Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T03:33:23.325Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Receptive number morphosyntax in children with Down syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2020

ROBERTO A. ABREU-MENDOZA
Affiliation:
Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
TANIA JASSO
Affiliation:
Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
ELIA E. SOTO-ALBA
Affiliation:
Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
NATALIA ARIAS-TREJO*
Affiliation:
Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
*
Address for correspondence Natalia Arias-Trejo, Facultad de Psicología, Av. Universidad 3004, Sótano, Edificio “C”, Col. Copilco Universidad C. P. 04510, Coyoacán, Cdad. de México, México. tel: 52 55 56222287; E-mail: nariast@unam.mx

Abstract

This study investigated the comprehension of plural morphosyntactic markers and its relationship with numerical comparison abilities in children with Down syndrome (DS). It evaluated 16 Spanish-speaking children with DS (mean verbal mental age = 3;6) and 16 typically developing children with similar receptive vocabulary (mean chronological age = 3;5). Children participated in two preferential looking tasks assessing their abilities to map singular and plural markers to their visual referents and to distinguish one object from more than one. Results showed that both groups of children correctly mapped plural markers to their referents but failed to map singular ones. Furthermore, results also indicated that both groups also looked at collections of more than one object with four objects but not at those with two. The eye movement patterns of children who looked at collections of more than one object suggest a counting-like strategy. These results indicate that comprehension of plural markers of children with DS is similar to that of their typically developing peers; however, it is not related to their numerical abilities.

Type
Article
Copyright
© UK Cognitive Linguistics Association, 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza is now affiliated with Rutgers University, USA. The authors have declared that no conflict of interests exist. The research described in this paper was supported by a PAPIIT (IN304417) research grant awarded to the last author. We thank all the institutions, children, and their families who contributed and participated in this study. We also thank Ana Rosa Serra and Brenda Razo for their assistance on reaching and recruiting participants, and Omar Isaúl Elorza Padilla for drawing the figures used as stimuli. R.A.M, T.J., and N.A.T. conceived and designed the study; R.A.M, T.J., and E.S.A. performed the experiments; R.A.M. carried out the statistical analyses. All contributors participated in writing the manuscript. The research data to reproduce our results can be found in the following OSF link: <https://osf.io/yh7sc/>.

References

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., Warren, S. F. & Conners, F. A. (2007). Language development in Down syndrome: from the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 13(3), 247261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abreu-Mendoza, R. A. & Arias-Trejo, N. (2015). Numerical and area comparison abilities in Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 41/42, 5865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abreu-Mendoza, R. A. & Arias-Trejo, N. (2017). Counting abilities in Down syndrome: the role of the one-to-one correspondence principle and receptive vocabulary. Neuropsychology 31(7), 750758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N., Abreu-Mendoza, R. A. & Aguado-Servín, O. A. (2014). Spanish-speaking children’s production of number morphology. First Language 34(4), 372384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N. & Barrón-Martínez, J. B. (2017). Language skills in Down syndrome. In Benavides, A. A. & Schwarts, R. G. (eds), Language development and disorders in Spanish-speaking children (pp. 329341). Berlin: Springer International.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arias-Trejo, N., Cantrell, L. M., Smith, L. B. & Alva Canto, E. A. (2014). Early comprehension of the Spanish plural. Journal of Child Language 41(6), 13561372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barner, D., Thalwitz, D., Wood, J., Yang, S. & Carey, S. (2007). On the relation between the acquisition of singular–plural morpho-syntax and the conceptual distinction between one and more than one. Developmental Science 10(3), 365373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, N., Pogue, A. & Barner, D. (2011). Piecing together numerical language: children’s use of default units in early counting and quantification. Developmental Science 14(1), 4457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (1978). Less may never be more. In Campbell, R. & Smith, P. (eds), Recent advances in the psychology of language (pp. 109132). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Monaco, L., Trasciani, M. & Vicari, S. (2008). Langauge in Italian children with Down syndrome and with Specific Language Impairment. Neuropsychology 22(1), 2735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R. S., Schwartz, S. E. & Bird, E. K. (1991). Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 34(5), 11061120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coubart, A., Izard, V., Spelke, E. S., Marie, J., & Streri, A. (2014). Dissociation between small and large numerosities in newborn infants. Developmental Science 17(1), 1122. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, B., Xu Rattanasone, N. & Demuth, K. (2017). Two-year-olds’ sensitivity to inflectional plural morphology: allomorphic effects. Language Learning and Development 13(1), 3853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, B., Xu Rattanasone, N., Schembri, T. & Demuth, K. (2019). Preschoolers’ developing comprehension of the plural: the effects of number and allomorphic variation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 185, 95108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolscheid, S. & Penke, M. (2018). Quantifier comprehension is linked to linguistic rather than to numerical skills: evidence from children with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome. PLoS ONE 13(6), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, L. & Carey, S. (2005). On the limits of infants’ quantification of small object arrays. Cognition 97, 295313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, L., Carey, S. & Hauser, M. (2002). The representations underlying infants’ choice of more: object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychological Science 13(2), 11561501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, L., Carey, S. & Spelke, E. (2002). Infants’ discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology 44, 3366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galeote, M., Soto, P., Sebastián, E., Checa, E. & Sánchez-Palacios, C. (2014). Early grammatical development in Spanish children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Language 41(1), 111131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2009). Language development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Hyde, D. & Spelke, E. (2011). Neural signatures of number processing in humans infants: evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. Developmental Science 14(2), 360371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Im, H. Y., Zhong, S. hua & Halberda, J. (2016). Grouping by proximity and the visual impression of approximate number in random dot arrays. Vision Research 126, 291307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeffreys, H. (1961). The theory of probability. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Joffe, V. & Varlokosta, S. (2007). Language abilities in Williams syndrome: exploring comprehension, production and repetition skills. Advances in Speech–Language Pathology 9(3), 213225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolly, H. & Plunkett, K. (2008). Inflectional vootstrapping in 2-year-olds. Language and Speech 51, 4559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Justicia, F., Santiago, J., Palma, A., Huertas, D. & Gutiérrez, N. (1996). La frecuencia silábica del español escrito por niños: estudio estadístico. Cognitiva 8, 131168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., D’Souza, D., Dekker, T. M., Van Herwegen, J., Xu, F., Rodic, M. & Ansari, D. (2012). Genetic and environmental vulnerabilities in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(Supplement_2), 1726117265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, K. N. & Gerlanc, D. (2013). BootES: an R package for bootstrap confidence intervals on effect sizes. Behavior Research Methods 45, 905927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kouider, S., Halberda, J., Wood, J. & Carey, S. (2006). Acquisition of English number marking: the singular–plural distinction. Language Learning and Development 2(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kover, S. & Atwood, A. (2013). Establishing equivalence: methodological progress in group-matching design and analysis. American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 118(1), 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology 4(863). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazaro, M., Garayzabal, E. & Moraleda, E. (2013). Differences on morphological and phonological processing between typically developing children and children with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34(7), 20652074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Corre, M. & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, four, nothing more: an investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles. Cognition 105, 395438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Corre, M., Li, P., Huang, B. H., Jia, G. & Carey, S. (2016). Numerical morphology supports early number word learning: evidence from a comparison of young Mandarin and English learners. Cognitive Psychology 88, 162186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leibovich, T., Katzin, N., Harel, M. & Henik, A. (2017). From ‘sense of number’ to ‘sense of magnitude’: the role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40, e164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Ogura, T., Barner, D., Yang, S. & Carey, S. (2009). Does the conceptual distinction between singular and plural sets depend on language? Developmental Psychology 45(6), 16441653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukyanenko, C. & Fisher, C. (2016). Where are the cookies? Two- and three-year-olds use number-marked verbs to anticipate upcoming nouns. Cognition 146, 349370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odic, D., Pietroski, P., Hunter, T., Lidz, J. & Halberda, J. (2013). Children’s understanding of ‘more’ and discrimination of number and surface area. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 39(2), 451461.Google ScholarPubMed
Paterson, S. J., Girelli, L., Butterworth, B. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Are numerical impairments syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 190204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Paz, V. I., Arias-Trejo, N. & Alva Canto, E. A. (2016). La Influencia del número de objetos y las claves verbales en la distinción temprana del plural. Anales de Psicología 32(3), 863870.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1989). The acquisition of morphemes: some evidence from Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistics Research 2, 289312.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1995). Strategies in the acquisition of syntax. In Fletcher, J. & MacWhitnney, B. (eds), The handbook of child language (pp. 462482). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Plummer, P., DeWolf, M., Bassok, M., Gordon, P. C. & Holyoak, K. J. (2017). Reasoning strategies with rational numbers revealed by eye tracking. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 79(5), 14261437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, J. (2019). Discriminating quantity: New points for teaching children with Down syndrome about number? International Journal of Disability, Development and Education 66(2), 133150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouder, J. N., Love, J., Marwick, B. & Morey, R. (2015). BayesFactor: 0.9.12-2 CRAN. Online <https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.31202>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Sarnecka, B. W., Kamenskaya, V. G., Yamana, Y., Ogura, T. & Yudovina, Y. (2007). From grammatical number to exact numbers: early meaning of ‘one’, ‘two’, and ‘three’ in English, Russian, and Japanese. Cognitive Psychology 55, 136168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sattler, J. (2010). Evaluación infantil: fundamentos cognitivos (5th ed.). México: Manual Moderno.Google Scholar
Scott, K., Chu, J. & Schulz, L. (2017). Lookit (Part 2): assessing the viability of online developmental research, results from three case studies. Open Mind: Discoveries in Cognitive Science 1(1), 1529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sella, F., Lanfranchi, S. & Zorzi, M. (2013). Enumeration skills in Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 37983806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Herwegen, J., Ranzato, E., Karmiloff-Smith, A. & Simms, V. (2019). Eye movement patterns and approximate number sense task performance in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome: a developmental perspective. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 68, 40304038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Bates, E., Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F. & Volterra, V. (2004). Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: An example of a dissociation between language and cognition? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 10, 862886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C. & Tonucci, F. (2000). Asynchrony of lexical and morphosyntactic development in children with Down Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 38, 634644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walles, H., Robins, A. & Knott, A. (2014). A perceptually grounded model of the singular–plural distinction. Language and Cognition 6(3), 327369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2011). Escala Wechsler de Inteligencia para los niveles preescolar y primario-III. México: Manual Moderno.Google Scholar
Witecy, B. & Penke, M. (2017). Language comprehension in children, adolescents, and adults with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 62, 184196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, J., Kouider, S. & Carey, S. (2009). Acquisition of singular–plural morphology. Developmental Psychology 45(1), 202206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, F., & Spelke, E. (2000). Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition 74, B1B11. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00066-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed