Skip to main content
×
Home

Self-organization of foraging behaviour: From simplicity to complexity without goals

  • Frederick D Provenza (a1), Juan J Villalba (a1), Carl D Cheney (a2) and Scott J Werner (a1)
Abstract
Abstract

A herbivore faces challenges while foraging—ongoing changes in its physiological condition along with variation in the nutrient and toxin concentrations of foods, spatially and temporally—that make selecting a nutritious diet a vital affair. Foraging behaviours arise from simple rules that operate across levels of resolution from cells and organs to individuals and their interactions with social and physical environments. At all these levels, behaviour is a function of its consequences: a behaviour operating on the environment to induce changes is itself changed by those events. Thus, behaviour emerges from its own functioning—behaviour self-organizes-not from that of its surroundings. This ostensible autonomy notwith-standing, no self-organizing system (cell, organ, or individual) is independent of its environs because existence consists of an ongoing exchange of energy and matter. According to this view, the notion of cause and effect is replaced with functional relationships between behaviours and environmental consequences. Changes in physical environments alter the distribution, abundance, nutritional, and toxicological characteristics of plants, which affect food preference. Social interactions early in life influence behaviour in various ways: animals prefer familiar foods and environments, and they prefer to be with companions. Animals in unfamiliar environments often walk farther, ingest less food, and suffer more from malnutrition and toxicity than animals in familiar environments. An individual's food preferences—and its ability to discriminate familiar from novel foods—arise from the functional integration of sensory (smell, taste, texture) and postingestive (effects of nutrients and toxins on chemo-, osmo-, and mechano-receptors) effects. The ability to discriminate among foods is critical for survival: all problems with poisonous plants are due to an inability to discriminate or a lack of alternatives. Animals eat a variety of foods as a result of nearing or exceeding tolerance limits for sensory and postingestive effects unique to each food. After eating any food too frequently or excessively, the likelihood increases that animals will eat alternative foods owing to exceeding sensory-, nutrient-, and toxin-specific tolerance limits. Cyclic patterns of intake of a variety of foods reflect seemingly chaotic interactions among flavours, nutrients, and toxins interacting along continua.

    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      Self-organization of foraging behaviour: From simplicity to complexity without goals
      Available formats
      ×
      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      Self-organization of foraging behaviour: From simplicity to complexity without goals
      Available formats
      ×
      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      Self-organization of foraging behaviour: From simplicity to complexity without goals
      Available formats
      ×
Copyright
References
Hide All
Andelt W. F., Baker D. L. & Burnham K. P. (1992). Relative preference of captive cow elk for repellent-treated diets. Journal of Wildlife Management 56, 164173.
Andersen R. (1991). Habitat deterioration and the migratory behaviour of moose (Alces alces L.) in Norway. Journal of Applied Ecology 28, 102108.
Aoki C. & Siekevitz P. (1988). Plasticity in brain development. Scientific American 256(12). 5664.
Barber W. D. & Burks T. F. (1987). Brain-gut interactions: brain stem neuronal response to local gastric effects of substance P. American Journal of Physiology 253, G369G377.
Bazely D. R. (1989). Carnivorous herbivores: mineral nutrition and the balanced diet. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 4, 155156.
Berteaux D., Crete M., Huot J., Maltais J. & Ouellet J.-P. (1998). Food choice by white-tailed deer in relation to protein and energy content of the diet: a field experiment. Oecologia 115, 8492.
Biquand S. & Biquand-Guyot V. (1992). The influence of peers, lineage and environment on food selection of the criollo goat (Capra hircus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 34, 231245.
Birch L. L. & Deysher M. (1986). Caloric compensation and sensory specific satiety: evidence for self regulation of food intake by young children. Appetite 7, 323331.
Black J. L. & Kenney P. A. (1984). Factors affecting diet selection by sheep. II. Height and density of pasture. Australian Journal of Agricultural Research 35, 565578.
Bonabeau E., Theraulaz G., Deneubourg J. L., Aron S. & Camazine S. (1997). Self-organization in social insects. TREE 12, 188193.
Bond N. & Di Guisto E. (1975). Amount of solution drunk is a factor in the establishment of taste aversion. Animal Learning and Behavior 3, 8184.
Booth D. A. & Toase A. M. (1983). Conditioning of hunger/satiety signals as well as flavour cues in dieters. Appetite 4, 235236.
Brothers L. (1997). Friday's Footprint. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bryant J. P., Chapin F. S. & Klein D. R. (1983). Carbon/nutrient balance of boreal plants in relation to vertebrate herbivory. Oikos 40, 357368.
Bryant J. P., Provenza F. D., Pastor J., Reichardt P. B., Clausen T. P. & Du Toit J. T. (1991). Interactions between woody plants and browsing mammals mediated by secondary metabolites. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 22, 431446.
Bryant J. P., Reichardt P. B., Clausen T. P., Provenza F. D. & Kuropat P. J. (1992). Woody plant-mammal interactions. In Herbivores: their interactions with secondary plant metabolites. 2. Ecological and evolutionary processes, 2nd edn, pp. 343370 [Rosenthal G. A. and Berenbaum M. R., editors]. New York: Academic Press.
Burritt E. A. & Provenza F. D. (1989). Food aversion learning: ability of lambs to distinguish safe from harmful foods. Journal of Animal Science 67, 17321739.
Burritt E. A. & Provenza F. D. (1991). Ability of lambs to learn with a delay between food ingestion and consequences given meals containing novel and familiar foods. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32, 179189.
Burritt E. A. & Provenza F. D. (1992). Lambs form preferences for nonnutritive flavors paired with glucose. Journal of Animal Science 70, 11331136.
Burritt E. A. & Provenza F. D. (1996). Amount of experience and prior illness affect the acquisition and persistence of conditioned food aversions in lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 48, 7380.
Burritt E. A. & Provenza F. D. (1997). Effect of an unfamiliar location on the consumption of novel and familiar foods by sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54, 317325.
Cabanac M. (1971). Physiological role of pleasure. Science 173, 11031107.
Capra F. (1991). The Tao of Physics. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Capra F. (1996). The Web of Life. New York: Doubleday.
Caraco T., Blanckenhorn W. U., Gregory G. M., Newman J. A., Recer G. M. & Zwicker S. M. (1990). Risk-sensitivity: ambient temperature affects foraging choice. Animal Behaviour 39, 338345.
Chase L. E., Wangsness P. J., Kavanaugh J. F., Griel L. C. & Gahagan J. H. (1977). Changes in portal blood metabolites and insulin with feeding steers twice daily. Journal of Dairy Science 60, 403409.
Cheeke P. R. (1998). Natural Toxicants in Feeds, Forages, and Poisonous Plants. Danville IL: Interstate.
Cheney C. D. (1994). The source and control of behavior. In Human Behavior in Today's World, pp. 7386 [Waris Ishaq , editor]. New York: Praeger.
Chiesa M. (1994). Radical Behaviorism: the philosophy and the science. Boston, MA: Authors Cooperative Inc.
Coates K. P., Schemnitz S. D. & Peters J. T. (1991). Use of rodent middens as mineral licks by bighorn sheep. In Seventh Biennial Symposium, Northern Wild Sheep and Goat Council, pp. 206209 [Bailey J. A., editor]. Alberta: Northern Wild Sheep and Goat Council, Alberta Fish and Wildlife.
Cole B. J. (1994). Chaos and behavior: the perspective of nonlinear dynamics. In Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology, pp. 423443 [Real L. A., editor]. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Coley P. D., Bryant J. P. & Chapin F. S. (1985). Resource availability and plant antiherbivore defense. Science 230, 895899.
Cooper S. D. B., Kyriazakis I., Anderson D. H. & Oldham J. D. (1993). The effect of physiological state (late pregnancy) on the diet selection of ewes. Animal Production 56, 469 (abstr.).
Coppersmith R. & Leon M. (1984). Enhanced neural response to familiar olfactory cues. Science 225, 849851.
Cottrell D. F. & Iggo A. (1984). Mucosal enteroceptors with vagal afferent fibers in the proximal duodenum of sheep. Journal of Physiology 354, 497522.
Damasio A.R. (1994). Descartes' Error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon Books.
Day J. E. L., Kyriazakis I & Rogers P. J. (1998). Food choice and intake: towards a unifying framework of learning and feeding motivation. Nutrition Research Reviews 11, 2543.
De Jong A. (1981). The effect of feed intake on nutrient and hormone levels in jugular and portal blood in goats. Journal of Agricultural Science 96, 643657.
Demment M. W., Distel R. A., Griggs T. C., Laca E. A. & Deo G. P. (1993). Selective behaviour of cattle grazing ryegrass swards with horizontal heterogeneity in patch height and bulk density. In Proceedings, International Grassland Congress XVII, 712714.
Dennett D. (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea. New York: Norton.
Distel R. A. & Provenza F. D. (1991). Experience early in life affects voluntary intake of blackbrush by goats. Journal of Chemical Ecology 17, 431450.
Dohi H. & Yamada A. (1997). Preference of sheep and goats for extracts from high-grain concentrate. Journal of Animal Science 75, 20732077.
Drewnowski A., Grinker J. A. & Hirsch J. (1982). Obesity and flavor perception: multidimensional scaling of soft drinks. Appetite 3, 361368.
Dudzinski M. L., Müller W. J., Low W. A. & Schuh H.-J. (1982). Relationship between dispersion behaviour of free-ranging cattle and forage conditions. Applied Animal Ethology 8, 225241.
Dudzinski M. L., Schuh H.-J., Wilcox D. G., Gardiner H. G. & Morrissey J. G. (1978). Statistical and probabilistic estimators of forage conditions from grazing behaviour of merino sheep in a semi-arid environment. Applied Animal Ethology 4, 357368.
du Toit J. T., Provenza F. D & Nastis A. S. (1991). Conditioned taste aversions: how sick must a ruminant get before it learns about toxicity in foods? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 30, 3546.
Early D. M. & Provenza F. D. (1998). Food flavor and nutritional characteristics alter dynamics of food preference in lambs. Journal of Animal Science 76, 728734.
Egan A. R. (1980). Host animal-rumen relationships. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 39, 7987.
English-Loeb G., Stout M. J. & Duffey S. S. (1997). Drought stress in tomatoes: changes in plant chemistry and potential nonlinear consequences for insect herbivores. Oikos 79, 456468.
Evans E. J., Buchanan-Smith J. G. & Macleod G. K. (1975). Postprandial patterns of plasma glucose, insulin and volatile fatty acids in ruminants fed low- and high-roughage diets. Journal of Animal Science 41, 14741479.
Fisher D. S., Burns J. C. & Mayland H. F. (1997 a). Variation in preference for morning or afternoon harvested hay in sheep, goats, and cattle. Journal of Animal Science 75 (Suppl.), 201.
Fisher D. S., Mayland H. F. & Burns J. C. (1997 b). Diurnal timing of hay harvest and ruminant preferences for hays. Agronomy Abstracts p. 142.
Flores E. R., Provenza F. D. & Balph D. F. (1989). Role of experience in the development of foraging skills of lambs browsing the shrub serviceberry. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 23, 271278.
Freeland W. J. & Janzen D. H. (1974). Strategies in herbivory by mammals: the role of plant secondary compounds. American Naturalist 108, 269289.
Freeman W. J. (1991). The physiology of perception. Scientific American 264(2), 7885.
Friedman M. I. (1997). An energy sensor for control of energy intake. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 56, 4150.
Furness R. W. (1988). Predation on ground-nesting seabirds by island populations of red deer Cervus elaphus and sheep Ovis. Journal of Zoology 216, 565573.
Garcia J. (1989). Food for Tolman: cognition and cathexis in concert. In Aversion, Avoidance and Anxiety, pp. 4585 [Archer T. and Nilsson L., editors]. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Garcia J., Lasiter P. A., Bermudez-Rattoni F. & Deems D. A. (1985). A general theory of aversion learning. In Experimental Assessments and Clinical Applications of Conditioned Food Aversions, pp. 821 [Braveman N.S. and Bronstein P., editors]. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Gibson E. L. & Booth D. A. (1986). Acquired protein appetite in rats: dependence on a protein-specific need state. Experientia 42, 10031004.
Gibson E. L. & Booth D. A. (1989). Dependence of carbohydrate-conditioned flavor preference on internal state in rats. Learning and Motivation 20, 3647.
Glass L., Beuter A. & Larocque D. (1988). Time delays, oscillations, and chaos in physiological control systems. Mathematical Biosciences 90, 111125.
Glass L. & Mackey M. C. (1988). From Clocks to Chaos: the rhythms of life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Glass L. & Malta C. P. (1990). Chaos in multi-looped negative feedback systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology 145, 217223.
Gluesing E. A. & Balph D. F. (1980). An aspect of feeding behavior and its importance to grazing systems. Journal of Range Management 33, 426427.
Gordon I. J., Illius A. W. & Milne J. D. (1996). Sources of variation in the foraging efficiency of grazing ruminants. Functional Ecology 10, 219226.
Goss S., Beckers R., Deneubourg J. L., Aron S. & Pasteels J. M. (1990). How trail laying and trail following can solve foraging problems for ant colonies. In Behavioural Mechanism of Food Selection (NATO ASI Series G: Ecological Sciences, vol. 20). pp. 661677 [Hughes R.N., editor.] Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Gould S. J. (1989). Wonderful Life: the Burgess Shale and the nature of history. New York: Norton.
Green G. C., Elwin R. L., Mottershead B. E. & Lynch J. J. (1984). Long-term effects of early experience to supplementary feeding in sheep. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 15, 373375.
Green H. H. (1925). Perverted appetites. Physiological Reviews 5, 336348.
Griffith B., Scott J. M., Carpenter J. W. & Reed C. (1989). Translocation as a species conservation tool: status and strategy. Science 245, 477480.
Hobbs N. T. (1996). Modification of ecosystems by ungulates. Journal of Wildlife Management 60, 695713.
Hollick M. (1993). Self-organizing systems and environmental management. Environmental Management 17, 621628.
Howery L. D., Provenza F. D., Banner R. E. & Scott C. B. (1996). Differences in home range and habitat use among individuals in a cattle herd. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 49, 305320.
Howery L. D., Provenza F. D., Banner R. E. & Scott C. B. (1998). Social and environmental factors influence cattle distribution on rangeland. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 55, 231244.
Hunter R. F. & Milner C. (1963). The behaviour of individual, related and groups of south country Cheviot hill sheep. Animal Behaviour 11, 507513.
Illius A. W. & Hodgson J. (1996). Progress in understanding the ecology and management of grazing systems. In The Ecology and Management of Grazing Systems, pp. 429457 [Hodgson J. & Illius A.W., editors]. Wallingford: CAB International.
Johnson H. B. & Mayeux H. S. (1992). Viewpoint: A view on species additions and deletions and the balance of nature. Journal of Range Management 45, 322333.
Kalat J. W. (1974). Taste salience depends on novelty, not concentration, in taste-aversion learning in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86, 4750.
Kalat J. W. & Rozin P. (1970). ‘Salience’: A factor which can override temporal contiguity in taste-aversion learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 71, 192197.
Kalil R. E. (1989). Synapse formation in the developing brain. Scientific American 256 (12), 5664.
Kamil A. C., Misthal R. L. & Stephens D. W. (1993). Failure of simple optimal foraging models to predict residence time when patch quality is uncertain. Behavioral Ecology 4, 350363.
Kaplan M. L. & Kaplan N. R. (1991). The self-organization of human psychological functioning. Behavioral Science 36, 161178.
Kauffman S. (1995). At Home in the Universe: the search for laws of self-organization and complexity. London: Penguin Books.
Kaufmann W. J. (1994). Universe, 4th edn. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Keeler R. F., Baker D. C. & Evans J. O. (1988). Individual animal susceptibility and its relationship to induced adaption or tolerance in sheep to Galega officinalis. Letters in Veterinary and Human Toxicology 30, 420423.
Keeler R. F., Baker D. C. & Panter K. E. (1992). Concentration of galegine in Verbesina encelioides and Galega officinalis and the toxic and pathologic effects induced by the plants. Journal of Environmental Pathology, Toxicology and Oncology 11, 7581.
Kelsall J. P. (1968). The Migratory Barren-Ground Caribou of Canada. Ottawa: Canadian Wildlife Service.
Kelso J. A. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kennedy M. & Gray R. D. (1993). Can ecological theory predict the distribution of foraging animals? A critical analysis of experiments on the Ideal Free Distribution. Oikos 68, 158166.
Key C. & Maclver R. M. (1980). The effects of maternal influences on sheep: breed differences in grazing, resting and courtship behaviour. Applied Animal Ethology 6, 3348.
Kimball B. A. (1997). Chemical Ecology of Vascular Tissue Foraging by Black Bears. PhD thesis, Colorado State University.
Konarzewski M. & Diamond J. (1994). Peak sustained metabolic rate and its individual variation in cold-stressed mice. Physiological Zoology 67, 11861212.
Kyriazakis I. & Oldham J. D. (1993). Diet selection in sheep: the ability of growing lambs to select a diet that meets their crude protein (nitrogen × 6.25) requirements. British Journal of Nutrition 69, 617629.
Kyriazakis I. & Oldham J. D. (1997). Food intake and diet selection in sheep: the effect of manipulating the rates of digestion of carbohydrates and protein of the foods offered as a choice. British Journal of Nutrition 77, 243254.
Kyriazakis I., Oldham J. D., Coop R. L. & Jackson F. (1994). The effect of subclinical intestinal nematode infection on the diet selection of growing sheep. British Journal of Nutrition 72, 665677.
Langvatn R. & Hanley T. A. (1993). Feeding-patch choice by red deer in relation to foraging efficiency-an experiment. Oecologia 95, 164170.
Launchbaugh K. L. & Provenza F. D. (1993). Can plants practice mimicry to avoid grazing by mammalian herbivores? Oikos 66, 501504.
Launchbaugh K. L., Provenza F. D. & Burritt E. A. (1993). How herbivores track variable environments: Response to variability of phytotoxins. Journal of Chemical Ecology 19, 10471056.
Launchbaugh K. L., Provenza F. D. & Werkmeister M. J. (1997). Overcoming food neophobia in domestic ruminants through addition of a familiar flavor and repeated exposure to novel foods. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 54, 327334.
Lawler I. R., Stapley J., Foley W. J. & Eschler B. M. (1998). An ecological example of conditioned flavor aversion in plant-herbivore interactions: the effect of terpenes of Eucalyptus leaves on feeding by common ringtail and brushtail possums. Journal of Chemical Ecology in press.
Leek B. F. (1977). Abdominal and pelvic visceral receptors. British Medical Bulletin 33, 163168.
McArthur C., Hagerman A. E. & Robbins C. T. (1991). Physiological strategies of mammalian herbivores against plant defenses. In Plant Defenses Against Mammalian Herbivory, pp. 103114 [Palo R.T. and Robins C.T., editors]. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Mach E. (1960). The Science of Mechanics: a critical and historical account of its development. (Original English translation published 1893). LaSalle: Open Court Publishing.
Maclean N. (1976). A River Runs through It, and other stories. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
McNulty S. A., Porter W. F., Mathews N. E. & Hill J. A. (1997). Localized management for reducing white-tailed deer populations. Wildlife Society Bulletin 25, 265271.
Maturana H. R. & Varela F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: the realization of the living. Dordrecht: Reidel.
May R. M. (1986). When two and two do not make four: nonlinear phenomena in ecology. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 228, 241266.
Mehansho H., Hagerman A., Clements S., Butler L., Rogler J. & Carlson D. M. (1983). Modulation of proline-rich protein biosynthesis in rat parotid glands by sorghums with high tannin levels. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 80, 39483952.
Mei N. (1985). Intestinal chemosensitivity. Physiological Reviews 65, 211237.
Millonas M. M. (1992). A connectionist type model of self-organized foraging and emergent behaviour in ant swarms. Journal of Theoretical Biology 159, 529552.
Mirza S. N. & Provenza F. D. (1990). Preference of the mother affects selection and avoidance of foods by lambs differing in age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 28, 255263.
Mirza S. N. & Provenza F. D. (1992). Effects of age and conditions of exposure on maternally mediated food selection by lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 33, 3542.
Morris R. (1993). Cosmic Questions. New York: Wiley.
Naim M., Brand J. G. & Kare M. R. (1986). Role of variety of food flavor in fat deposition produced by ‘cafeteria’ feeding of nutritionally controlled diets. In Interaction of the Chemical Senses with Nutrition, pp. 269292 [Kare M.R. and Brand J.G., editors]. New York: Academic Press.
Nolte D. L. & Provenza F. D. (1991). Food preferences in lambs after exposure to flavours in milk. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 32, 381389.
Nolte D. L., & Provenza F. D., Callan R. & Panter K. E. (1992). Garlic in the ovine fetal environment. Physiology & Behavior 52, 10911093.
O'Reagain P. J., Goetsch B. C. & Owen-Smith R. N. (1996). Effect of species composition and sward structure on the ingestive behaviour of cattle and sheep grazing South African sourveld. Journal of Agricultural Science 127, 271280.
Ortega-Reyes L. & Provenza F. D. (1993). Amount of experience and age affect the development of foraging skills of goats browsing blackbrush (Coleogyne ramosissima). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 36, 169183.
Ortega-Reyes L., Provenza F. D., Parker C. F. & Hatfield P. G. (1992). Drylot performance and ruminal papillae development of lambs exposed to a high concentrate diet while nursing. Small Ruminant Research 7, 101112.
Owen-Smith R. N. (1993). Evaluating optimal diet models for an African browsing ruminant, the kudu: how constraining are the assumed constraints? Evolutionary Ecology 7, 499524.
Perez C., Ackroff K. & Sclafani A. (1996). Carbohydrate- and protein-conditioned flavor preferences: effects of nutrient preloads. Physiology and Behavior 59; 467474.
Pfister J. A., Provenza F. D., Manners G. D., Gardner D. R. & Ralphs M. H. (1997). Tall larkspur ingestion: Can cattle regulate intake below toxic levels? Journal of Chemical Ecology 23, 759777.
Phillips R. J. & Powley T. L. (1996). Gastric volume rather than nutrient content inhibits food intake. American Journal of Physiology 271, R766R779.
Piersma T. & Lindstrom A. (1997). Rapid reversible changes in organ size as a component of adaptive behaviour. TREE 12, 134138.
Prigogine I. (1980). From Being to Becoming: time and complexity in the physical sciences. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Prigogine I. & Stengers I. (1984). Order Out of Chaos: man's new dialogue with nature. New York: Bantam Books.
Provenza F. D. (1977). Biological Manipulation of Blackbrush (Coleogyne ramosissima Torr.) by Browsing with Goats. MS thesis, Utah State University.
Provenza F. D. (1995). Postingestive feedback as an elementary determinant of food preference and intake in ruminants. Journal of Range Management 48, 217.
Provenza F. D. (1996). Acquired aversions as the basis for varied diets of ruminants foraging on rangelands. Journal of Animal Science 74, 20102020.
Provenza F. D. (1997). Feeding behavior of animals in response to plant toxicants. In CRC Handbook of Plant and Fungal Toxicants, pp. 231242 [D'Mello J.P.F., editor]. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press Inc.
Provenza F. D. & Balph D. F. (1988). Development of dietary choice in livestock on rangelands and its implications for management. Journal of Animal Science 66, 23562368.
Provenza F. D. & Balph D. F. (1990). Applicability of five diet-selection models to various foraging challenges ruminants encounter. In Behavioural Mechanisms of Food Selection (NATO ASI Series G: Ecological Sciences, vol. 20) pp. 423459 [Hughes R.N., editor]. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Provenza F. D. & Burritt E. A. (1991). Socially-induced diet preference ameliorates conditioned food aversion in lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 31, 229236.
Provenza F. D., Burritt E. A., Clausen T. P., Bryant J. P., Reichardt P. B. & Distel R. A. (1990). Conditioned flavor aversion: a mechanism for goats to avoid condensed tannins in blackbrush. American Naturalist 136, 810828.
Provenza F. D. & Cincotta R. P. (1993). Foraging as a self-organizational learning process: accepting adaptability at the expense of predictability. In Diet Selection: an interdisciplinary approach to foraging behaviour. pp. 78101 [Hughes R.N., editor]. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Provenza F. D., Lynch J. J., Burritt E. A. & Scott C. B. (1994 a). How goats learn to distinguish between novel foods that differ in postingestive consequences. Journal of Chemical Ecology 20, 609624.
Provenza F. D., Lynch J. J. & Cheney C. D. (1995). Effects of a flavor and food restriction on the response to novel foods by sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 43, 8393.
Provenza F. D., Lynch J. J. & Nolan J. V. (1993 a). The relative importance of mother and toxicosis in the selection of foods by lambs. Journal Chemical Ecology 19, 313323.
Provenza F. D., Lynch J. J., & Nolan J. V. (1994 b). Food aversion conditioned in anesthetized sheep. Physiology & Behavior 55, 429432.
Provenza F. D., Nolan J. V & Lynch J. J. (1993 b). Temporal contiguity between food ingestion and toxicosis affects the acquisition of food aversions in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38, 269281.
Provenza F. D., Pfister J. A. & Cheney C. D. (1992). Mechanisms of learning in diet selection with reference to phytotoxicosis in herbivores. Journal of Range Management 45, 3645.
Provenza F. D., Scott C. B., Phy T. S. & Lynch J. J. (1996). Preference of sheep for foods varying in flavors and nutrients. Journal of Animal Science 74, 23552361.
Provenza F. D., Villalba J. J. & Augner M. (1998). The physics of foraging. Proceedings, International Grassland Congress XVIII, in press.
Ralphs M. R. (1997). Persistence of aversions to larkspur in naive and native cattle. Journal of Range Management 50, 367370.
Ramirez I. (1997). Intragastric carbohydrate exerts both intake-stimulating and intake-suppressing effects. Behavioral Neuroscience 111. 612622.
Revusky S. H. & Bedarf E. W. (1967). Association of illness and prior ingestion of novel foods. Science 155, 219220.
Ricardo J. A. & Koh E. T. (1978). Anatomical evidence of direct projections from the nucleus of the solitary tract to the hypothalamus, amygdala, and other forebrain structures in the rat. Brain Research 153, 126.
Rolls B. J. (1986 a). Sensory-specific satiety. Nutrition Reviews 44, 93101.
Rolls B. J., Van Duijvenvoorde P. M. & Rolls E. T. (1984). Pleasantness changes and food intake in a varied four-course meal. Appetite 5, 337348.
Rolls E. T. (1986 b). Neuronal activity related to the control of feeding. In Feeding Behavior, Neural and Humoral Controls, pp. 163190 [Ritter R. C., Ritter S. and Barnes C. D., editors]. New York: Academic Press.
Ryle G. (1949). The Concept of Mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Sclafani A. (1996). How food preferences are learned: laboratory animal models. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 54, 419427.
Scott C. B., Banner R. E. & Provenza F. D. (1996). Observations of sheep foraging in familiar and unfamiliar environments: familiarity with the environment influences diet selection. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 49, 165171.
Scott C. B., Provenza F. D. & Banner R. E. (1995). Dietary habits and social interactions affect choice of feeding location by sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 45, 225237.
Scott L. L. & Provenza F. D. (1998). Variation among sheep in food selection: effects of basal ration and foods offered in a meal. Journal of Animal Science in press.
Scott T. R. (1990). Gustatory control of food selection. In Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, vol. 10. pp. 243263 [Stricker E.M., editor]. New York: Plenum Press.
Seynaeve C., De Mulder P. H. M. & Verweij J. (1991). Pathophysiology of cytotoxic drug-induced emesis: far from crystal-clear. Pharmaceutisch Weekblad, Scientific Edition 13, 16.
Shatz C. J. (1992). The developing brain. Scientific American 267 (3), 6167.
Simpson S. J. & Raubenheimer D. (1993). A multi-level analysis of feeding behaviour: the geometry of nutritional decisions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences 342, 381402.
Skinner B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms: an experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science 213, 501504.
Skinner B. F. (1984).The evolution of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 41, 217221.
Solé R.V., Miramontes O. & Goodwin B. C. (1993). Oscillations and chaos in ant societies. Journal of Theoretical Biology 161, 343357.
Squibb R. C., Provenza F. D. & Balph D. F. (1990). Effect of age of exposure on consumption of a shrub by sheep. Journal of Animal Science 68, 987997.
Stebbins G.L. (1981). Coevolution of grasses and herbivores. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden 68, 7586.
Stickland T. R., Britton N. F. & Franks N. R. (1995). Complex trails and simple algorithms in ant foraging. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 260, 5358.
Sutcliffe A. J. (1977). Further notes on bones and antlers chewed by deer and other ungulates. Journal of the British Deer Society 4, 7382.
Swithers S. E. & Hall W. G. (1994). Does oral experience terminate ingestion? Appetite 23, 113138.
Swithers-Mulvey S. E., Miller G. L. & Hall W. G. (1991). Habituation of oromotor responding to oral infusions in rat pups. Appetite 17, 5567.
Tausch R. J., Wigand P. E. & Burkhardt J. W. (1993). Viewpoint. Plant community thresholds, multiple steady states, and multiple successional pathways: legacy of the Quaternary? Journal of Range Management 46, 439447.
Thorhallsdottir A. G., Provenza F. D. & Balph D. F. (1990). Social influences on conditioned food aversions in sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 25, 4550.
Tien D. V., Lynch J. J., Hinch G. N. & Nolan J. V. (1998). Grass odor and flavour overcome feed neophobia in sheep. Small Ruminunt Research in press.
Toates F. M. (1997). Has pure ethology the right foundations for applied ethology? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53, 113.
Van Soest P. J. (1994). Nutritional Ecology of the Ruminant, 2nd edn. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
van Wieren S.E. (1996). Do large herbivores select a diet that maximizes short-term energy intake? Forest Ecology and Management 88, 149156.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1996). Preference for flavored wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal administrations of sodium propionate. Journal of Animal Science 74, 23622368.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1997 a). Preference for wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal infusions of starch. British Journal of Nutrition 77, 287297.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1997 b). Preference for flavoured foods by lambs conditioned with intraruminal administration of nitrogen. Brirish Journal of Nutrition 78, 545561.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1997 c). Preference for flavoured wheat straw by lambs conditioned with intraruminal infusions of acetate and propionate. Journal of Animal Science 75, 29052914.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1998 a). Nutrient-specific preferences by lambs conditioned with intraruminal infusions of starch, casein, and water. Journal of Animal Science in press.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1998 b). Discriminating among foods: effects of energy provision on preferences of lambs for poor-quality foods. Applied Animal Behaviour Science in press.
Villalba J. J. & Provenza F. D. (1998 c). Effects of food structure and nutritional quality and animal nutritional state on intake behaviour and food preferences of sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science in press.
Wallis de Vries M. F. (1994). Foraging in a Landscape Mosaic: Diet Selecfion and Performance of Free-Ranging Cattle in Heathland and Riverine Grassland. PhD thesis, Wageningen.
Wang J. & Provenza F. D. (1996 a). Food preference and acceptance of novel foods by lambs depend on the composition of the basal diet. Journal of Animal Science 74. 23492354.
Wang J. & Provenza F. D. (1996 b). Food deprivation affects preference of sheep for foods varying in nutrients and a toxin. Journal of Chemical Ecology 22, 20112021.
Wang J. & Provenza F. D. (1997). Dynamics of preference by sheep offered foods varying in flavors, nutrients, and a toxin. Journal of Chemical Ecology 23, 275288.
Warren J. T. & Mysterud I. (1993). Extensive ranging by sheep released onto an unfamiliar range. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38, 6773.
Westoby M. (1978). What are the biological bases of varied diets? American Naturalist 112, 627631.
Wilmshurst J. F., Fryxell J. M. & Hudson R. J. (1995). Forage quality and patch choice by wapiti (Cervus elaphus). Behavioral Ecology 6, 209217.
Wilson E. O. (1998). Consilience: the unity of knowledge. New York: Alfred A. Knopf Inc.
Wooley O. W., Wooley S. C. & Dunham R. B. (1972). Calories and sweet taste: effects on sucrose preference in the obese and nonobese. Physiology and Behavior 9, 765768.
Wu J. & Loucks O. L. (1995). From balance of nature to hierarchical patch dynamics: a paradigm shift in ecology. Quarterly Review of Biology 70, 439466.
Yuan C.-S. & Barber W. D. (1992). Hypothalamic unitary responses to gastric vagal input from the proximal stomach. American Journal of Physiology 262, G74G80.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Nutrition Research Reviews
  • ISSN: 0954-4224
  • EISSN: 1475-2700
  • URL: /core/journals/nutrition-research-reviews
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 91 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 182 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 19th November 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.