Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Dorsal Stream Contributions to Perceptual Asymmetries

  • Nicole A. Thomas (a1), Oliver Schneider (a2), Carl Gutwin (a3) and Lorin J. Elias (a4)

Abstract

Neurologically normal individuals show a bias toward the left side of space, referred to as pseudoneglect due to its similarity to clinical hemispatial neglect. The left bias appears to be stronger in the lower visual field during free-viewing, which could result from preferential dorsal stream processing. The current experiments used modified greyscales tasks, incorporating motion and isoluminant color, to explore whether targeting dorsal or ventral stream processing influenced the strength of the left bias. It was expected that the left bias would be stronger on the motion task than on a task incorporating isoluminant color. In Study 1, similar left biases were observed during prolonged viewing for luminance, motion and red, but not green color. The unexpected finding of a leftward bias for red under prolonged viewing was replicated in Study 2. A leftward bias for motion was also evident during 150 ms viewing in Study 2. In Study 3, the left bias was not apparent when using a blue/yellow condition, suggesting the left bias for red under prolonged viewing was likely unique to red. Furthermore, the leftward bias for red disappeared under brief viewing conditions. It is suggested that dorsal stream processing likely underlies visual field differences in pseudoneglect. (JINS, 2012, 18, 251–259)

Copyright

Corresponding author

Correspondence and reprint requests to: Nicole A. Thomas, School of Psychology, Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA 5001 Australia. E-mail: nicole.thomas@flinders.edu.au

References

Hide All
Bálint, R., Harvey, M. (1995). Psychic paralysis of gaze, optic ataxia, and spatial disorder of attention. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 265281. doi:10.1080/02643299508251999
Barrett, A.M., Crosson, B., Crucian, G.P., Heilman, K.M. (2000). Horizontal line bisections in upper and lower body space. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 455459. doi:10.1017/S135561770064403X
Bauer, R.M., Demery, J.A. (2003). Agnosia. In K.M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 236295). New York: Oxford University Press.
Bellizzi, J.A., Hite, R.E. (1992). Environmental color, consumer feelings, and purchase likelihood. Psychology and Marketing, 9, 347363. doi:10.1002/mar.4220090502
Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain, 125, 20122022. doi:10.1093/brain/awf211
Braun, C.C., Sansing, L., Silver, N.C. (1994). The interaction of signal word and color on warning labels: Differences in perceived hazard. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting Proceedings, 2, 831835. doi:10.1177/154193129403801407
Braun, C.C., Silver, N.C. (1995). Interaction of signal word and colour on warning labels: Differences in perceived hazard and behavioural compliance. Ergonomics, 38, 22072220. doi:10.1080/00140139508925263
Bowers, D., Heilman, K.M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18, 491498. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(80)90151-7
Christman, S.D., Niebauer, C.L. (1997). The relation between left-right and upper-lower visual field asymmetries (or: what goes up goes right, while what's left lays low). In S.D. Christman (Ed.), Cerebral asymmetries in sensory and perceptual processing (pp. 263296). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B. V.
Çiçek, M., Deouell, L.Y., Knight, R.T. (2009). Brain activity during landmark and line bisection tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 18. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.007.2009
Clynes, M. (1977). Sentics: The touch of emotions. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press.
Corbetta, M., Shulman, G.L., Miezin, F.M., Petersen, S.E. (1995). Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Science, 270, 802805. doi:10.1126/science.270.5237.802
Drago, V., Crucian, G.P., Pisani, F., Heilman, K.M. (2006). Distribution of attention in normal people as a function of spatial location: Right-left, up-down. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 532537. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060681
Duncan, J., Bundesen, C., Olson, A., Humphreys, G., Ward, R., Kyllingsbaek, S., Chavda, S. (2003). Attentional functions in dorsal and ventral simultanagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(8), 675701. doi:10.1080/02643290342000041
Elias, L.J., Bryden, M.P., Bulman-Fleming, M.B. (1998). Footedness is a better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization. Neuropsychologia, 36, 3743. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00107-3
Elliot, A.J., Maier, M.A. (2007). Color and psychological functioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 250254. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00514.x
Elliot, A.J., Maier, M.A., Moller, A.C., Friedman, R., Meinhardt, J. (2007). Color and psychological functioning: The effect of red on performance attainment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 154168. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.154
Elliot, A.J., Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances men's attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 11501164. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150
Fink, G.R., Marshall, J.C., Shah, N.J., Weiss, P.H., Halligan, P.W., Grosse-Ruyken, M., Freund, H.J. (2000). Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI. Neurology, 54, 13241331.
Fink, G.R., Marshall, J.C., Weiss, P.H., Zilles, K. (2001). The neural basis of vertical and horizontal line bisection judgments: An fMRI study of normal volunteers. Neuroimage, 14, S59S67. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0819
Foxe, J.J., McCourt, M.E., Javitt, D.C. (2003). Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: Line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis. Neuroimage, 19, 710726. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00057-0
Goodale, M.A., Milner, A.D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 2025. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8
Goodale, M.A., Westwood, D.A. (2004). An evolving view of duplex vision: Separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 203211. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.002
Gur, M., Akri, V. (1992). Isoluminant stimuli may not expose the full contribution of color to visual functioning: Spatial contrast sensitivity measurements indicate interaction between color and luminance processing. Vision Research, 32, 12531262. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(92)90220-D
Haxby, J.V., Grady, C.L., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L.G., Mishkin, M., Carson, R.E., Rapoport, S.I. (1991). Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 88, 16211625. doi:10.1073/pnas.88.5.1621
He, S., Cavanagh, P., Intriligator, J. (1996). Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness. Nature, 383, 334337. doi:10.1038/383334a0
Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., Shimojo, S. (1993). Voluntary and stimulus-induced attention detected as motion sensation. Perception, 22, 517526.
Hupka, R.B., Zaleski, A., Otto, J., Reidl, L., Tarabrina, N.V. (1997). The colors of anger, envy, fear, and jealousy: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 156171. doi:10.1177/0022022197282002
James, T.W., Culham, J., Humphrey, K., Milner, A.D., Goodale, M.A. (2003). Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: An fMRI study. Brain, 126, 24632475. doi:10.1093/brain/awg248
Jewell, G., McCourt, M.E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: A review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 93110. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7
Kastner, S., Ungerleider, L.G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 315341. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315
Kaya, N., Epps, H.H. (2004). Relationship between color and emotion: A study of college students. College Student Journal, 38, 396405.
Kinsbourne, M., Warrington, E.K. (1962). A disorder of simultaneous form perception. Brain, 85, 461486. doi:10.1093/brain/85.3.461
Livingstone, M.S., Hubel, D.H. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. The Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 34163468.
Livingstone, M., Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240, 740749. doi:10.1126/science.3283936
Lu, S., Zhou, K. (2005). Stimulus-driven attentional capture by equiluminant color change. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 567572. doi:10.3758/BF03193806
Luh, K.E. (1995). Line bisection and perceptual asymmetries in normal individuals: What you see is not what you get. Neuropsychology, 9, 435448. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.9.4.435
Luria, A.R. (1959). Disorders of “simultaneous perception” in a case of bilateral occipito-parietal brain injury. Brain, 82(3), 437449. doi:10.1093/brain/82.3.437
Maier, M.A., Barchfeld, P., Elliot, A.J., Pekrun, R. (2009). Context specificity of implicit preferences: The case of human preference for red. Emotion, 9, 734738. doi:10.1037/a0016818
McCourt, M.E., Garlinghouse, M. (2000). Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: Pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex, 36, 715731. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70548-3
McCourt, M.E., Jewell, G. (1999). Visuospatial attention in line bisection: Stimulus modulation of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 37, 843855. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00140-7
McCourt, M.E., Olafson, C. (1997). Cognitive and perceptual influences on visual line bisection: Psychophysical and chronometric analyses of pseudoneglect. Neuropsychologia, 35, 369380. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00143-1
Mehta, R., Zhu, R. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323, 12261229. doi:10.1126/science.1169144
Mishkin, M., Lewis, M.E., Ungerleider, L.G. (1982). Equivalence of parieto-preoccipital subareas for visuospatial ability in monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research, 6, 4155. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(82)90080-8
Moller, A.C., Elliot, A.J., Maier, M.A. (2009). Basic hue-meaning associations. Emotion, 9, 898902. doi:10.1037/a0017811
Mullen, K. (1985). The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red-green and blue-yellow chromatic gratings. The Journal of Physiology, 359, 381400.
Nicholls, M.E., Bradshaw, J.L., Mattingley, J.B. (1999). Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size. Neuropsychologia, 37, 307314. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00074-8
Nicholls, M.E., Roberts, G.R. (2002). Can free-viewing perceptual asymmetries be explained by scanning pre-motor or attentional biases? Cortex, 38, 113136. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70645-2
Niemeier, M., Stojanoski, B., Singh, V.W., Chu, E. (2008). Paradoxical cross-over due to attention to high or low spatial frequencies. Brain and Cognition, 67, 115125. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.12.002
Nieuwenhuis, S., Jepma, M., La Fors, S., Olivers, C.N. (2008). The role of the magnocellular and parvocellular pathways in the attentional blink. Brain and Cognition, 68, 4248. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.02.119
Perenin, M.T., Vighetto, A. (1988). Optic ataxia: A specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. Brain, 111, 643674. doi:10.1093/brain/111.3.643
Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 123. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516
Previc, F.H. (1990). Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 519575. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00080018
Previc, F.H. (1998). The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 123164. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.124.2.123
Previc, F.H., Blume, J. (1993). Visual search asymmetries in three-dimensional space. Vision Research, 33, 26972704. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(93)90229-P
Previc, F.H., Breitmeyer, B.G., Weinstein, L. (1995). Discriminability of random-dot stereograms in three-dimensional space. International Journal of Neuroscience, 80, 247253. doi:10.3109/00207459508986103
Rubens, A. (1985). Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology, 35, 10191024.
Siman-Tov, T., Mendelsohn, A., Schonberg, T., Avidan, G., Podlipsky, I., Pessoa, L., Hendler, T. (2007). Bihemispheric leftward bias in a visuospatial attention-related network. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1127111278. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0599-07.2007
Skottun, B.C., Skoyles, J.R. (2006). Is coherent motion an appropriate test for magnocellular sensitivity? Brain and Cognition, 61, 172180. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.004
Tchernikov, I., Fallah, M. (2010). A color hierarchy for automatic target selection. Public Library of Science, 5, 14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0009338
Thomas, N.A., Elias, L.J. (2010a). Do perceptual asymmetries differ in peripersonal and extrapersonal space? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 210214. doi:10.1017/S135561770999097X
Thomas, N.A., Elias, L.J. (2010b). Perceptual asymmetries in greyscales: Object-based versus space-based influences. Cortex doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.015 [Epub ahead of print].
Thomas, N.A., Elias, L.J. (2011). Upper and lower visual field differences in perceptual asymmetries. Brain Research, 1387, 108115. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.02.063
Weiss, P.H., Marshall, J.C., Wunderlich, G., Tellmann, L., Halligan, P.W., Freund, H., Fink, G.R. (2000). Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: A physiological basis for clinical dissociations. Brain, 123, 25312541. doi:10.1093/brain/123.12.2531
White, B.J., Kerzel, D., Gegenfurtner, K.R. (2006). Visually guided movements to color targets. Experimental Brain Research, 175, 110126. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0532-5
Wilson, G.D. (1966). Arousal properties of red versus green. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 947949. doi:10.2466/pms.1966.23.3.947

Keywords

Related content

Powered by UNSILO
Type Description Title
VIDEO
Supplementary materials

Thomas Supplementary Material
Thomas Supplementary Material

 Video (2.9 MB)
2.9 MB

Dorsal Stream Contributions to Perceptual Asymmetries

  • Nicole A. Thomas (a1), Oliver Schneider (a2), Carl Gutwin (a3) and Lorin J. Elias (a4)

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.