Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-99c86f546-zzcdp Total loading time: 0.622 Render date: 2021-11-29T06:41:55.938Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "metricsAbstractViews": false, "figures": true, "newCiteModal": false, "newCitedByModal": true, "newEcommerce": true, "newUsageEvents": true }

How do infants build a semantic system?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2014

Suzy J. Styles*
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Kim Plunkett
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
*
Correspondence address: Suzy Styles, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford, OX1-3UD, UK. E-mail: suzy.styles@psy.ox.ac.uk.

Abstract

Do infants learn their early words in semantic isolation? Or do they integrate new words into an inter-connected semantic system? In an infant-friendly adaptation of the adult lexical priming paradigm, infants at 18 and 24 months-of-age heard two words in quick succession. The noun-pairs were either related or unrelated. Following the onset of the target word, two pictures were presented, one of which depicted the target. Eye movements revealed that both age groups comprehended the target word. In addition, 24-month-olds demonstrated primed picture looking in two measures of comprehension: Named target pictures preceded by a related word pair took longer to disengage from and attracted more looking overall. The finding of enhanced target recognition demonstrates the emergence of semantic organisation by the end of the second year.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, John R. 1983. A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22. 261295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antos, Stephen J. 1979. Processing facilitation in a lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5. 527545.Google Scholar
Aslin, Richard. 2007. What's in a look? Developmental Science 10. 4853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, Helen. 1979. Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language 6. 183200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Paul. 2002. How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA & London, UK: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bretherton, Inge, McNew, Sandra, Snyder, Lynn & Bates, Elizabeth. 1983. Individual differences at 20 months: Analytic and holistic strategies in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10. 293320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canfield, Richard L., Smith, Elliott G., Brezsnyak, Michael P. & Snow, Kyle L.. 1997. Information processing through the first year of life: A longitudinal study using the visual expectation paradigm. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 62. 1170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, Allan M. & Loftus, Elizabeth F.. 1975. A spreading–activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review 82. 407428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cree, George S. & McRae, Ken. 2003. Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, cheese and cello (and many other such concrete nouns). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 132. 163201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Federmeier, Kara D. & Kutas, Marta. 1999. A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language 41. 469495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, Larry, Dale, Philip S., Reznick, J. Stephen, Bates, Elizabeth, Thal, Donna J. & Pethick, Stephen J.. 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59. 1173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, Anne, Pinto, John P., Weinberg, Amy & McRoberts, Gerald. 1998. Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science 9. 228231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, Anne, Swingley, Daniel & Pinto, John P.. 2001. When halfa word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development 72. 10031015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedrich, Manuela & Friederici, Angela D.. 2004. N400–like semantic incongruity efect in 19-month-olds: Processing known words in picture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16. 14651477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedrich, Manuela & Friederici, Angela D.. 2005a. Lexical priming and semantic integration reflected in the event–related potential of 14-month-olds. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology 16. 653656.Google ScholarPubMed
Friedrich, Manuela & Friederici, Angela D.. 2005b. Semantic sentence processing reflected in the event-related potentials of one- and two-year-old children. Neuroreport 16. 18011804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick, Hirsh-Pasek, Kathryn, Cauley, Kathleen M. & Gordon, Laura. 1987. The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language 14. 2345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halberda, Justin. 2003. The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition 87. B23B34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, Antonia, Plunkett, Kim & Schafer, Graham. 2000. Infant vocabulary development assessed with a British communicative development inventory. Journal of Child Language 27. 689705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hashimoto, Naomi, McGregor, Karla K. & Graham, Anne. 2007. Conceptual organisation at 6 and 8 years of age: Evidence from the semantic priming of object decisions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50. 161176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinze, Hans-Jochen, Muente, Thomas-Frank & Kutas, Marta. 1998. Context effects in a category verification task as assessed by event-related brain potential (ERP) measures. Biological Psychology 47. 121135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hills, Thomas, Maouene, Maunir, Maouene, Josita, Sheya, Adam & Smith, Linda B.. 2008. Is there preferential attachment in the growth of early semantic networks. In Love, B. C., McRae, K. & Sloutsky, V. M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 155160. Washington, DC: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Huettig, Falk & Altmann, Gerry T. M.. 2005. Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: Semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm. Cognition 96. B23B32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huettig, Falk & McQueen, James M.. 2007. The tug of war between phonological, semantic and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language 57. 460482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Yuki, Altmann, Gerry T. M. & Haywood, Sarah L.. 2001. The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 40. 133156.Google Scholar
Krackow, Elisa & Gordon, Peter. 1998. Are lions and tigers substitutes or associates? Evidence against the slot filler accounts of children's early categorization. Child Development 69. 347354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutas, Marta & Hillyard, Steven A.. 1980. Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words. Biological Psychology 11. 99116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mani, Nivedita & Plunkett, Kim. 2007. Phonological specificity of consonants and vowels in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language 57. 252272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, Ellen M. 1989. Categorization and naming in children. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McCauley, Charley, Weil, Carolyne M. & Sperber, Richard D.. 1976. The development of memory structure as reflected by semantic-priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 22. 511518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, Timothy P. 2005. Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. London: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McRae, Ken, de Sa, Virginia R. & Seidenberg, Mark S.. 1997. On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126. 99130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meints, Kerstin, Plunkett, Kim & Harris, Paul. 1999. When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. Developmental Psychology 35. 10721078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merriman, William E. & Bowman, Laura L.. 1989. The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 54. 1132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, D. E. & Schvaneveldt, R. W.. 1971. Facilitation in recognising pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology 90. 227234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, George A. 1995. WordNet: An on–line lexical database. International Journal of Lexicography 3.Google Scholar
Moss, Helen E., McCormick, Samantha F. & Tyler, Lorraine K.. 1997. The time course of activation of semantic information during spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes 12. 695731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, Helen E. & Older, Lianne. 1996. Birkbeck Word Association Norms. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Moss, Helen E., Ostrin, Ruth K., Tyler, Lorraine K. & Marslen-Wilson, William D.. 1995. Accessing diferent types of lexical semantic information: Evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21. 863883.Google Scholar
Nation, Kate & Snowling, Margaret J.. 1999. Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition 70. B1B13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, Douglas L., McEvoy, Cathy L. & Schreiber, Thomas A.. 1998. The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. http://www.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/.Google Scholar
Raajmakers, Jeroen G. W. 2003. A further look at the “Language-as-a-fixed-effect Fallacy”. Canadian Journal of Psychology 57. 141151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raajmakers, Jeroen G. W., Schrijnemakers, Joseph M. C. & Gremmen, Frans. 1999. How to deal with the “Language-as-a-fixed-effect fallacy”: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language 41. 416426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radeau, Monique. 1983. Semantic priming between spoken words in adults and children. Canadian Journal of Psychology 4. 547556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznick, J. Stephen. 1990. Visual preference as a test of infant word comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics 11. 145166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roget, Peter Mark. 1991. Roget's thesaurus of English words and phrases. Project Guttenberg, MICRA Inc.Google Scholar
Schafer, Graham & Plunkett, Kim. 1998. Rapid word learning by 15-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. Child Development 69. 309320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schvaneveldt, R. W., Ackerman, Brian P. & Semlear, Teddy. 1977. The effect of semantic context on children's word recognition. Child Development 48. 612616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steyvers, Mark & Tenenbaum, Joshua B.. 2005. The large-scale structure of semantic networks: Statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. Cognitive Science 29. 4178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Styles, Suzy J., Arias-Trejo, Natalia & Plunkett, Kim. 2008. Priming and lexical interference in infancy. In Love, B. C., McRae, K., and Sloutsky, V. M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 651656. Washington, DC: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Styles, Suzy J. & Plunkett, Kim. In Press. What is ‘word understanding’ for the parent of a 1-year-old? Matching the difficulty of lexical comprehension tasks to parental CDI report. Journal of Child Language.Google Scholar
Swingley, Daniel & Aslin, Richard. 2007. Lexical competition in young children's word learning. Cognitive Psychology 54. 99132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swingley, Daniel, Pinto, John P. & Fernald, Anne. 1999. Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition 71. 73108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swinney, David A. 1979. Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 18. 645659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael & Mervis, Carolyn B.. 1994. The instrument is great, but measuring comprehension is still a problem. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59. 174179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torkildsen, Jan van Koss, Syversen, Gro, Simonsen, Hanne Gram, Moen, Inger & Lindgren, Magnus. 2007. Electrophysiological correlates of auditory semantic priming in 24-month-olds. Journal of Neurolinguistics 20. 332351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, Katherine S. & Morgan, James L.. 2008. Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representation. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 114132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
46
Cited by

Send article to Kindle

To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

How do infants build a semantic system?
Available formats
×

Send article to Dropbox

To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

How do infants build a semantic system?
Available formats
×

Send article to Google Drive

To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

How do infants build a semantic system?
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *