Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T11:24:42.128Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How words anchor categorization: conceptual flexibility with labeled and unlabeled categories*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 July 2014

JACKSON TOLINS*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz
ELIANA COLUNGA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder
*
Address for correspondence: Jackson Tolins, University of California, Psychology Department, 1156 High St., Santa Cruz, CA 95060. e-mail: jtolins@ucsc.edu

Abstract

Labeled categories are learned faster, and are subsequently more robust than categories learned without labels. The label feedback hypothesis (Lupyan, 2012) accounts for these effects by introducing a word-driven top-down modulation of perceptual processes involved in categorization. By testing categorization flexibility with and without labels, we demonstrate the ways in which labels do and do not modulate category representations. In Experiment 1, transfer involved a change in selective attention, and results indicated that labels did not impact relearning. In Experiment 2, when transfer involved a change in the behavioral response to categories whose structures did not change, a reversal shift, learning the categories with labels speeded recovery. We take this finding as evidence that the augmentation of perceptual processes by words is on the one hand fairly weak without explicit reinforcement, but on the other allows for category representations to be more abstract, allowing greater flexibility in behavior.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Portions of this study were presented at the 35th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, in Berlin, Germany. The authors thank the conference reviewers and attendees for their useful feedback. The authors also thank the many research assistants at the CU Language Project who assisted in this project.

The original version of this article was published with last word of the title missing. A notice detailing this has been published and the error rectified in the online and print PDF and HTML copies.

References

references

Boroditsky, L., Schmidt, L. A., & Phillips, W. (2003). Sex, syntax, and semantics. In Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 6180). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M., & Choi, S. (2001). Shaping meaning for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475522). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brojde, C. L., Porter, C., & Colunga, E. (2011). Words can slow down category learning. Psychological Bulleting & Review, 18, 798804.Google Scholar
Casasola, M. (2005). Can language do the driving? The effect of linguistic input on infants’ categorization of support spatial relations. Developmental Psychology, 41, 183192.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. TRENDS in Cognitive Science, 10, 370374.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 181204.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. D., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 99, 4577.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 6670.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2006). Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 489494.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2005). Language and thought. In Holyoak, K. & Morrison, B. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 633661). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 178200.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 585612.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., Lippa, Y., & Shiffrin, R. (2001). Altering object representations through category learning. Cognition, 78, 2743.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., & Steyvers, M. (2001). The sensitization and differentiation of dimensions during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 116139.Google Scholar
Gumperz, J. J., & Levinson, S. C. (1996). Rethinking linguistic relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Haider, H., & Frensch, P. A. (1996). The role of information reduction in skill acquisition. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 304337.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (2005). Cognition is categorization. In Cohen, H. & Lefebvre, C. (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (pp. 1943). San Diego: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hendrickson, A. T., Kachergis, G., Fausey, C. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2012). Re-learning labeled categories reveals structured representations. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 34th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Sapporo, Japan.Google Scholar
Hespos, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Conceptual precursors to language. Nature, 430, 453455.Google Scholar
Honey, R. C., & Hall, G. (1989). The acquired equivalence and distinctiveness of cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 338346.Google Scholar
Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir−Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86, 6579.Google Scholar
Kendler, H. H., & Kendler, T. S. (1962). Vertical and horizontal processes in problem solving. Psychological Review, 69, 116.Google Scholar
Ketels, S. L., & Jones, M. (2010). Verbal labels are not always useful. Poster presented at the Ninth Annual Summer Interdisciplinary Conference, Bend, OR.Google Scholar
Kharitonova, M., Chien, S., Colunga, E., & Munakata, Y. (2009). More than a matter of getting ‘unstuck’: flexible thinkers use more abstract representations than perseverators. Development Science, 12, 662669.Google Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. (1996). Dimensional relevance shifts in category learning. Connection Science, 8, 201223.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (1997). From outer to inner space: linguistic categories and non- linguistic thinking. In Nuyts, J & Pederson, E. (Eds.), Language and conceptualization (pp. 1345). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Li, P., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning. Cognition, 83, 265294.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2007). The label feedback hypothesis: linguistic influences on visual processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2008). From chair to ‘chair’: a representational shift account of object labeling effects on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 348369.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2009). Extracommunicative functions of language: verbal interference causes selective categorization impairments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 711718.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 113.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., Rakison, D. H., & McClelland, J. L. (2007). Language is not just for talking: redundant labels facilitate learning of novel categories. Psychological Science, 18, 10771083.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., & Spivey, M. (2010). Redundant spoken labels facilitate perceptions of multiple items. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 22362253.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2012). The evocative power of words: activation concepts by verbal and nonverbal means. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 141170.Google Scholar
Maddox, W. T., Glass, B. D., O’Brien, J. B., Filoteo, J. V., & Ashby, F. G. (2010). Category label and response location shifts in category learning. Psychological Research, 74, 219236.Google Scholar
Malt, B. C., Sloman, S. A., & Gennari, S. (2003). Universality and language specificity in object naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 2042.Google Scholar
Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implications for object permanence theories: a PDP model of the A-not-B task. Developmental Science, 1, 161211.Google Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification−categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 3957.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1995). The language instinct: the new science of language and mind. New York, NY: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., & Davidoff, J. (2000). The categorical perception of colors and facial expressions: the effect of verbal interference. Memory and Cognition, 28, 977986.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Davidoff, J., Davies, I. R. L., & Shapiro, L. R. (2005). Color categories: evidence for the cultural relativity hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 378411.Google Scholar
Ross, B. H., & Murphy, G. L. (1999). Food for thought: cross-classification and category organization in a complex real-world domain. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 495553.Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127190.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., & Tsivkin, S. (2001). Initial knowledge of conceptual change: space and number. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language (rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R., & Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitation to form categories: evidence from 12- to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257302.Google Scholar
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M., Wu, L., Wade, A., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 77807785.Google Scholar
Yoshida, H., & Smith, L. B. (2005). Linguistic cues enhance the learning of perceptual cues. Psychological Science, 16, 564577.Google Scholar