Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T13:32:18.022Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Processing of local and global visual features in obsessive-compulsive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2006

STEFFEN MORITZ
Affiliation:
University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Clinical Neuropsychology Unit, Hamburg, Germany
MIKE WENDT
Affiliation:
Institute for Cognitive Research, Helmut-Schmidt-University/University of the Federal Armed Forces Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany

Abstract

Research conducted with the Rey-figure task has suggested that patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) process local aspects of a complex stimulus more efficiently than the overall gestalt. The aim of the present study was to investigate if this “local bias” is established already during early stimulus encoding or occurs only during later processing, once a percept has been formed (e.g., memory retrieval). To this end, responding to local and global targets of hierarchical letters (e.g., an “E” composed out of small “T”s) was assessed in 30 OCD patients and 28 healthy controls. OCD patients and controls performed comparably on all parameters. These results lend no support to the notion of an early perceptual bias towards local elements in OCD patients. It remains to be tested whether a bias towards local features is confined to situations where local and global features compete for selection. (JINS, 2006, 12, 566–569.)

Type
BRIEF COMMUNICATION
Copyright
© 2006 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bohne, A., Savage, C.R., Deckersbach, T., Keuthen, N.J., Jenike, M.A., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Wilhelm, S. (2005). Visuospatial abilities, memory, and executive functioning in trichotillomania and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 385399.Google Scholar
Cabrera, A.R., McNally, R.J., & Savage, C.R. (2001). Missing the forest for the trees? Deficient memory for linguistic gist in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine, 31, 10891094.Google Scholar
Deckersbach, T., Savage, C.R., Phillips, K.A., Wilhelm, S., Buhlmann, U., Rauch, S.L., Baer, L., & Jenike, M.A. (2000). Characteristics of memory dysfunction in body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 673681.Google Scholar
Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 28, 111.Google Scholar
Goodman, W.K., Price, L.H., Rasmussen, S.A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R.L., Hill, C.L., Heninger, G.R., & Charney, D.S. (1989). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 10061011.Google Scholar
Lamb, M.R. & Robertson, L.C. (1989). Do response time advantage and interference reflect the order of processing of global- and local-level information? Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 245258.Google Scholar
Lamb, M.R. & Robertson, L.C. (1998). The processing of hierarchical stimuli: Effects of retinal locus, locational uncertainty, and stimulus identity. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 172181.Google Scholar
Mataix-Cols, D., Alonso, P., Hernandez, R., Deckersbach, T., Savage, C.R., Manuel Menchon, J., & Vallejo, J. (2003). Relation of neurological soft signs to nonverbal memory performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 842851.Google Scholar
Miller, J. (1981). Global precedence in attention and decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 11611174.Google Scholar
Moritz, S., Kloss, M., Jahn, H., Schick, M., & Hand, I. (2003). Impact of comorbid depressive symptoms on nonverbal memory and visuospatial performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 8, 261272.Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353383.Google Scholar
Penades, R., Catalan, R., Andres, S., Salamero, M., & Gasto, C. (2005). Executive function and nonverbal memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 133, 8190.Google Scholar
Rachman, S. & de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 16, 233248.Google Scholar
Robertson, L.C. & Lamb, M.R. (1991). Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 299330.Google Scholar
Robertson, L.C., Lamb, M.R., & Knight, R.T. (1988). Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 37573769.Google Scholar
Savage, C.R., Baer, L., Keuthen, N.J., Brown, H.D., Rauch, S.L., & Jenike, M.A. (1999). Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 905916.Google Scholar
Savage, C.R., Deckersbach, T., Wilhelm, S., Rauch, S.L., Baer, L., Reid, T., & Jenike, M.A. (2000). Strategic processing and episodic memory impairment in obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology, 14, 141151.Google Scholar
Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. (1998). The MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59 (Suppl. 20), 2233.Google Scholar
Shin, M.S., Park, S.J., Kim, M.S., Lee, Y.H., Ha, T.H., & Kwon, J.S. (2004). Deficits of organizational strategy and visual memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology, 18, 665672.Google Scholar
Yovel, G., Yovel, I., & Levy, J. (2001). Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local visual perception: Effects of stimulus and task factors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 13691385.Google Scholar
Yovel, I., Revelle, W., & Mineka, S. (2005). Who sees trees before forest? The obsessive-compulsive style of visual attention. Psychological Science, 16, 123129.Google Scholar