (2010). Maternal representations, infant psychiatric status, and mother-child relationship in clinic-referred and non-referred infants. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19, 2536.
Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., McClenny, B. D., & Porges, S. W. (2001). Neonatal cardiac vagal tone and school-age developmental outcome in very low birth weight infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 38, 5666.
Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Porges, S. W., Scanlon, J. W., Alemi, B., & Scanlon, K. B. (1997). Vagal regulation of heart rate in the prediction of developmental outcome for very low birth weight preterm infants. Child Development, 68, 173186.
Eckerman, C. O., Hsu, H. C., Molitor, A., Leung, E. H., & Goldstein, R. F. (1999). Infant arousal in an en-face exchange with a new partner: Effects of prematurity and perinatal biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 35, 282293.
Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality and regulation in children's social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66, 13601384.
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children's maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495525.
Emde, R. N., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R. B., & Oppenheim, D. (1991). The moral self of infancy: Affective core and procedural knowledge. Developmental Review, 11, 251270.
Essex, M. J., Klein, M. H., Cho, E., & Kalin, N. H. (2002). Maternal stress beginning in infancy may sensitize children to later stress exposure: Effects on cortisol and behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 776784.
Feldman, R. (1998). Coding Interactive Behavior (CIB) manual. Unpublished manuscript. Bar-Ilan University.
Feldman, R. (2006). From biological rhythms to social rhythms: Physiological precursors of mother-infant synchrony. Developmental Psychology, 42, 175188. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.175
Feldman, R. (2007a). Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing: Physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 48, 329354. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x
Feldman, R (2007b). Maternal versus child's risk and the development of parent–infant and family relationships in five high-risk populations. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 293312.
Feldman, R. (2007c). Mother–infant synchrony and the development of moral orientation in childhood and adolescence: Direct and indirect mechanisms of developmental continuity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 77, 582597.
Feldman, R. (2009). The development of regulatory functions from birth to 5 years: Insights from premature infants. Child Development, 80, 544–61. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01278.x
Feldman, R. (2010). The relational basis of adolescent adjustment: Trajectories of mother-child interactive behaviors from infancy to adolescence shape adolescents’ adaptation. Attachment and Human Development, 12, 173192.
Feldman, R. (2012a). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 380391.
Feldman, R. (2012b). Parent-infant synchrony: A bio-behavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 4251.
Feldman, R. (2012c). Parenting behavior as the environment where children grow. In Mayes, L. C. & Lewis, M. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of environment in human development. (pp. 535567). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Feldman, R. (2015). Sensitive periods in human social development: New insights from research on oxytocin, synchrony, and high-risk parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 369395.
Feldman, R., Bamberger, E., & Kanat-Maymon, Y. (2013). Parent-specific reciprocity from infancy to adolescence shapes children's social competence and dialogical skills. Attachment and Human Development, 15, 407423.
Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 274281.
Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2004). Parent-infant synchrony and the social-emotional development of triplets. Developmental Psychology, 40, 11331147. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1133
Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant-mother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: The role of neonatal vagal tone. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 290302.
Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2009). Biological and environmental initial conditions shape the trajectories of cognitive and social-emotional development across the first years of life. Developmental Science, 12, 194200. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00761.x
Feldman, R., Eidelman, A. I., & Rotenberg, N. (2004). Parenting stress, infant emotion regulation, maternal sensitivity, and the cognitive development of triplets; A model for parent and child influences in a unique ecology. Child Development, 75, 17741791.
Feldman, R., Gordon, I., Influs, M., Gutbir, T., & Ebstein, R. P. (2013). Parental oxytocin and early caregiving jointly shape children's oxytocin response and social reciprocity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38, 11541162.
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223231.
Feldman, R., & Klein, P. S. (2003). Toddlers’ self-regulated compliance to mothers, caregivers, and fathers: Implications for theories of socialization. Developmental Psychology, 39, 680692. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.680
Feldman, R., Rosenthal, Z., & Eidelman, A. I. (2014). Maternal-preterm skin-to-skin contact enhances child physiologic organization and cognitive control across the first 10 years of life. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 5664. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.012
Feldman, R., Weller, A., Sirota, L., & Eidelman, A. I. (2002). Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) promotes self-regulation in premature infants: Sleep-wake cyclicity, arousal modulation, and sustained exploration. Developmental Psychology, 38, 194207.
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Fox, N. A, Almas, A. N., Degnan, K. A., Nelson, C. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2011). The effects of severe psychosocial deprivation and foster care intervention on cognitive development at 8 years of age: Findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52, 919928. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02355.x
Fox, N. A., & Pine, D. S. (2012). Temperament and the emergence of anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 125128.
Frenkel, T. I., Fox, N. A., Pine, D. S., Walker, O. L., Degnan, K. A., & Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2015). Early childhood behavioral inhibition, adult psychopathology and the buffering effects of adolescent social networks: A twenty-year prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12390
Garcia-Coll, C. T., Emmons, L., Vohr, B. R., Ward, A. M., Brann, B. S., & Shaul, P. W., et al. (1988). Behavioral responsiveness in preterm infants with intraventricular hemorrhage. Pediatrics, 81, 412418.
Geva, R., & Feldman, R. (2008). A neurobiological model for the effects of early brain stem functioning on the development of behavior and emotion regulation in infants: Implications for prenatal and perinatal risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 49, 10311041. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01918.x
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 413.
Groome, L. J., Loizou, P. C., Holland, S. B., Smith, L. A., & Hoff, C. (1999). High vagal tone is associated with more efficient regulation of homeostasis in low-risk human fetuses. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 2534.
Gunnar, M. R., & Davis, E. P. (2003). Stress and emotion in early childhood. Handbook of Psychology, 2, 113134.
Harel, H., Gordon, I., Geva, R., & Feldman, R. (2011). Gaze behaviors of preterm and full-term infants in non-social and social contexts of increasing dynamics: Visual recognition, attention regulation, and gaze synchrony. Infancy, 16, 6970.
Harlap, S., Davies, A., Grover, N., & Prywes, R. (1977). The Jerusalem perinatal study: The first decade 1964–73. Israel Journal of Medical Sciences, 13, 10731091.
Halligan, S. L., Cooper, P. J., Fearon, P., Wheeler, S. L., Crosby, M., & Murray, L. (2013). The longitudinal development of emotion regulation capacities in children at risk for externalizing disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 391406.
Hauser, M., McAuliffe, K., & Blake, P. R. (2009). Evolving the ingredients for reciprocity and spite. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 32553266.
Hofer, M. A. (1995). Hidden regulators: Implication for a new understanding of attachment, separation, and loss. In Golberg, S., Muir, R., & Kerr, J. (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives (pp. 203230). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Huffman, L. C., Bryan, E., & Pedersen, F. A. (1998). Infant temperament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve weeks of age. Child Development, 69, 624635.
Isabella, R. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional synchrony and the origins of infant-mother attachment: A replication study. Child Development, 62, 373384.
Jean, A. D., & Stack, D. M. (2012). Full-term and very-low-birth-weight preterm infants’ self-regulating behaviors during a still-face interaction: Influences of maternal touch. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 779791.
Kagan, J. (1980). Perspectives on continuity. In Brim, O. G. & Kagan, J. (Eds.), Constancy and change in human development (pp. 2674). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Arcus, D., & Reznick, J. S. (1994). Galen's prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.
Kagan, J., & Zentner, M. (1996). Early childhood predictors of adult psychopathology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 3, 341350.
Kochanska, G. (1994). Beyond cognition: Expanding the search for the early roots of internalization and conscience. Developmental Psychology, 30, 2022.
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Development of mutual responsiveness between parents and their young children. Child Development, 75, 16571676.
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 10911111.
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199214.
Lester, B. M. (1984). A biosocial model of infant crying. Advances in Infancy Research, 3, 167212.
Levine, S. (2005). Developmental determinants of sensitivity and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 939946.
Levitt, P. (2003). Structural and functional maturation of the developing primate brain. Journal of Pediatrics, 143, S35S45.
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2007). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543562.
Maughan, A., Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Rogosch, F. A. (2007). Early-occurring maternal depression and maternal negativity in predicting young children's emotion regulation and socioemotional difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 685703.
Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene x environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 4179. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01381.x
Moss, M., Colombo, J., Mitchell, D. W., & Horowitz, F. D. (1988). Neonatal behavioral organization and visual processing at three months. Child Development, 59, 12111220.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2010). Mplus user's guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles: Author.
Nugent, J. K., Petrauskas, B. J., & Brazelton, T. B. (2009). The newborn as a person: Enabling healthy infant development worldwide. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Petersen, I. T., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., & Petit, G. S. (2014). Describing and predicting developmental profiles of externalizing problems from childhood to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000789
Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2011). Prenatal programming of postnatal plasticity? Development and Psychopathology, 23, 2938. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000623
Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J., Bolt, D. M., Hane, A., Burnson, C., & Winters, J. (2011). Infant physiological regulation and maternal risks as predictors of dyadic interaction trajectories in families with a preterm infant. Developmental Psychology, 47, 91105.
Popper, K. (2005). The logic of scientific discovery. New York: Routledge. (Original work published 1959)
Porges, S. W. (1985). US Patent No. 4510944 A. Method and apparatus for evaluating rhythmic oscillations in a periodic physiological response system. Washington, DC: US Patent & Trademark Office.
Porges, S. W. (2003). The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic contributions to social behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 79, 503513. doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00156-2
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441.
Rosenblatt, J. S. (1965). The basis of synchrony in the behavioral interaction between the mother and her offspring in the laboratory rat. In Foss, B. M. (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 345). London: Methuen.
Rossi, A. S., & Rossi, P. H. (1990). Of human bonding: Parent-child relations across the life course. New York: Transaction.
Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 2351.
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 265296.
Schneiderman, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin at the first stages of romantic attachment: Relations to couples’ interactive reciprocity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 12771285.
Schwebel, D. C, & Plumert, J. M. (1999). Longitudinal and concurrent relations among temperament, ability estimation, and injury proneness. Child Development, 70, 700712.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2014). Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 12.
Shannon, C., Schwandt, M. L., Champoux, M., Shoaf, S. E., Suomi, S. J., Linnoila, M., et al. (2005). Maternal absence and stability of individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in rhesus monkey infants. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 16581664. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1658
Singer, T. (2012), The past, present and future of social neuroscience: A European perspective. NeuroImage 61, 437449.
Smith, P. (2014). The butterfly effect. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 91, 247267.
Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 6, 498510.
Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 349367. doi:10.1080/14616730500365928
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Thompson, R. A. (1987). Empathy and emotional understanding: The early development of empathy. In Eisenberg, N. & Strayer, J. (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 119145). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 2552.
Tucker, D. M., Derryberry, D., & Luu, P. (2000). Anatomy and physiology of human emotion: Vertical integration of brain stem, limbic and cortical systems. In Borod, J. C. (Ed.), The neuropsychology of emotion: Series in affective science (pp. 5679). New York: Oxford Press.
Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity and chaos. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 71, 684689.
Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In Harris, D. (Ed.), The concept of development. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Wienberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1997). Maternal depression and infant maladjustment: A failure of mutual regulation. In Noshpitz, J. D. (Ed.), The handbook of child and adolescent psychiatry: Vol. 1. Infancy and preschool: Development and syndromes (pp. 177191). New York: Wiley.
Williams, L. R., Degnan, K. A., Perez-Edgar, K. E., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Pine, D. S., et al. (2009). Impact of behavioral inhibition and parenting style on internalizing and externalizing problems from early childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 3, 10631075. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9331-3
Winnicott, S. W. (1971). Playing and reality. New York: Basic Books.
Wray-Lake, L., & Syversten, A. K. (2011). The developmental roots of social responsibility in childhood and adolescence. New Directions in Child & Adolescent Development, 134, 1125.
Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: A motivated account. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 16081647.
Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 1217.

Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology

  • Ruth Feldman (a1)

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed