Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

Self-reported memory impairment and brain PET of amyloid and tau in middle-aged and older adults without dementia

  • David A. Merrill (a1), Prabha Siddarth (a1), Nathan Y. Saito (a1), Linda M. Ercoli (a1), Alison C. Burggren (a2), Vladimir Kepe (a3) (a4), Helen Lavretsky (a1), Karen J. Miller (a1), Jeanne Kim (a1), S. C. Huang (a4), Susan Y. Bookheimer (a2) (a5), Jorge R. Barrio (a4) and Gary W. Small (a1) (a3)...

Abstract

Background: Whether perceived changes in memory parallel changes in brain pathology is uncertain. Positron emission tomography (PET) scans using 2-(1-{6-[(2-[F-18]fluoroethyl)(methyl)amino]-2-naphthyl}ethylidene)malononitrile (FDDNP) can measure levels of amyloid plaques and tau neurofibrillary tangles in vivo. Here we investigate whether degree of self-reported memory impairment is associated with FDDNP-PET binding levels in persons without dementia.

Methods: Fifty-seven middle-aged and older adults without dementia (mean age ±standard deviation = 66.3 ± 10.6 years), including 25 with normal aging and 32 with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), were assessed. The outcome measures were the four factor scores of the Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ) (frequency of forgetting, seriousness of forgetting, retrospective functioning, and mnemonics use) and FDDNP-PET binding levels in medial temporal, lateral temporal, posterior cingulate, parietal, frontal, and global (overall average) regions of interest.

Results: After controlling for age, higher reported frequency of forgetting was associated with greater medial temporal (r = −0.29, p = 0.05), parietal (r = −0.30, p = 0.03), frontal (r = −0.35, p = 0.01), and global FDDNP-PET binding levels (r = −0.33, p = 0.02). The remaining MFQ factor scores were not significantly associated with FDDNP-PET binding levels, and no significant differences were found between normal aging and MCI subjects. Item analysis of the frequency of forgetting factor revealed five questions that yielded similar results as the full 32-question scale (r = −0.52, p = 0.0002).

Conclusions: These findings suggest that some forms of memory self-awareness, in particular the reported frequency of forgetting, may reflect the extent of cerebral amyloid and tau brain pathology.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Correspondence should be addressed to: David A. Merrill, MD, PhD, Assistant Clinical Professor, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, UCLA Semel Institute for Neuroscience & Human Behavior, 760 Westwood Plaza, Room 38-231, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA. Phone: +1 310-267-0274; Fax: +1 310-825-3910. Email: dmerrill@mednet.ucla.edu.

References

Hide All
Amariglio, R. E., Townsend, M. K., Grodstein, F., Sperling, R. A. and Rentz, D. M. (2011). Specific subjective memory complaints in older persons may indicate poor cognitive function. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 59, 16121617. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03543.x.
Bassett, S. S. and Folstein, M. F. (1993). Memory complaint, memory performance, and psychiatric diagnosis: a community study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 6, 105111.
Blazer, D. G., Hays, J. C., Fillenbaum, G. G. and Gold, D. T. (1997). Memory complaint as a predictor of cognitive decline: a comparison of African American and White elders. Journal of Aging and Health, 9, 171184.
Braak, H. and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica, 82, 239259.
Delacourte, A. et al. (1999). The biochemical pathway of neurofibrillary degeneration in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 52, 11581165.
Ercoli, L. et al. (2006). Perceived loss of memory ability and cerebral metabolic decline in persons with the apolipoprotein E-IV genetic risk for Alzheimer's disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 442448. doi: 63/4/442 [pii] 10.1001/archpsyc.63.4.442.
Ercoli, L. M. et al. (2009). Differential FDDNP PET patterns in nondemented middle-aged and older adults. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 397406. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318198750b 00019442-200905000-00007 [pii].
Gilewski, M. J., Zelinski, E. M. and Schaie, K. W. (1990). The memory functioning questionnaire for assessment of memory complaints in adulthood and old age. Psychology and Aging, 5, 482490.
Jessen, F. et al. (2010). Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 414422. doi: 67/4/414 [pii] 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.30.
Jonker, C., Geerlings, M. I. and Schmand, B. (2000). Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, 983991. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200011)15:11<983:AID-GPS238>3.0.CO; 2-5[pii].
Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A. and Mackinnon, A. (1997). Do cognitive complaints either predict future cognitive decline or reflect past cognitive decline? A longitudinal study of an elderly community sample. Psychological Medicine, 27, 9198.
Kepe, V. et al. (2006). Serotonin 1A receptors in the living brain of Alzheimer's disease patients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103, 702707. doi: 0510237103 [pii] 10.1073/pnas.0510237103.
Liu, J. et al. (2007). High-yield, automated radiosynthesis of 2-(1-{6-[(2-[18F]fluoroethyl)(methyl)amino]-2-naphthyl}ethylidene)malononi trile ([18F]FDDNP) ready for animal or human administration. Molecular Imaging and Biology, 9, 616. doi: 10.1007/s11307-006-0061-4.
Logan, J., Fowler, J. S., Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Ding, Y. S. and Alexoff, D. L. (1996). Distribution volume ratios without blood sampling from graphical analysis of PET data. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, 16, 834840. doi: 10.1097/00004647-199609000-00008.
Merema, M. R., Speelman, C. P., Kaczmarek, E. A. and Foster, J. K. (2011). Age and premorbid intelligence suppress complaint-performance congruency in raw score measures of memory. International Psychogeriatrics, Epublished ahead of print, doi: 10.1017/S1041610211001918.
Minett, T. S., Dean, J. L., Firbank, M., English, P. and O'Brien, J. T. (2005). Subjective memory complaints, white-matter lesions, depressive symptoms, and cognition in elderly patients. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 665671. doi: 13/8/665 [pii] 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.8.665.
Pike, K. E. et al. (2011). Cognition and beta-amyloid in preclinical Alzheimer's disease: data from the AIBL study. Neuropsychologia, 49, 23842390. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.012.
Reisberg, B. et al. (2008). The pre-mild cognitive impairment, subjective cognitive impairment stage of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 4, S98S108. doi: S1552-5260(07)00658-9 [pii] 10.1016/j.jalz.2007.11.017.
Rodda, J., Okello, A., Edison, P., Dannhauser, T., Brooks, D. J. and Walker, Z. (2010). (11)C-PIB PET in subjective cognitive impairment. European Psychiatry, 25, 123125. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2009.07.011.
Rowe, C. C. et al. (2010). Amyloid imaging results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study of aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 31, 12751283. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.007.
Saykin, A. J. et al. (2006). Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy similar to that of amnestic MCI. Neurology, 67, 834842. doi: 67/5/834 [pii] 10.1212/01.wnl.0000234032.77541.a2.
Small, G. W., Stern, C. E., Mandelkern, M. A., Fairbanks, L. A., Min, C. A. and Guze, B. H. (1992). Reliability of drawing regions of interest for positron emission tomographic data. Psychiatry Research, 45, 177185.
Small, G. W. et al. (1994). Age-associated memory loss: initial neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic findings of a longitudinal study. International Psychogeriatrics, 6, 2344; discussion 60–62.
Small, G. W., Komo, S., La Rue, A., Kaplan, A. and Mandelkern, M. A. (1995). Memory self-appraisal and cerebral glucose metabolism in age-associated memory impairment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 3, 132143.
Small, G. W. et al. (2006). PET of brain amyloid and tau in mild cognitive impairment. New England Journal of Medicine, 355, 26522663. doi: 355/25/2652 [pii] 10.1056/NEJMoa054625.
Smith, G. E., Petersen, R. C., Parisi, J. E. and Ivnik, R. J. (1996). Definition, course and outcome of mild cognitive impairment. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 3, 141147.
Sperling, R. A. et al. (2011). Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 280292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003.
Striepens, N. et al. (2010). Volume loss of the medial temporal lobe structures in subjective memory impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 29, 7581. doi: 000264630 [pii] 10.1159/000264630.
Talairach, J. and Tournoux, P. (1988). Coplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Three-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. New York: Thieme.
Zelinski, E. M., Gilewski, M. J. and Anthony-Bergstone, C. R. (1990). Memory Functioning Questionnaire: concurrent validity with memory performance and self-reported memory failures. Psychology and Aging, 5, 388399.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

International Psychogeriatrics
  • ISSN: 1041-6102
  • EISSN: 1741-203X
  • URL: /core/journals/international-psychogeriatrics
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed