We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
Article purchase
Temporarily unavailable
Footnotes
†
Send reprint requests to the author, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA, 94720.
References
Bullock, M. and Gelman, R. (1979), “Preschool Children's Assumptions about Cause and Effect: Temporal Ordering”, Child Development50: 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983), The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/ MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977), Ontogeny and Phytogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1993), “Molyneux's Babies: Cross-Modal Perception, Imitation, and the Mind of the Preverbal Infant”, in Eilan, N., McArthy, R., and Brewer, B. (ed.) Spatial Representation: Problems in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Gopnik, A. (1993), “The Role Of Imitation In Understanding Persons And Developing A Theory Of Mind”, in Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. (ed.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 335–366.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1995), “A Theory of the Role of Imitation in the Emergence of Self”, in Rochat, P. (ed.), The Self in Infancy. Amsterdam: North-Holland-Elsevier Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1929), The Child's Conception of the World. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar