Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa

Neural substrates of semantic memory

  • JOHN HART (a1) (a2), RAKSHA ANAND (a1), SANDRA ZOCCOLI (a3), MANDY MAGUIRE (a1), JACQUE GAMINO (a1), GAIL TILLMAN (a1), RICHARD KING (a1) (a2) and MICHAEL A. KRAUT (a1) (a4)...
Abstract

Semantic memory is described as the storage of knowledge, concepts, and information that is common and relatively consistent across individuals (e.g., memory of what is a cup). These memories are stored in multiple sensorimotor modalities and cognitive systems throughout the brain (e.g., how a cup is held and manipulated, the texture of a cup's surface, its shape, its function, that is related to beverages such as coffee, and so on). Our ability to engage in purposeful interactions with our environment is dependent on the ability to understand the meaning and significance of the objects and actions around us that are stored in semantic memory. Theories of the neural basis of the semantic memory of objects have produced sophisticated models that have incorporated to varying degrees the results of cognitive and neural investigations. The models are grouped into those that are (1) cognitive models, where the neural data are used to reveal dissociations in semantic memory after a brain lesion occurs; (2) models that incorporate both cognitive and neuroanatomical information; and (3) models that use cognitive, neuroanatomic, and neurophysiological data. This review highlights the advances and issues that have emerged from these models and points to future directions that provide opportunities to extend these models. The models of object memory generally describe how category and/or feature representations encode for object memory, and the semantic operations engaged in object processing. The incorporation of data derived from multiple modalities of investigation can lead to detailed neural specifications of semantic memory organization. The addition of neurophysiological data can potentially provide further elaboration of models to include semantic neural mechanisms. Future directions should incorporate available and newly developed techniques to better inform the neural underpinning of semantic memory models. (JINS, 2007, 13, 865–880.)

Copyright
Corresponding author
Correspondence and reprint requests to: John Hart, Jr., MD, Center for BrainHealth, University of Texas at Dallas, 2200 W. Mockingbird Lane, Dallas, Texas 75235. E-mail: jhart@utdallas.edu
Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

Albanese, E., Capitani, E., Barbarotto, R., & Laiacona M. (2000). Semantic category dissociations, familiarity and gender. Cortex, 36, 733746.

Barbarotto, R., Laiacona, M., Macchi, V., & Capitani E. (2002). Picture reality decision, semantic categories and gender. A new set of pictures, with norms and an experimental study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 16371653.

Barsalou, L.W., Kyle Simmons, W., Barbey, A.K., & Wilson, C.D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 8491.

Beauchamp, M.S., Lee, K.E., Argall, B.D., & Martin, A. (2004). Integration of auditory and visual information about objects in superior temporal sulcus. Neuron, 41, 809823.

Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., & Barbarotto, R. (1999). Gender affects word retrieval of certain categories in semantic fluency tasks. Cortex, 35, 273278.

Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Mohan, B., & Caramazza, A. (2003). What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 213261.

Caramazza, A., Hillis, A., & Rapp, B. (1990). The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 161189.

Caramazza, A. & Shelton, J.R. (1998). Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain the animate-inanimate distinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 134.

Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehericy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Henaff, M.A., & Michel, F. (2000). The visual word form area—Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain, 123, 291307.

Crosson, B., Moberg, P.J., Boone, J.R., Rothi, L.J.G., & Raymer, A.M. (1997). Category specific naming deficit for medical terms after dominant thalamic/capsular hemorrhage. Brain and Language, 60, 407442.

Crutch, S.J. & Warrington, E.K. (2003). The selective impairment of fruit and vegetable knowledge: A multiple processing channels account of fine-grain category specificity. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 355372.

Damasio, A.R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 2562.

Freese, J.L. & Amaral, D.G. (2005). The organization of projections from the amygdala to visual cortical areas TE and V1 in the macaque monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 486, 295317.

Gainotti, G. (1990). The categorical organization of semantic and lexical knowledge in the brain. Behavioural Neurology, 3, 109115.

Gainotti, G. (2000). What the locus of brain lesion tells us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders: A review. Cortex, 36, 539559.

Gainotti, G. & Silveri, M.C. (1996). Cognitive and anatomical locus of lesion in a patient with category specific semantic impairment for living beings. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 357389.

Gainotti, G., Silveri, M., Daniele, A., & Giustolisi, L. (1995). Neuroanatomical correlates of category-specific semantic disorders: A critical survey. Memory, 3, 247264.

Garrard, P., Lambon Ralph, M., Patterson, K., Pratt, J., & Hodges, J. (2005). Semantic feature knowledge and picture naming in dementia of Alzheimer's type: A new approach. Brain and Language, 93, 7994.

Gil-da-Costa, R., Martin, A., Lopes, M., Munoz, M., Fritz, J.B., & Braun, A.R. (2006). Species-specific calls activate homologs of Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the macaque. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 10641070.

Grossman, M., Smith, E., Koenig, P., Glosser, G., Rhee, J., & Dennis, K. (2003). Categorization of object descriptions in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia: Limitation in rule-based processing. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 120132.

Gutierrez, C., Cola, M.G., Seltzer, B., & Cusick, C. (2000). Neurochemical and connectional organization of the dorsal pulvinar complex in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 419, 6186.

Hart, J. & Gordon, B. (1990). Delineation of single-word semantic comprehension deficits in aphasia, with anatomical correlation. Annals of Neurology, 27, 226231.

Hart, J. & Gordon, B. (1992). Neural subsystems for object knowledge. Nature, 359, 6064.

Haxby, J., Gobbini, M.I., Furey, M.L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J.L., & Pietrini, P. (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science, 293, 24052407.

Hillis, A.E., Rapp, B., Romani, C., & Caramazza, A. (1990). Selective impairment of semantics in lexical processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7, 191243.

Hillis, A.E., Wityk, R.J., Tuffiash, E., Beauchamp, N.J., Jacobs, M.A., Barker, P.B., & Selnes, O.A. (2001). Hypoperfusion of Wernicke's area predicts severity of semantic deficit in acute stroke. Annals of Neurology, 50, 561566.

Humphreys, G.W. & Riddoch, M.J. (2003). A case series analysis of “category-specific” deficits of living things: The HIT account. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 263306.

Ilinsky, I.A., Jouandet, M.L., & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1985). Organization of the nigrothalamocortical system in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 236, 315330.

Inase, M., Tokuno, H., Nambu, A., Akazawa, T., & Takada, M. (1996). Origin of thalamocortical projections to the presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) in the macaque. Neuroscience Research, 25, 217227.

Klimesch, W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, 61100.

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Brain Research Reviews, 29, 169195.

Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schwaiger, J., Auinger, P., & Winkler, T. (1999). ‘Paradoxical’ alpha synchronization in a memory task. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 493501.

Klimesch, W., Schimke, H., & Pfurtscheller, G. (1993). Alpha frequency, cognitive load and memory performance. Brain Topography, 5, 241251.

Kraut, M., Calhoun, V., Pitcock, J.A., Cusick, C., & Hart, J. (2003). Neural hybrid model of semantic object memory: Implications from event-related timing using fMRI. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 10311040.

Kraut, M., Cherry, B., Pitcock, J., Vestal, L., Henderson, V., & Hart, J. (2006b). The Semantic Object Retrieval Test (SORT) in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 19, 177184.

Kraut, M.A., Kremen, S., Segal, J.B., Calhoun, V., Moo, L., & Hart, J. (2002a). Object activation from features in the semantic system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 2436.

Kraut, M., Moo, L., Segal, J., & Hart, J. (2002b). Neural activation during an explicit categorization task: Category- or feature-specific effects? Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 213220.

Kraut, M., Pitcock, J., Calhoun, V., Li, J., & Hart, J. (2006a). Neuroanatomic organization of sound memory in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 18771888.

Kraut, M., Pitcock, J., & Hart, J. (2004). Neural mechanisms of semantic memory. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 4, 461465.

Kronbichler, M., Hutzler, F., Wimmer, H., Mair, A., Staffen, W., & Ladurner, G. (2004). The visual word form area and the frequency with which words are encountered: Evidence from a parametric fMRI study. Neuroimage, 21, 946953.

Mahon, B.Z. & Caramazza, A. (2003). Constraining questions about the organisation and representation of conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 433450.

Martin, A. & Chao, L.L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 194201.

Martin, A., Wiggs, C.L., Ungerleider, L.G., & Haxby, J.V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649652.

McKenna, P. & Parry, R. (1994). Category-specificity in the naming of natural and manmade objects: Normative data from adults and children. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 225281.

Miceli, G., Fouch, E., Capasso, R., Shelton, J., Tomaiuolo, F., & Caramazza, A. (2001). The dissociation of color from form and function knowledge. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 662667.

Moss, H.E., Rodd, J.M., Stamatakis, E.A., Bright, P., & Tyler, L.K. (2005). Anteromedial temporal cortex supports fine-grained differentiation among objects. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 616627.

Polk, T.A. & Farah, M.J. (1998). The neural development and organization of letter recognition: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, computational modeling, and behavioral studies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science United States of America, 95, 847852.

Pulvermuller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 6, 576582.

Pulvermuller, F., Shtyrov, Y., & Ilmoniemi, R. (2005). Brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 884892.

Rademacher, J., Morosan, P., Schormann, T., Schleicher, A., Werner, C., Freund, H.J., & Zilles, K. (2001). Probabilistic mapping and volume measurement of human primary auditory cortex. Neuroimage, 13, 669683.

Rapp, B., Hillis, A., & Caramazza, A. (1993). The role of representations in cognitive theory: More on multiple semantics and the agnosias. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 235249.

Sartori, G., Job, R., Miozzo, M., Zago, S., & Marchiori, G. (1993). Category-specific form-knowledge deficit in a patient with herpes simplex virus encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 280299.

Schier, M.A. (2000). Changes in EEG alpha power during simulated driving: A demonstration. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 155162.

Segal, J.B., Williams, R., Kraut, M.A., & Hart, Jr., J. (2003). Semantic memory deficit with a left thalamic infarct. Neurology, 61, 252254.

Simmons, W.K. & Barsalou, W. (2003). The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 451486.

Slotnick, S., Moo, L., Kraut, M., Lesser, R., & Hart, J. (2002). Thalamic modulation of cortical rhythms during semantic memory recall in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of America, 99, 64406443.

Tyler, L.K. & Moss, H.E. (2001). Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 244252.

Tyler, L.K., Moss, H.E., Durrant-Peatfield, M.R., & Levy, J.P. (2000). Conceptual structure and the structure of concepts: A distributed account of category-specific deficits. Brain and Language, 75, 195231.

Tyler, L.K., Stamatakis, E.A., Dick, E., Bright, P., Fletcher, P., & Moss, H. (2003). Objects and their actions: Evidence for a neurally distributed semantic system. Neuroimage, 18, 542557.

Warrington, E.K. & McCarthy, R.A. (1987). Categories of knowledge: Further fractionation and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 12731296.

Warrington, E.K. & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829854.

Zola-Morgan, S., Cohen, N.J., & Squire, L.R. (1983). Recall of remote episodic memory in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 21, 487500.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society
  • ISSN: 1355-6177
  • EISSN: 1469-7661
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-the-international-neuropsychological-society
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords:

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 3
Total number of PDF views: 16 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 116 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 24th May 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.