Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-tr9hg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-13T21:02:15.433Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can Policy Responses to Pandemics Reduce Mass Fear?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 April 2022

Michael M. Bechtel*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science and European Affairs and Cologne Center for Comparative Politics, University of Cologne, 80870 Cologne, Germany Swiss Institute for International Economics and Applied Economic Research, St. Gallen, Switzerland
William O’Brochta
Affiliation:
School of History and Social Science, Louisiana Tech University, 1308 West Railroad Avenue, Ruston, LA 71272, USA
Margit Tavits
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Washington University in St. Louis, One Brookings Drive, Campus Box 1063, St. Louis, MO 63130-4899, USA
*
*Corresponding author: m.bechtel@uni-koeln.de; Twitter: @mm_bechtel
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To successfully address large-scale public health threats such as the novel coronavirus outbreak, policymakers need to limit feelings of fear that threaten social order and political stability. We study how policy responses to an infectious disease affect mass fear using data from a survey experiment conducted on a representative sample of the adult population in the USA (N = 5,461). We find that fear is affected strongly by the final policy outcome, mildly by the severity of the initial outbreak, and minimally by policy response type and rapidity. These results hold across alternative measures of fear and various subgroups of individuals regardless of their level of exposure to coronavirus, knowledge of the virus, and several other theoretically relevant characteristics. Remarkably, despite accumulating evidence of intense partisan conflict over pandemic-related attitudes and behaviors, we show that effective government policy reduces fear among Democrats, Republicans, and Independents alike.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Experimental Research Section of the American Political Science Association

The outbreak of the novel coronavirus (COVID-19) has caused one of the largest public health threats in human history. The ability of governments to cope with this challenge hinges on designing and successfully implementing policies that curb the pandemic and, as a result, minimize the likelihood of fear and resulting social instability. Mass fear undermines human well-being and can cause individuals to seek a sense of security and political stability (Hetherington and Suhay Reference Hetherington and Suhay2011; Lupia and Menning Reference Lupia and Menning2009). This may increase trust in political institutions (Bol et al. Reference Bol, Giani, Blais and Loewen2021), but it could also undermine norm compliance (Jørgensen, Bor, and Petersen Reference Jørgensen, Bor and Petersen2021) and raise support for authoritarian leaders and restrictive, aggressive, even antidemocratic policies (Feldman and Stenner Reference Feldman and Stenner1997; Hetherington and Suhay Reference Hetherington and Suhay2011; Kakkar and Sivanthan Reference Kakkar and Sivanthan2017).

Prior research on the political drivers of mass fear has focused on terrorism (Boscarino, Figley, and Adams Reference Boscarino, Figley and Adams2003; Duckitt and Fisher Reference Duckitt and Fisher2003; Vasilopoulos, Marcus, and Foucault Reference Vasilopoulos, Marcus and Foucault2018) and economic insecurity (Kakkar and Sivanthan Reference Kakkar and Sivanthan2017). This literature has largely ignored other types of threats, such as public health emergencies. Existing research on COVID-19 has established links between the virus, perceptions of fear (Didar-Ul Islam et al. Reference Didar-Ul Islam, Bodrud-Doza, Mahmud Khan, Abidul Haque and Mamun2020), and panic-related behavior (Prentice, Chen, and Stantic Reference Prentice, Chen and Stantic2020) but has not explored whether government responses to infectious disease, e.g., SARS, Ebola, or Nipah virus, could prevent these outcomes. A related area of research has examined how policy responses to natural disasters affect evaluations of incumbent performance (Bechtel and Mannino Reference Bechtel and Mannino2020; Bol et al. Reference Bol, Giani, Blais and Loewen2021; Chen Reference Chen2013; Healy and Malhotra Reference Healy and Malhotra2009) rather than fear as the outcome. As a result, we lack systematic knowledge about whether government responses during crises can reduce feelings of fear among the mass public. The urgency of finding answers to this question is underscored by the repeated spikes in COVID-19 infections occurring across the globe. At the same time and beyond its relevance for the current crisis, our study carries broader implications for our understanding of the linkages between government action, mass fear, and incumbent approval.

Policy responses and feelings of fear

Policy responses to a public health threat can be conceptualized as a dynamic policy decision under uncertainty, with three phases of policymaking (Gilligan and Krehbiel Reference Gilligan and Krehbiel1987). The first phase represents the initial conditions, which, in the case of an infectious disease, refer to the severity of the initial outbreak. The second phase is the policy response, where policymakers decide (a) how quickly to act and (b) what action to take. Actions include doing nothing, responding with a mild intervention (e.g., social distancing), or with a strong intervention (e.g., full lockdown). The third phase represents the outcome, i.e., the effectiveness of the policy decision in terms of whether the infection rates are decreasing, remaining constant, or increasing.

We are interested in how the three phases of policy response affect individuals’ feelings of fear in the context of a public health crisis. We define fear in the conventional way as “a basic, intense emotion aroused by the detection of imminent threat” (VandenBos, Reference VandenBos2015, 413). While previous work has not studied the relationship between fear and policy responses to pandemics, research on both natural disasters (including pandemics) and terrorism demonstrates that collective threats induce feelings of fear because of heightened uncertainty and loss of control (e.g., Brooks et al. Reference Brooks, Webster, Smith, Woodland, Wessely, Greenberg and Rubin2020; Vasilopoulos and Brouard Reference Vasilopoulos and Brouard2020). This suggests that government responses that restore a sense of order, control, and predictability have the potential to reduce fear amid a pandemic.

Which phase of policymaking is likely to matter and why? Prior work on the consequences of collective threats argues that to cope with the fear that major threats produce, people tend to prefer restrictive, even authoritarian policies (Hetherington and Suhay Reference Hetherington and Suhay2011; Jost et al. Reference Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski and Sulloway2003). This suggests that policy design (phase two of the policy response) should affect feelings of fear: a rapid and strong response (with severe restrictions) offers a coping mechanism – a promise to restore control and predictability – and should thereby reduce fear.

However, policy design choices are only an effort by the government to try to restore order, not a guarantee. Arguably, a clear policy effort may heighten expectations about the ability to alleviate threat-related uncertainty. Yet, even very invasive policy responses can sometimes prove insufficient to curb the spread of a collective threat such as an infectious disease. Therefore, the third phase – the effectiveness of the government policy – may offer the highest potential to affect fear. Interventions followed by a falling rate of infections reduce beliefs about the severity of the collective threat, restore a sense of control and predictability, and may thereby effectively reduce feelings of fear. In short, we expect that the effectiveness of the policy response (phase 3) matters more for reducing fear than the precise policy measures that have been implemented (phase 2). Our focus on how policy responses impact fear does not take away from potential individual-level differences, which we take care to control for in the empirical analysis.

Methods and data

Disentangling the potentially competing effects of the outbreak of an infectious disease, the policy response, and the policy outcome is difficult with non-experimental data. This is because incumbents will likely implement policy measures that are endogenous to the initial severity of the outbreak and a multitude of political, social, and economic factors that are related to the causes of the public health emergency, its expected impact, and the anticipated effectiveness of the policy response. To overcome this problem, we devised a randomized vignette experiment embedded in a survey that we fielded in June 2020 to a representative sample of the adult population in the USA (N = 5,461); see the Supplemental Information 1 (SI.1) for details on sampling, design, and measurement.Footnote 1 The research questions, experimental design, and survey questions were pre-registered with EGAP (Preregistration Plan #20200529AB). The modeling strategy was not pre-registered. The survey instrument contained several items to evaluate a range of research questions, some of which we have not explored yet. Here, we document how the design of policy responses to public health threats affects mass fear and policy support using a randomized experiment (see Q3.3 and 3.4 in the Preregistration Plan).

Our experimental design avoids deception while providing information that is both theoretically plausible and able to capture scenarios that were relevant empirically at the time that the survey was fielded. The experiment described the outbreak of a hypothetical, potentially deadly infectious disease, and provided information about how the state government responded and the impact of that response. The study field period (June 2020) represented the period immediately after the first wave of coronavirus cases in the USA. Initial stay-at-home orders had just or were in the process of expiring for many states while coronavirus cases had begun to reach rural areas with previously low exposure. In other words, the pandemic was in its early stages, and state governments were grappling with decisions regarding whether and how to act, meaning that the hypothetical experiment was plausible.

Fielding our study during an actual pandemic is important given our interest in how policy responses to infectious diseases affect mass fear. In contrast, if one were to perform the same study during nonpandemic times, one would attempt to answer the illogical question of how policy responses to a pandemic would affect mass fear in the absence of a pandemic. Moreover, this would potentially confuse respondents who might deem the scenarios unrealistic.

Our focus on policy action at the state level is based upon state governments’ abilities to implement direct policy responses to potentially curb the spread of infection. This was followed by information about the three phases of policymaking (outbreak severity, policy response, and policy outcome) that randomly varied the precise attributes of the scenario, using values that were empirically observable or plausible in the summer of 2020. The question wording we used for this monadic vignette experiment was as follows:

  1. 1. Outbreak Phase: “Suppose there has been an outbreak of an infectious, potentially deadly disease such as the coronavirus. The disease is spreading [very slowly/at a moderate rate/very quickly]. So far, [10,000/100,000/1,000,000] individuals have been infected in the U.S.”

  2. 2. Policy Response Phase: “The state government has been monitoring the outbreak for [10/30/60] days without taking action. It has then decided to implement the following measure: [do nothing/social distancing order with businesses and schools allowed to remain open (no large gatherings)/stay-at-home order with only essential businesses allowed to remain open].”

  3. 3. Outcome Phase: “Two weeks later the number of new cases has [decreased a lot/remained the same/increased a lot].”

For each attribute, we randomly selected one level that was presented to respondents and each respondent sequentially assessed four scenarios.

For each scenario, participants indicated their level of agreement with several statements on a 5-point scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” We used four measures to capture feelings of fear and closely related mental states and behavior. Two statements captured feelings of fear (Forsell et al. Reference Forsell, Kraepelien, Blom, Isacsson, Jernelöv, Svanborg, Rosén and Kaldo2019): “I feel worried, fearful, or frightened” and “I have thoughts of losing control or bad things happening.” A third item expressed feelings of fear about the future: “I am afraid that the situation could worsen.” The fourth statement captured fear-related behavior in the form of panic buying: “I feel the need to stock up on essential products (for example, food).” Finally, we included a statement to elicit government approval: “The state government is handling the situation well.” Each respondent assessed four scenarios. For the empirical estimation, we generated agreement indicator variables that equaled one if the answer was “somewhat agree” or “strongly agree” and zero otherwise.Footnote 2 We also created a fear index that equaled the average level of agreement across the four items and a binary fear indicator that is one if the average exceeded the midpoint of the scale and zero otherwise. All results remain substantively unchanged when we use the raw outcome variables, see Figure S1.

Results

We estimate the causal effects nonparametrically by regressing measures of fear on indicator variables for each attribute level (using one level as the reference category). All regressions include sociodemographic covariates that control for four age groups, four education levels, five income groups, and three residence categories (rural, urban, and suburban). To simplify exposition, Figure 1 shows the causal effects on our binary fear index indicator along with 95% and 99% respondent-clustered confidence intervals. Focusing on this index is justifiable given the very high inter-item correlation and the homogeneity of the results when estimating the effects separately for each of the four constitutive fear measures (see Figure S1).Footnote 3 We find that the initial outbreak severity increases all four measures of fear by about 2 to 8 percentage points when moving from a scenario where the rate of infections increases very slowly to one where the rate of infections increases very quickly. We find similar sensitivities when conceptualizing outbreak severity in terms of the number of infections.

Figure 1 Causal Effects of Pandemic Policy Response on Fear.

Note: Dots with horizontal lines are point estimates with 95% and 99% respondent-clustered confidence intervals from a linear least squares regression of a binary indicator of fear (that equals 1 if the average level of fear across four fear measures exceeds 3, which is the midpoint of the underlying 1 to 5 (strongly disagree to strongly agree) scale) on randomly assigned infection scenario and policy response attributes. N(scenarios) = 21,844, N(respondents) = 5,461. The results are very similar when analyzing the continuous fear index (Figure S1) and when re-estimating the effects separately for each of the four individual fear items (Figure S2). The results are also very similar when using survey weights (Figure S3).

When exploring the effect of the policy response, we find that the rapidity of action significantly reduces fear, while the precise measure that is implemented in response to the outbreak has little to no systematic effect. The strongest effects, by far, come from the policy outcome, i.e., the impact of the policy response on how the infection develops. If the policy response stabilizes the rate of infections, fear decreases substantially and significantly. A decreasing rate of infections further reduces average levels of fear by 30 to 35 percentage points. These results can be viewed as consistent with the attentive electorate argument, given that feelings of fear appear to be strongly linked to the policy outcome.Footnote 4

Effects by partisanship and other subgroups

We explored various factors that might account for our findings. First, we considered partisanship. The COVID-19 crisis hit the USA during a period with very high partisan polarization (e.g., Baldassarri and Park Reference Baldassarri and Park2020; Martherus, Martinez, Piff and Theodoridis Reference Martherus, Martinez, Piff and Theodoridis2021; Simas, Clifford, and Kirkland Reference Simas, Clifford and Kirkland2020). Indeed, emerging research on COVID-19 documents strong partisan differences in elite communication (Green et al. Reference Green, Edgerton, Naftel, Shoub and Cranmer2020) as well as mass attitudes toward the virus and public health measures (Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Barry Ryan2021, Gagan Reference Gagan2020; Pickup, Stecula, and van der Linden Reference Pickup, Stecula and van der Linden2020), leading pollsters to declare the sharp partisan divide “the biggest takeaway about U.S. public opinion in the first year of the coronavirus outbreak” (Deane et al. Reference Deane, Parker and Gramlich2021). Republicans have been considerably more skeptical of the severity of the virus; they tend to share a general preference for limiting government intervention and reducing public spending (Rudolph and Evans Reference Rudolph and Evans2005); and they may be more likely to perceive government responses to public health threats as imposing overly fierce restrictions on civil liberties. It is therefore possible that fear among Republicans is not as sensitive to either the type or outcome of the policy response or the initial infection severity.

Figure 2, however, shows that the estimated effects are strikingly similar for Democrats, Republicans, and Independents, indicating that partisanship plays no systematic role in how policy interventions affect feelings of fear. Table S3 offers statistical tests of the null hypothesis of no partisan differences in the treatment effects. These results further confirm that the impact of pandemic policy features on fear is quite similar for Democrats and Republicans.Footnote 5 This is a noteworthy null finding given the high levels of partisan polarization in the USA in general (e.g., Baldassarri and Park Reference Baldassarri and Park2020; Martherus, Martinez, Piff and Theodoridis Reference Martherus, Martinez, Piff and Theodoridis2021; Simas, Clifford, and Kirkland Reference Simas, Clifford and Kirkland2020) and regarding the pandemic in particular (Deane et al. Reference Deane, Parker and Gramlich2021). While Democrats and Republicans are often strongly divided in political attitudes and behavior, it appears that they are united in how government failure to contain the virus affects their feelings of fear. This, of course, does not demonstrate that respondents are immune to partisan biases in how the media communicates large-scale public health threats. Therefore, we further explore the effect of a closely related phenomenon – exposure to partisan news (Green et al. Reference Green, Edgerton, Naftel, Shoub and Cranmer2020; Hart, Chinn, and Soroka Reference Hart, Chinn and Soroka2020) – and find no profound differences between consumers of right-wing media (Fox News or One America News Network) and other respondents (see Figure S7). Better understanding the reasons behind these null findings requires a separate study. It is possible that the sources of a primal emotion such as fear were less subject to the kinds of biases that affect expressed attitudes, especially early in the pandemic when the nature of partisan divisions on the coronavirus response had not yet fully formed.

Figure 2 Causal Effects of Pandemic Policy Response on Fear by Partisanship.

Note: Dots with horizontal lines are point estimates with 95% and 99% respondent-clustered confidence intervals from a linear least squares regression of a binary fear indicator variable on randomly assigned policy design and infection scenario attributes. N(scenarios | Democrats) = 7,728; N(respondents | Democrats) = 1,932; N(scenarios | Independents) = 5,972; N(respondents | Independents) = 1,439; N(scenarios | Republicans) = 6,724; N(respondents | Republicans) = 1,681.

We further investigate heterogeneity in the main results across a number of other theoretically interesting subgroups (see SI.2). Moreover, we assess whether the causal effects we document are themselves conditioned by outbreak severity (Figures S12 to S14) and policy measures (Figure S15). The exploration of these interactions between context and policy response indicates that while outbreak severity does shift feelings of fear somewhat, the policy outcome remains far more influential.

We find that the treatment effects do not systematically vary by exposure to COVID-19 (Figure S8) or knowledge of COVID-19 symptoms (Figure S9). The effects are also strikingly similar when grouping respondents by gender (Figure S10), age (Figure S11), and subjective perceptions of COVID-19 infection risk (Figure S16). Fear is most strongly driven by the effectiveness of the policy response to the outbreak of an infectious disease for all of these subgroups of respondents. In terms of race, we find that the effects are somewhat less pronounced for non-white respondents but are generally in the same direction (Figure S17). We also explore the stability of the treatment effects by performing the split-by-round test (Bechtel and Liesch Reference Bechtel and Liesch2021) and find that the effects are quite similar across rounds (Figure S18).

Finally, we assess whether the design of pandemic policy responses also drives government approval (Figure S19). Consistent with our main results, we find that the policy outcome is the most powerful driver of incumbent approval, as a successful response increases government support by 26 percentage points. At the same time, respondents are also quite sensitive to outbreak severity as well as response rapidity and type. The only striking difference is that Democratic respondents are considerably more supportive of more invasive policy interventions. Yet, we note that the sensitivities are still in the same direction, i.e., individuals generally prefer stricter interventions irrespective of their partisan identity.

Conclusion

How can governments effectively respond to large-scale societal threats that evolve over time and require repeated interventions? Answering this question is challenging using observational data. Our approach relied on a randomized experiment that distinguished between outbreak, response, and outcome to offer a comparative perspective on how these features affect feelings of fear and policy approval. The results reveal several key findings. First, policymakers can reduce fear among the public during a crisis if their interventions prove effective. In contrast, the rapidity and type of policy response remain largely inconsequential. Such a pattern is consistent with a modified attentive electorate model. While voters do pay attention to policy responses, design features such as response type and rapidity have smaller effects on fear than policy effectiveness. This may reflect that the latter (i.e., the impact of government policy on the rate of infections) is the most informative feature of a policy response for relatively uninformed individuals. Other features such as the appropriateness of the response type and rapidity of the response are more difficult for citizens to assess without having access to highly specialized knowledge (e.g., knowing the particular characteristics of the disease, how it is transmitted, and how it affects humans) which most individuals lack. These features are therefore less likely to matter for emotional reactions such as fear as well as for policy approval.

Second, and perhaps most importantly, our null effects on partisanship are particularly interesting in the context of a growing literature that has documented widespread and intensifying partisan differences in public attitudes, behavior, and evaluations in general (Bisgaard Reference Bisgaard2015) as well as very noticeably in the context of the pandemic (Deane et al. Reference Deane, Parker and Gramlich2021). Here, we unearth a rare instance where such differences are absent. While further research is needed to fully understand the reasons behind the absence of partisan differences, taken together, our findings have important practical implications. They suggest that an outcome-oriented policy response to pandemics that prioritizes policy effectiveness is likely to result in decreased fear (and, possibly, improved social stability) even across partisan divides. This knowledge may provide incentives for opposing political sides to work together to produce an effective response. At the same time, an unfavorable policy outcome incentivizes incumbents to obfuscate information that would reveal their policy failure while office-seeking opposition parties may attempt to discredit policy responses even if they have been effective. Overall, this would imply that partisan cleavages over policy responses to public health threats originate from polarized political elites, not from voters applying different evaluative standards depending on their own partisan identity. Recent research documenting that elite communication on the COVID-19 pandemic is strongly polarized along party lines (Green et al. Reference Green, Edgerton, Naftel, Shoub and Cranmer2020) is consistent with this argument.

Last but not least, our study provides an example for how to study a complex and important phenomenon – dynamic policy response to a pandemic – in a tractable and realistic way. Fielding a randomized survey experiment during a global health crisis allows scholars to draw causal inferences while maintaining a level of realism that would be absent during non-pandemic times. Future work can employ this design and its logic to study the consequences of crises responses in different contexts.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2022.7

Data availability statement

The data, code, and any additional materials required to replicate all analyses in this article are available at the Journal of Experimental Political Science Dataverse within the Harvard Dataverse Network, at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/UMVB6G.

Acknowledgements

We thank Guillermo Rosas for helpful comments. We thank Sydney Weiss, Sam Short, and Emma Singh for valuable research assistance.

Funding

We acknowledge financial support from the Weidenbaum Center on the Economy, Government, and Public Policy at Washington University in St. Louis. Bechtel gratefully acknowledges support from the German Research Foundation under Germany’s Excellence Strategy (EXC 2126/1–390838866).

Conflicts of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Ethics statement

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Washington University in St. Louis (Protocol #202004256). See the Supplemental Information 1 (SI.1) for details on sampling, recruitment of subjects, data collection, experimental design, and measurement.

Footnotes

This article has earned badges for transparent research practices: Open Data and Open Materials. For details see the Data Availability Statement.

1 Table S1 reports sociodemographic margins for the raw sample and the weighted sample along with population margins.

2 The means of our raw fear measures are: Feel Fear = 3.5, Lose Control = 3.2, Concern: Worsen = 3.8, Stock Up = 3.4. The fear index average is 3.5. The correlations between the individual fear measures range from 0.62 to 0.78 and are all significant at the 1% level. Cronbach’s alpha is also very high: 0.9.

3 The few exceptions include: (a) stricter policy responses increase the desire to stock up on supplies and (b) the policy outcome has a somewhat stronger effect on feeling fearful than on other items (see Figure S1).

4 Our findings remain unchanged when computing Bonferroni-corrected p-values, see Table S2.

5 The only significantly different causal effect is that fear is reduced more strongly in response to an unchanged rate of infections among Republicans than among Democrats.

References

Baldassarri, Delia and Park, Barum. 2020. Was There a Culture War? Partisan Polarization and Secular Trends in US Public Opinion. Journal of Politics 82(3): 809827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, Michael M. and Liesch, Roman. 2021. Reforms and Redistribution. Disentangling the Egoistic and Sociotropic Origins of Voter Preferences. Public Opinion Quarterly 84(1): 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, Michael M. and Mannino, Massimo. 2020. Retrospection, Fairness, and Economic Shocks: How Do Voters Judge Policy Responses to Natural Disasters? Political Science Research and Methods. doi: 10.1017/psrm.2020.39 Google Scholar
Bechtel, Michael M., O’Brochta, William and Tavits, Margit. 2021. Replication Data for: Can Policy Responses to Pandemics Reduce Mass Fear? Harvard Dataverse, V3. doi: 10.7910/DVN/UMVB6G Google Scholar
Bisgaard, Martin. 2015. Bias Will Find a Way: Economic Perceptions, Attributions of Blame, and Partisan-Motivated Reasoning During Crisis. Journal of Politics 77(3): 849860.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bol, Damien, Giani, Marco, Blais, André and Loewen, Peter John. 2021. The Effect of COVID-19 Lockdowns on Political Support: Some Good News for Democracy? European Journal of Political Research 60(2): 497505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boscarino, Joseph A., Figley, Charles R. and Adams, Richard E.. 2003. Fear of Terrorism in New York after the September 11 Terrorist Attacks: Implications for Emergency Mental Health and Preparedness. International Journal of Emergency Mental Health 5(4): 199209.Google ScholarPubMed
Brooks, Samantha K., Webster, Rebecca K., Smith, Louise E., Woodland, Lisa, Wessely, Simon, Greenberg, Neil and Rubin, Gideon James. 2020. The Psychological Impact of Quarantine and How to Reduce It: Rapid Review of the Evidence. The Lancet 395: 912920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Jowei. 2013. Voter Partisanship and the Effect of Distributive Spending on Political Participation. American Journal of Political Science 57(1): 200217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deane, Claudia, Parker, Kim and Gramlich, John. 2021. A Year of U.S. Public Opinion on the Coronavirus Pandemic. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/2021/03/05/a-year-of-u-s-public-opinion-on-the-coronavirus-pandemic/, accessed on Dec 14th, 2021.Google Scholar
Didar-Ul Islam, S.M., Bodrud-Doza, Md., Mahmud Khan, Rafid, Abidul Haque, Md. and Mamun, Mohammed A.. 2020. Exploring COVID-19 Stress and its Factors in Bangladesh: A Perception-Based Study. Heliyon 6(7): e04399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Druckman, James N., Klar, Samara, Krupnikov, Yanna, Levendusky, Matthew and Barry Ryan, John. 2021. How Affective Polarization Shapes Americans’ Political Beliefs: A Study of Response to the COVID-19 Pandemic. Journal of Experimental Political Science 8(3): 223234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckitt, John and Fisher, Kirstin. 2003. The Impact of Social Threat on Worldview and Ideological Attitudes. Political Psychology 24(1): 199222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, Stanley and Stenner, Karen. 1997. Perceived Threat and Authoritarianism. Political Psychology 18(4): 741770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsell, Erik, Kraepelien, Martin, Blom, Kerstin, Isacsson, Nils, Jernelöv, Susanna, Svanborg, Cecilia, Rosén, Ann and Kaldo, Viktor. 2019. Development of a Very Brief Scale for Detecting and Measuring Panic Disorder Using Two Items from the Panic Disorder Severity Scale Self-Report. Journal of Affective Disorders 257: 615622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gagan, Jindal. 2020. An Online Card-Sorting Study to Inform the Initial Low-Fidelity Design of a Crowdsourced Health Information System for Individuals who have Chronic Health Conditions Seeking Local Health-Related Resources. Human Behavior and Emerging Technology 2(2): 128139.Google Scholar
Gilligan, Thomas W. and Krehbiel, Keith. 1987. Collective Decisionmaking and Standing Committees: An Informational Rationale for Restrictive Amendment Procedures. Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization 3(2): 287335.Google Scholar
Green, Jon, Edgerton, Jared, Naftel, Daniel, Shoub, Kelsey and Cranmer, Skyler J.. 2020. Elusive Consensus: Polarization in Elite Communication on the COVID-19 Pandemic. Science Advances 6(28): eabc2717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, P. Sol, Chinn, Sedona and Soroka, Stuart. 2020. Politicization and Polarization in COVID-19 News Coverage. Science Communication 42(5): 679697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healy, Andrew and Malhotra, Neil. 2009. Myopic Voters and Natural Disaster Policy. American Political Science Review 103(3): 387406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hetherington, Marc J. and Suhay, Elizabeth. 2011. Authoritarianism, Threat, and Americans’ Support for the War on Terror. American Journal of Political Science 55(3): 546560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jørgensen, Frederik, Bor, Alexander, and Petersen, Michael Bang. 2021. Compliance Without Fear: Individual-level Predictors of Protective Behavior During the First Wave of the COVID-19 Pandemic. British Journal of Health Psychology 26(2): 679696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, John T., Glaser, Jack, Kruglanski, Arie W. and Sulloway, Frank J.. 2003. Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition. Psychological Bulletin 129(3): 339375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kakkar, Hermant and Sivanthan, Niro. 2017. When the Appeal of a Dominant Leader is Greater than a Prestige Leader. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114(26): 67346739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupia, Arthuer and Menning, Jesse O.. 2009. When Can Politicians Scare Citizens into Supporting Bad Policies? American Journal of Political Science 53(1): 90106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martherus, James L., Martinez, Andres G., Piff, Paulk K. and Theodoridis, Alexander G.. 2021. Party Animals? Extreme Partisan Polarization and Dehumanization. Political Behavior 43(2): 517540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickup, Mark, Stecula, Dominik and van der Linden, Clifton. 2020. Novel Coronavirus, Old Partisanship: COVID-19 Attitudes and Behaviors in the United States and Canada. Canadian Journal of Political Science 53(2): 357364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prentice, Catherine, Chen, Jinyan and Stantic, Bela. 2020. Timed Intervention in COVID-19 and Panic Buying. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 57: 102203.10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudolph, Thomas J. and Evans, Jillian. 2005. Political Trust, Ideology, and Public Support for Government Spending. American Journal of Political Science 49(3): 660671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simas, Elizabeth, Clifford, Scott and Kirkland, Justin H.. 2020. How Empathic Concern Fuels Political Polarization. American Political Science Review 114(1): 392409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VandenBos, Gary R. 2015. APA Dictionary of Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.10.1037/14646-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasilopoulos, Pavlos and Brouard, Sylvain. 2020. System Justification and Affective Responses to Terrorism: Evidence from the November 2015 Paris Attacks. Political Psychology 41(3): 569586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasilopoulos, Pavlos, Marcus, George E. and Foucault, Martial. 2018. Emotional Responses to the Charlie Hebdo Attacks: Addressing the Authoritarianism Puzzle. Political Psychology 39(3): 557575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Causal Effects of Pandemic Policy Response on Fear.Note: Dots with horizontal lines are point estimates with 95% and 99% respondent-clustered confidence intervals from a linear least squares regression of a binary indicator of fear (that equals 1 if the average level of fear across four fear measures exceeds 3, which is the midpoint of the underlying 1 to 5 (strongly disagree to strongly agree) scale) on randomly assigned infection scenario and policy response attributes. N(scenarios) = 21,844, N(respondents) = 5,461. The results are very similar when analyzing the continuous fear index (Figure S1) and when re-estimating the effects separately for each of the four individual fear items (Figure S2). The results are also very similar when using survey weights (Figure S3).

Figure 1

Figure 2 Causal Effects of Pandemic Policy Response on Fear by Partisanship.Note: Dots with horizontal lines are point estimates with 95% and 99% respondent-clustered confidence intervals from a linear least squares regression of a binary fear indicator variable on randomly assigned policy design and infection scenario attributes. N(scenarios | Democrats) = 7,728; N(respondents | Democrats) = 1,932; N(scenarios | Independents) = 5,972; N(respondents | Independents) = 1,439; N(scenarios | Republicans) = 6,724; N(respondents | Republicans) = 1,681.

Supplementary material: Link

Bechtel et al.

Link
Supplementary material: File

Bechtel et al. supplementary material

Bechtel et al. supplementary material

Download Bechtel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 3.9 MB