Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:49:33.985Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ethno-Religious Identification and Support for Interreligious Violence: A Study of Muslim and Christian Students in Indonesia and the Philippines

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2021

Agnieszka Kanas*
Affiliation:
Erasmus University Rotterdam
Peer Scheepers
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
Carl Sterkens
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Agnieszka Kanas, Department of Public Administration and Sociology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Postbus 1738 3000 DR Rotterdam, the Netherlands. E-mail: kanas@essb.eur.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study examines mechanisms and conditions under which ethnoreligious identification is related to support for out-group violence. It uses unique survey data collected among religious minorities and majorities in conflict and non-conflict regions in Indonesia and the Philippines. We find that strong ethno-religious identification is positively related to support for out-group violence. This relationship is fully mediated by the perception of out-group threat, suggesting that ethno-religious identification facilitates the perception of out-group threat, which, in turn, is positively related to support of violence. While the experience of communal violence increases support for interreligious violence, it does not influence the relationship between perceived group threat and support for violence. Interestingly, there is some evidence that the negative influence of intergroup contact on violence support is weaker for those who experienced communal violence.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Religion and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

INTRODUCTION

Ethno-religious-based conflicts are disturbingly prevalent in all parts of the world. A recent study of the PEW Research Center shows that communal violence had increased over the period between 2010 and 2017, with 56 out of 200 countries reporting high or very high social hostilities involving ethnicity and religion in 2017 (PEW 2019). In Ambon, the Moluccas in Indonesia, one of the regions examined in this study, the communal violence between Christians and Muslims in 1999–2000 caused an estimated 5,000 deaths and roughly 700,000 displacements (International Crisis Group 2002). Communal tensions escalated again into violence in September 2011 when a Muslim motorcycle driver unexpectedly died in a Christian area in Ambon (International Crisis Group 2012a), exposing the vulnerability of the region's reconciliation process.

Likewise, the Philippines has been locked in a continuous conflict on its southern island of Mindanao involving terrorist violence and military attacks that have led to thousands of killed and displaced millions (Zartman and Hopmann Reference Zartman and Hopmann2011: 199). The establishment of the Bangsamoro Autonomous Region in Muslim Mindanao (BARMM) in March 2019 can be seen as the culmination of a decades-long peace process between the Philippine government and the Moro Islamic Liberation Front (MILF). However, violence, kidnapping for ransom, and communal tensions continued after the destruction and depopulation of Marawi city––one of our research locations––after a 5-month battle in 2017. In mid-January 2020, the Philippine military rescued Indonesian fishers who were taken hostage by suspected Islamic State (ISIS)-affiliated Abu Sayyaf Group (ASG) militants a few months earlier in Sulu province. After liberation, suspected ASG militants abducted immediately five other Indonesians fishing in Philippine waters. Philippine military and Moro National Liberation Front factions continued to clash with the Philippine military in Sulu, Tawi Tawi Basilan, and North Cotabato provinces (International Crisis Group 2019, 2020b).

Although ethno-religious differences are not the sole reason for conflicts in the Moluccas (Indonesia) and Mindanao (Philippines), ethnic and religious identification together with social, economic, and political inequalities between the indigenous population and internal migrants from other parts of the country are considered important triggers of conflict in both countries (Zartman and Hopmann Reference Zartman and Hopmann2011; Schulze Reference Schulze2017).

Given the prevalence of ethno-religious-based prejudice and violence in many regions of the world, finding mechanisms and conditions explaining out-group prejudice and the violent forms it can take is of critical importance. This research is guided by two lines of inquiry: efforts to identify important mechanisms explaining the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for interreligious violence, as well as conditions under which these explanatory processes take place. Although a great deal of empirical research has examined the relationship between ethnic and religious identification and out-group prejudice (see Jackson and Hunsberger Reference Jackson and Hunsberger1999; Scheepers and Eisinga Reference Scheepers and Eisinga2015 for overviews of empirical findings), considerably less attention has been devoted to mechanisms and conditions under which these identities are related to violent behavior. Based on Social Identity Theory (SIT) (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979; Tajfel Reference Tajfel1982), we expect ethno-religious identification to be positively related to support for out-group violence. We set out to include mechanisms derived from ethnic group conflict theory (Scheepers, Gijsberts, and Coenders Reference Scheepers, Gijsberts and Coenders2002) and contact theory (Pettigrew and Tropp Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006), explaining the role of ethno-religious identification in support for out-group violence. In addition, we examine these sets of relationships while conditioning on the actual exposure to intergroup conflict, which is known to affect support for violence (e.g., Rouhana and Bar-Tal Reference Rouhana and Bar-Tal1998; Bauman and Leech Reference Bauman and Leech2012).

Previous research on the link between religious identification and out-group prejudice has predominantly focused on ethnic and religious majorities in Western countries, namely White Christians in the United States (see Scheepers and Eisinga Reference Scheepers and Eisinga2015 for an overview). Only a few studies have focused on prejudicial attitudes among religious minorities, but again these studies were mainly concerned with Western countries (Verkuyten Reference Verkuyten2007; Verkuyten and Yildiz Reference Verkuyten and Yildiz2007; Reference Verkuyten and Yildiz2010; Ysseldyk et al. Reference Ysseldyk, Haslam, Matheson and Anisman2012). Thus, there is a gap in our knowledge, whether group identification processes are equally important for majorities and minorities in social contexts where intergroup relations are influenced by severe communal conflict. This study contributes to previous research by examining the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for interreligious violence among both religious majorities and minorities, moreover, in contexts where relations between the two largest religious communities—Christians and Muslims—are far from harmonious.

RESEARCH CONTEXT

In this study, we focus on four ethnically and religiously diverse regions, i.e., Maluku and Yogyakarta (Central Java) in Indonesia, and in and around what is since 2019 called the BARMM and Metro-Manila in the Philippines. These four regions of Indonesia and the Philippines provide a unique opportunity to answer our research questions. In all regions, there are sizable ethnic and religious communities, which are alternating in the majority and minority position (at both local and national level), power and status relations, and some regions have histories of communal conflicts. More specifically, while the majority of the population in Indonesia and Yogyakarta is Javanese Muslim, Ambon island has long been a mainly Ambonese Christian region, albeit with a substantial Ambonese Muslim population and other Muslim migrant groups.Footnote 1 Moreover, Muslims, who in Indonesia have the most political power because of their sheer number, have, on average lower socio-economic status than Christians, who are a minority group with little political influence. Still, Christians are being perceived as gaining too much strength in political and economic domains (Mujiburrahman 2006; Van Bruinessen Reference Van Bruinessen2018; Setiawan et al. Reference Setiawan, Scheepers, de Jong and Sterkens2021), in line with the observation that in poor economic circumstances, the dominant group usually blames the subordinate group for economic hardship (Quillian Reference Quillian1995).

In the Philippines, the vast majority of ethnic groups are Christian (mainly Roman Catholic). In contrast, in BARMM, the majority of the population belong to the Bangsamoro and are Muslims. Moreover, Christians in the Philippines enjoy both political power and have higher socio-economic status than Muslims, regardless of the region. Finally, whereas Yogyakarta and Metro-Manila have had relatively peaceful histories of relations between ethno-religious groups, Maluku and BARMM have had severe ethno-religious conflicts.Footnote 2

ETHNO-RELIGIOUS IDENTIFICATION AND SUPPORT FOR OUT-GROUP VIOLENCE

SIT offers a particularly useful approach to understand the relationship between ethno-religious group membership and intergroup relations (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979; Tajfel Reference Tajfel1982). According to SIT, group membership provides knowledge and emotional value, which are central to achieve a sense of self-concept (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979; Roccas et al. Reference Roccas, Sagiv, Schwartz, Halevy and Eidelson2008; Ysseldyk, Matheson, and Anisman Reference Ysseldyk, Matheson and Anisman2010). Moreover, social identification with a group motivates individuals to distinguish their group from others to achieve and maintain positive self-esteem (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979; Tajfel Reference Tajfel1982; Ysseldyk, Matheson, and Anisman Reference Ysseldyk, Matheson and Anisman2010; Spears Reference Spears, Schwartz, Luyckx and Vignoles2011). Concerning religious groups, the need for achieving and preserving self-esteem is likely to increase beliefs that one's religion teaches the absolute truth or is a unique source of morality, which reinforces “us” versus “them” distinctions and is likely to foster perceived superiority, i.e., in-group glorification relative to other religious groups (Rowatt et al. Reference Rowatt, Ottenbreit, Nesselroade and Cunningham2002; Roccas, Klar, and Liviatan Reference Roccas, Klar and Liviatan2006).

While the positive relationship between in-group identification and in-group favoritism does not necessarily induce out-group derogation, this may happen when groups find themselves in a conflicting situation or when the in-group's identity is threatened by the out-group (Hinkle and Brown Reference Hinkle and Brown1990; Brewer Reference Brewer1999). In both Indonesia and the Philippines, social cleavages occur along ethnic and religious lines, and in some regions, there is ongoing communal conflict and violence. Based on the previous, we expect that because of these tensions, ethno-religious identification will be positively related to support of interreligious violence (Hypothesis 1).

PERCEIVED AND ACTUAL GROUP THREAT AND THE QUALITY OF INTERGROUP CONTACT

According to ethnic group conflict theory, actual or perceived competition over scarce resources such as affordable housing or secure employment and conflict over cultural values between groups create circumstances in which identification with an in-group may feed perceptions of out-group threat (Scheepers, Gijsberts, and Coenders Reference Scheepers, Gijsberts and Coenders2002).

In both countries, there are regions with strong resentments between ethnic Christians and Muslims who differ in socio-economic status and political influence and have been involved in hostilities and sectarian conflict, at least in some regions. Therefore, we expect ethno-religious identification to be positively related to perceptions of out-group threat, which, in turn, is positively related to support for violence. Previous research has shown that intergroup threat plays a central mediating role in fostering prejudice and support for intergroup conflict (e.g., Tausch et al. Reference Tausch, Hewstone, Kenworthy, Cairns and Christ2007; Schmid et al. Reference Schmid, Tausch, Hewstone, Hughes and Cairns2008; Stephan, Ybarra, and Morrison Reference Stephan, Ybarra and Morrison2009; Maoz and McCauley Reference Maoz and McCauley2009). However, relatively little attention has been paid to the mediating role of intergroup threat across regions with different intergroup conflict levels. Based on previous studies, we consider this mediating role of out-group threat and argue that the perception of threat should be thought of as a key determinant of support for out-group violence.

Moreover, we propose that this relationship may be stronger, particularly for those who have directly experienced communal violence, such as losing a home or beloved ones (e.g., Rouhana and Bar-Tal Reference Rouhana and Bar-Tal1998; Bauman and Leech Reference Bauman and Leech2012). Based on the preceding, we hypothesize that perceived out-group threat explains (some of) the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence on behalf of the ethno-religious in-group (Hypothesis 2). In addition, we expect that the positive relationship between perceived out-group threat and support for out-group violence is stronger for those who experienced actual violence than those who did not (Hypotheses 3).

Contact theory provides an additional mechanism explaining the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for intergroup violence. According to contact theory, more contact between ethno-religious groups is likely to reduce out-group prejudice (Allport Reference Allport1954). There is ample evidence that intergroup contact improves attitudes towards the out-group (Pettigrew and Tropp Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006; Pettigrew et al. Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011). Moreover, it has been argued and shown that the positive effect of intergroup contact is particularly strong for the quality of contact, which fulfills Allport's (Reference Allport1954) conditions for optimal contact effect, namely equal status, common goals, cooperation, and institutional support (Pettigrew et al. Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011).

Whereas most of the literature on intergroup contact has focused on Western countries, there is growing empirical evidence for a positive role of intergroup contact in non-western contexts, with a history of intergroup violence and where contemporary intergroup relations remain tense (Tausch, Hewstone, and Roy Reference Tausch, Hewstone and Roy2009; Rydgren, Sofi, and Hällsten Reference Rydgren, Sofi and Hällsten2013; Kanas, Scheepers, and Sterkens Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2015; Kanas, Scheepers, and Sterkens Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2017b; Putra and Wagner Reference Putra and Wagner2017).

In this study, we follow up on this recently growing research and examine the hypothesis that the lower quality of intergroup contact can partly explain the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence. The principal reason for this expectation is that people who strongly identify with their ethno-religious in-group may abstain from developing and maintaining high-quality intergroup contact to avoid consequences of social and institutional sanctions, as it is in the case of interfaith marriage in Indonesia (Kanas, Scheepers, and Sterkens Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2017b). In this way, the (lack of) intergroup contact is driven by a preference for in-group contact rather than negativity towards the out-group (Jackson and Hunsberger Reference Jackson and Hunsberger1999; Cairns et al. Reference Cairns, Kenworthy, Campbell and Hewstone2006; Verkuyten and Yildiz Reference Verkuyten and Yildiz2007; Licata et al. Reference Licata, Klein, Saade, Azzi and Branscombe2012; Ysseldyk et al. Reference Ysseldyk, Haslam, Matheson and Anisman2012).

More generally, in a historical context of communal violence and ongoing tensions between Christians and Muslims, we expect ethno-religious group members to have a lower quality of intergroup contacts and, in some cases, avoid intergroup contact whatsoever, as such contact is likely to be associated with disloyalty to the in-group (Rydgren, Sofi, and Hällsten Reference Rydgren, Sofi and Hällsten2013; Kanas, Scheepers, and Sterkens Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2016).

Crucially, however, we add that the partial explanation for the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for intergroup violence due to the quality of intergroup contact may depend on individuals' prior exposure to communal conflict. Specifically, we expect that the quality of intergroup contact is less important when it is susceptible to peer pressure and fear of repercussions, which is often the case in a situation of actual violent conflict (e.g., Rydgren, Sofi, and Hällsten Reference Rydgren, Sofi and Hällsten2013). Based on the preceding, we hypothesize that the quality of intergroup contact explains some of the positive relationships between ethno-religious identification and support for out-group violence (Hypothesis 4). Moreover, we expect that the negative relationship between the quality of contact and support for out-group violence is weaker for those who experienced actual communal violence than those who did not (Hypothesis 5).

Figure 1 presents the conceptual model at a glance. We expect that ethno-religious identification is positively related to support for out-group violence (Hypothesis 1). A higher perception of out-group threat and a lower quality of intergroup contact partly mediate this relationship (Hypotheses 2 and 4). Moreover, the experience of actual communal violence moderates the positive relationship between the perception of threat and support for violence on the one hand and the negative relationship between the quality of contact and support for violence on the other hand (Hypotheses 3 and 5).

Figure 1. Ethno-religious identification and support for out-group violence.

DATA AND METHODS

We tested our predictions using two large, cross-sectional surveys, “Ethno-religious conflicts in Indonesia 2012 (ERCI 2012)” and “Ethno-religious conflicts in the Philippines 2012 (ERCP 2012)” (Sterkens et al. Reference Sterkens, Abanes, Camacho, Kanas, Thijs, Scheepers and Nelson2014a; Reference Sterkens, Kanas, Pamungkas, Subagya, Thijs and Scheepers2014b), which are publicly available to researchers through DANS (https://dans.knaw.nl). The surveys were collected at the end of 2011 and the beginning of 2012 among second and third-year bachelor students in cities of Yogyakarta, Ambon (Maluku), Metro-Manila, and Marawi, Cotabato, and Iligan (in and around BARMM). In each region, we selected three universities: a public (state) university (with a majority of Muslim or Christian students dependent on the country), an Islamic university with a majority of Muslim students, and a Christian university with a majority of Christian students. The choice of public and denominational universities was to ensure a sufficient number of Christian and Muslim students in each region. In each university, a random sample of 250 students from enrollment lists took place, which resulted in a total of 3,000 respondents. Because of a small number of Muslim respondents in Metro-Manila, an additional nonrandom sample of Muslims in the Philippines' capital was added.Footnote 3 In Indonesia, the questionnaires were translated into Bahasa Indonesia by two independent translators, checked by a third one in case of differences in translation. Because English is an official language in the Philippines alongside Tagalog and most university-level courses are taught in English, there was no need to translate the questionnaires for the Philippine respondents. The response rate varied among countries and research regions, and it was the highest in Ambon city (63.1%) and the lowest in Metro-Manila (41.6%).

The unique value of the ERCI and ERCP surveys is that they focus on conflict and non-conflict regions of Indonesia and the Philippines, thus providing insights into the processes that fuel religious extremism. In addition, the surveys contain an extensive list of questions on ethnic and religious identification, perceived intergroup threat, interreligious contact, including the frequency and quality of contact, and political activism, differentiating between legal forms of activism, i.e., protest and demonstration and illegal forms, i.e., violence. In this study, we focus on the illegal forms of activism, i.e., support for property damage and support for harm to persons.

The analysis is restricted to male and female students who reported to be either Muslim or Christian (96.7% of the sample). Participants with missing values 16.2% (19.3% (Indonesia) and 13.3% (the Philippines)) were deleted listwise.Footnote 4 The final sample consists of 1,210 (Indonesia), and 1,362 (the Philippines) participants, 55% (Indonesia), and 22% (the Philippines) of whom are Muslims. Women make up 45 and 65% of our respondents in Indonesia and the Philippines, respectively.

Dependent variable

Support for out-group violence was assessed with five statements about support for damage to property and five statements about support for harm to persons (Kanas and Martinovic Reference Kanas and Martinovic2017a). The wording was the following: “I would support the damaging of property/harm to persons to get more jobs for my religious group,” “to fight abuse of political power against my religious group,” “to enforce the political influence of my religious group,” “to enforce free access to education for my religious group,” and “when my religion is deeply insulted,” measured on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree). We measured this intentional support rather than actual participation in out-group violence, which is typically rare and not openly discussed. So, trying to measure it would be problematic (Moskalenko and McCauley Reference Moskalenko and McCauley2009). Simultaneously, research has shown that radical behavior performed by extremist individuals, such as terrorist attacks, often depends on the moral and material support of a wider group of sympathizers and supporters (Moskalenko and McCauley Reference Moskalenko and McCauley2009). The items were loaded on a common factor for Christians in Indonesia and the Philippines and Muslims in the Philippines. For Muslims in Indonesia, the items were loaded on two factors, but most of the variance was accounted for by the first factor. We, therefore, combined all the items in highly reliable scales across samples: Muslims in Indonesia, α = 0.89; Christians in Indonesia, α = 0.94; Muslims in the Philippines α = 0.91; Christians in the Philippines, α = 0.95, with a higher score indicating stronger support for violence on behalf of the religious in-group. Approximately 14% of all respondents in total do not reject and even approves of out-group violence.

Independent Variables

Ethno-religious identification was measured with 10 statements capturing the importance of one's religious (five items) and ethnic identity (five items) (Ashmore, Deaux, and McLaughlin-Volpe Reference Ashmore, Deaux and McLaughlin-Volpe2004; Roccas, Klar, and Liviatan Reference Roccas, Klar and Liviatan2006; Verkuyten Reference Verkuyten2007). The wording was the following: “My religious (ethnic) identity is very important to me,” “I see myself as a committed member of my religious (ethnic) group,” “My religious beliefs (ethnic identity) have a great deal of influence in my daily life,” “My religious beliefs (ethnic identity) have a great deal of influence on how I make important decisions,” and “My religious beliefs (ethnic identity) have a great deal of influence on how I relate with others,” measured on a five-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree). A factor analysis (principal-component factor) was performed, with the number of factors fixed at one. All items were loaded on a common factor (loadings ranging from 0.5 to 0.72) and formed reliable scales across samples: Muslims in Indonesia, α = 0.81; Christians in Indonesia, α = 0.82; Muslims in the Philippines α = 0.87; Christians in the Philippines, α = 0.87, with a higher score indicating higher ethno-religious identification.Footnote 5 The perceived intergroup threat was assessed with nine statements such as “I am worried that job prospects for members of my group would decline due to the presence of other religious groups,” “I am worried that study grant opportunities will decline due to the presence of other religious groups,” and “I am afraid that the customs of my group will be lost due to the presence of other religious groups,” measured on a five-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree) (see Appendix for the complete list of items). Similar items have been successfully used in other studies to measure perceived intergroup threat posed by ethnic and racial groups (Scheepers, Gijsberts, and Coenders Reference Scheepers, Gijsberts and Coenders2002; Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Boniecki, Ybarra, Bettencourt, Ervin, Jackson, McNatt and Renfro2002; Gijsberts, Hagendoorn, and Scheepers Reference Gijsberts, Hagendoorn and Scheepers2004; Schneider Reference Schneider2008). Although previous studies distinguish between realistic threat and symbolic threat (e.g., Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Boniecki, Ybarra, Bettencourt, Ervin, Jackson, McNatt and Renfro2002; Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Ybarra and Morrison2009), the results from the factor analysis (principal-component factor) indicated that perceived out-group threat is one-dimensional in our sample. We combined the items to form reliable scales across samples: Muslims in Indonesia, α = 0.92; Christians in Indonesia, α = 0.92; Muslims in the Philippines α = 0.88; Christians in the Philippines, α = 0.92. A higher score indicates a stronger perception of intergroup threat.

Quality of intergroup contact was measured by 16 statements referring to four aspects of the quality of intergroup contact, namely goodness, closeness, equality, and cooperativeness (Allport Reference Allport1954; Pettigrew and Tropp Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006). For example, the four statements for contact with friends were: “How would you rate your contact with [religious group] as friends?”; “How close are you with your friends from other religious groups?”; “How equal would you say you are with your friends from other religious groups?”; and “How much do you cooperate with your friends from other religious groups?,” measured on a five-point scale. The same questions were repeated for contact with religious out-group neighbors, classmates, and housemates (see Appendix). We combined these items to form reliable scales across samples: Muslims in Indonesia, α = 0.96; Christians in Indonesia, α = 0.93; and Muslims in the Philippines, α = 0.90; Christians in the Philippines, α = 0.90), with a higher score indicating better interreligious contact.

The experience of out-group violence is measured by nine dichotomous statements asking whether in the past 10 years the respondents or their family, friends, or neighbors suffered any type of physical injury or death due to interreligious violence. Based on this information, we constructed a dichotomous variable taking the value one if the respondent gave a positive response to (at least) one item.

Majority status is measured by a dichotomous variable equal to one if a respondent belongs to a religious majority group at the national level, i.e., Muslims in Indonesia and Christians in the Philippines, and zero if a respondent belongs to a religious minority at the national level, i.e., Christians in Indonesia and Muslims in the Philippines.

Control Variables

We also control for the influence of three background factors that are established predictors of support for ethno-religious extremism in the examined regions (Kanas and Martinovic Reference Kanas and Martinovic2017a): gender (1 = male) and socio-economic background as indicated by parental education (average of mother's and father's education, measured on a 4-point scale, ranging from 1 (no education) to 4 (tertiary education)) and household income (measured on an 8-point scale, ranging from 1 (less than Rp 500,000/PHP 3,000) to 8 (Rp 6,000,000/PHP 45,000)). Since the focus is on the second- and third-year bachelor students, and there is very little variation in age (Mean = 21.45, S.D. = 2.68 (Indonesia), Mean = 19.72, S.D. = 1.93 (the Philippines)), we do not include age in the analyses.Footnote 6

Methods

We estimated ordinary least squares (OLS) models to address our hypotheses and research questions. We checked for high multicollinearity among the independent variables, but the correlations did not exceed critical levels. However, because living in a conflict region versus a non-conflict region is correlated with two main predictor variables (i.e., perceived out-group threat and the experience of actual interreligious violence), it was not included in the analyses.

RESULTS

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the independent and dependent variables. Tables 2 and 3 present the results from the OLS regression of support for violence. Model 1 includes ethno-religious identification and control variables; Model 2 adds the perception of intergroup threat and the quality of contact; and Model 3 includes additional interactions between perceived intergroup threat and the quality of contact on the one hand and experience of actual violence, on the other. To see whether the same patterns emerge in both countries and among religious majorities and minorities, the models are estimated separately for Indonesia and the Philippines and religious majorities and minorities (cf. Tables 2, 3). Finally, we followed these regression analyses up with bootstrap tests of mediation (MacKinnon, Krull, and Lockwood Reference MacKinnon, Krull and Lockwood2000; Preacher and Hayes Reference Preacher and Hayes2008; Hayes Reference Hayes2018) to examine whether the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence is mediated by out-group threat/quality of contact.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Source: ERCI 2012, ERCP 2012.

Table 2. OLS regression analyses of support for out-group violence, separate per country

t-statistics in parentheses; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, two-tailed test.

Source: ERCI 2012, ERCP 2012.

Table 3. OLS Regression analyses of support for out-group Violence, separate for religious majorities and minorities

t-statistics in parentheses; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, two-tailed test.

Source: ERCI 2012, ERCP 2012.

Our first hypothesis stated that ethno-religious identification is positively related to support for out-group violence. Table 2, Model 1, clearly supports this hypothesis. In both Indonesia and the Philippines, we find a positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for intergroup violence. That is, keeping other variables constant, a change from low to high ethno-religious identification is associated with an increase in ethno-religious identification by 0.35 (4 × 0.088) in Indonesia and 0.31 (4 × 0.078) points in the Philippines. Our second hypothesis stated that perceived intergroup threat (partly) mediates the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for out-group violence (Hypothesis 2). We find support for this hypothesis in both Indonesia and the Philippines. First, Table 2, Model 2 clearly shows that in both countries, the perception of intergroup threat is positively related to support for out-group violence. Second, the coefficient for ethno-religious identification on support for out-group violence is significantly reduced in magnitude when the perception of threat is included in the model: b = 0.088, p = 0.036 (Indonesia) and b = 0.078, p = 0.032 (the Philippines) in Model 1 versus b = 0.006, p = 0.872 (Indonesia) and b = 0.036, p = 0.293 (the Philippines) in Model 2. Additional analyses presented in the Appendix (Table A1) show that ethno-religious identification is positively significantly related to the perception of out-group threat: b = 0.255, p < 0.001 (Indonesia) and b = 0.140, p < 0.001(the Philippines). We followed these regression analyses up with bootstrap tests of mediation (Preacher and Hayes Reference Preacher and Hayes2008; Hayes Reference Hayes2018) and confirmed that perceived out-group threat mediates the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence in Indonesia: indirect effect = 0.090, S.E. = 0.017, 95% confidence interval: 0.057 and 0.123; and in the Philippines: indirect effect = 0.05, S.E. = 0.016, 95% confidence interval: 0.017 and 0.082.

Our third hypothesis stated that the relationship between perceived out-group threat and support for out-group violence is significantly stronger for those who experienced actual communal violence than those who did not. The results presented in Table 2, Model 3, provide no support for this hypothesis. Specifically, while we find that experience of violence induces support for violence (cf. Model 1, Table 2), there is no evidence that it strengthens the relationship between the perception of threat and support for violence. Importantly, these findings are the same in both countries. Interestingly, the perceived intergroup threat remains a strong predictor of support for violence, even when the actual experience of violence is taken into account. In sum, our results suggest that the perception of threat provides a valid mechanism explaining the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence. However, there is no evidence that the positive relationship between perceived intergroup threat and support for violence is stronger among those who experienced actual violence.

In our fourth hypothesis, as a corollary to Hypothesis 2, we expected that the quality of intergroup contact explains the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence. Our results presented in Table 2, Model 2 clearly reject this hypothesis as the relationship between the quality of contact and support for out-group violence, while negative, does not reach statistical significance, and this holds in both countries. The results from additional analyses, not presented in Table 2, show that when perceived intergroup threat is not included in the model, the quality of contact is negatively and significantly related to support for violence in both Indonesia and the Philippines (b = −0.116, p < 0.001 and b = −0.161, p < 0.001, respectively). These results suggest that the negative relationship between the quality of contact and support for out-group violence is influenced by the inclusion of perceived group threat in the model. It could be that people with higher quality intergroup contacts are less likely to perceive the intergroup threat and, because of that, less likely to support interreligious violence (Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Boniecki, Ybarra, Bettencourt, Ervin, Jackson, McNatt and Renfro2002; McLaren Reference McLaren2003). Alternatively, one could argue that a high perception of group threat prevents people from developing meaningful ties with out-group members inducing support for intergroup violence (McLaren Reference McLaren2003). With cross-sectional data, examining which of the mechanisms proposed is stronger is beyond this paper's scope.

The fifth hypothesis stated that a possible negative relationship between the quality of contact and support for out-group violence would be weaker among those who experienced actual communal violence. Our results provide merely weak evidence for this hypothesis as indicated by the positive and marginally significant interaction effect between the quality of contact and experience of actual violence in the Indonesian sample. Specifically, Table 2, Model 3 shows that while the quality of intergroup contact is negatively related to support for interreligious violence, the positive and marginally significant interaction effect between the quality of contact and actual violence suggests that this is largely among those without experience of violence. There is no evidence that the quality of interreligious contact is directly related to the support for interreligious violence in the Filipino sample, and this is regardless of the experience of actual violence.

In all estimated models and in line with previous research (Subagya Reference Subagya2015; Kanas and Martinovic Reference Kanas and Martinovic2017a), males and students coming from lower socio-economic backgrounds, as indicated by parental education and household income, report higher support for out-group violence than females and those who come from a higher socio-economic background. There is also some evidence that Filipino Muslims show more support for intergroup violence than Filipino Christians (cf. Table 2). This result is not surprising, given that most Muslims in the Filipino sample live or come from the country's most impoverished region of Mindanao, with a recent history of communal violence. Despite decennia-long peace negotiations and development programs, Muslims in the region still live in a context of social instability and poor living standards (Hutchcroft Reference Hutchcroft2015). Living conditions hit rock bottom shortly after our data collection during the 5-month-long Battle of Marawi from May 2017 onwards, resulting in more than a thousand deadly victims and many more displaced (Australian Strategic Policy Institute 2019; International Crisis Group 2019).

DIFFERENCES BETWEEN RELIGIOUS MAJORITIES AND MINORITIES

In additional analyses, we examined the robustness of our findings among religious majorities and minorities and test whether majority groups perceive minorities as a more significant threat than the other way around, as Blumer (Reference Blumer1958) proposed, resulting in more support for out-group violence among majority groups. Table 3 presents the OLS regression results of support for violence separately for religious majorities and minorities. Because the findings were very similar across two countries (cf. Table 2) and running separate analyses per country would result in a relatively small sample size, particularly for Muslims in the Philippines, we present the results for two surveys combined. Interestingly, Table 3, Model 1 shows that ethno-religious identification is positively related to support for violence among religious majorities (b = 0.107, p < 0.001) but not among minorities (b = −0.005, p = 0.907).Footnote 7

Table 3, Model 2, and Table A2 show that perceived out-group threat mediates the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence among religious majorities (indirect effect = 0.087, S.E. = 0.014, 95% confidence interval: 0.059 and 0.115. However, similar to our previous findings presented in Table 2, there is no evidence that intergroup contact quality mediates the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for interreligious violence, and this is for both religious majorities and minorities. Additional results reveal that intergroup contact's quality is negatively related to support for out-group violence among both religious majorities and minorities. However, this negative relationship becomes insignificant when the perception of out-group threat is taken into account.

Interestingly, Table 3, Model 3 provides some evidence that the negative relationship between the quality of contact and support for violence is significantly weaker for majority group members who experienced actual violence than those who did not experience violence.

DISCUSSION

This study examines the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for out-group violence. Over and beyond focusing on a direct relationship between ethno-religious identification and out-group hostility, we examine two complementary mediating mechanisms—the perception of out-group threat and the quality of intergroup contact—as well as the role of actual out-group violence as a moderator of these relationships. The study relies on unique survey data collected in ethnically and religiously diverse regions of Indonesia and the Philippines, where social cleavages occur along ethnic and religious lines and relations between the two largest religious communities—Christians and Muslims—are tense and have been violent. Finally, our focus on support for out-group violence provides an important contribution to previous research, predominantly focused on less radical indicators of out-group derogation, namely, negative attitudes towards the out-group, providing fewer insights into intentional behavior.

In line with our expectations, we find evidence for a positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence. These conclusions are in line with meta-analytic overviews by Jackson and Hunsberger (Reference Jackson and Hunsberger1999) and Hall, Matz, and Wood (Reference Hall, Matz and Wood2010), who reported a consistent positive relationship between religious identification and prejudice towards ethnic minorities in Western countries. In line with these findings, our results show a positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for interreligious violence in Indonesia and the Philippines and among religious majorities, providing reasons to propose that these results are valid across national contexts and groups with a dominant numerical size and political power.

As hypothesized, perceived out-group threat provides an important mechanism explaining the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence. In fact, when the perception of intergroup threat is included in the analyses, the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence is reduced to non-significance. This implies that ethno-religious identification is positively related to the perception of intergroup threat, which is positively associated with the support for violence (Riek, Mania, and Gaertner Reference Riek, Mania and Gaertner2006). However, because we rely on cross-sectional data, we cannot make strong causal inferences about these relationships. Under a reversed causality scenario, it could be that the perception of out-group threat increases identification with an ethnic and religious in-group, which in turn is positively related to support for violence (Moskalenko, McCauley, and Rozin Reference Moskalenko, McCauley and Rozin2006).

Interestingly, while the experience of communal conflict generally increases support for violence, it does not strengthen the positive relationship between perceived threat and support for violence. This implies that the perception of intergroup threat and violence experience have independent effects on support for violence. Thus, when the ethno-religious out-group is perceived as threatening, support for out-group violence is equally strong among those with and without violence experience.

In sum, these findings suggest that experience of violence is not a necessary condition to induce support for violence and that the perception of out-group threat can be equally powerful in predicting support for intergroup conflict. This finding is in line with previous research from Western countries, which showed that the perception of intergroup threat explains the relationship between actual intergroup threat, as indicated by the size of the out-group population in the region and out-group prejudice (Schlueter and Wagner Reference Schlueter and Wagner2008).

We find, however, no support for the hypothesis that the quality of interreligious contact mediates the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence. While we find that the quality of intergroup contact is negatively related to support for out-group violence, in line with existing studies on intergroup prejudice, this negative relationship becomes insignificant––and is thus explained away––when the perception of intergroup threat is taken into account (Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Boniecki, Ybarra, Bettencourt, Ervin, Jackson, McNatt and Renfro2002; McLaren Reference McLaren2003; Kanas, Scheepers, and Sterkens Reference Kanas, Scheepers and Sterkens2015). Moreover, the negative relationship between the quality of intergroup contact and support for violence seems to be particularly true for those without experience of communal violence. For those who experienced communal violence, intergroup contact quality does not influence support for violence.

A limitation of this study is that the analyses have been based on cross-sectional data. This means that all results observed in this study are correlational. While the causal order from the experience of actual intergroup violence (which occurred in the past) and support for interreligious violence is rather self-evident and previous research supports the causal order guiding the present analyses, other causal relations assumed in this research are less clear but probable. For example, consistent with our assumption, there has been strong empirical evidence from experimental (Stephan et al. Reference Stephan, Boniecki, Ybarra, Bettencourt, Ervin, Jackson, McNatt and Renfro2002; Riek, Mania, and Gaertner Reference Riek, Mania and Gaertner2006) and longitudinal analyses (Schlueter and Wagner Reference Schlueter and Wagner2008) that intergroup contact reduces and perceived intergroup threat increases out-group prejudice (Pettigrew and Tropp Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006; Pettigrew et al. Reference Pettigrew, Tropp, Wagner and Christ2011). Still, further work on the issue of causal relationships between group identification, on the one hand, and perceived intergroup threat and intergroup contact, on the other hand, is undoubtedly needed.

Another issue is the limited generalizability of our samples, which focuses on a student population in four regions of Indonesia and the Philippines. The choice of the student population was motivated by previous insights recognizing Indonesian and Filipino students as important politicized middle grounds between militias and groups of perpetrators on the one hand and ethnoreligious groups on the other hand (Magdalena Reference Magdalena1977; Montiel and Macapagal Reference Montiel and Macapagal2006). Another reason was that students more than other segments of Indonesian and Filipino society, have sufficient knowledge and experience to report on theoretical concepts such as ethnic and religious identifications and the quality of out-group contact examined in this study.

CONCLUSIONS

This study provides important insights into mechanisms and conditions under which ethno-religious identification is related to support for out-group violence. Strong ethno-religious identification is positively and significantly related to out-group violence. This is because identification with the ethnic and religious in-group increases perceptions of out-group threat, which induces support for violence. The experience of actual violence, while an important determinant of support for violence on its own, is not a necessary condition to induce stronger support for out-group violence. While the quality of intergroup contact does not explain the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence, there is some evidence that it reduces support for violence, although indirectly, via the decreased perception of out-group threat. While these findings are consistent across the two national contexts, they mainly hold among religious majorities which dominate in numerical size and have the most political power.

Financial support

Funding for work on this article came from the Dutch National Science Foundation under programme of Conflict and Security.

Data

All data/additional analyses requests should be sent to Agnieszka Kanas, e-mail:

APPENDIX

MEASUREMENT OF SELECTED VARIABLES USED IN THE ANALYSES

Quality of contact

Goodness

  • “How would you rate your contact with [religious group] as neighbors?”

  • “How would you rate your contact with [religious group] as classmates?”

  • “How would you rate your contact with [religious group] as housemates?”

  • “How would you rate your contact with [religious group] as friends?”

The items are measured on five-point ordinal scales ranging from 1 (very negative) to 5 (very positive).

Closure

  • “How close are you with your neighbors from other religious groups?”

  • “How close are you with your classmates from other religious groups?”

  • “How close are you with your housemates from other religious groups?”

  • “How close are you with your friends from other religious groups?”

The items are measured on six-point ordinal scales ranging from 1 (not close at all) to 6 (very close).

Equality

  • “How equal would you say you are with your neighbors from other religious groups?”

  • “How equal would you say you are with your classmates from other religious groups?”

  • “How equal would you say you are with your housemates from other religious groups?”

  • “How equal would you say you are with your friends from other religious groups?”

The items are measured on six-point ordinal scales ranging from 1 (not equal at all) to 6 (very equal).

Cooperativeness

  • “How much do you cooperate with your neighbors from other religious groups?”

  • “How much do you cooperate with your classmates from other religious groups?”

  • “How much do you cooperate with your housemates from other religious groups?”

  • “How much do you cooperate with your friends from other religious groups?”

The items are measured on six-point ordinal scales ranging from 1 (do not cooperate at all) to 6 (cooperate much).

Perceived group threat

  • “I am worried that job prospects for members of my group would decline due to the presence of other religious groups.”

  • “I am worried that study grant opportunities will decline due to the presence of other religious groups.”

  • “I am worried that security in my university will decline due to the presence of students of other religious groups.”

  • “I am worried that the security in my neighborhood will decline due to the presence of other religious groups.”

  • “I am afraid of increasing violence in my neighborhood due to the presence of other religious groups.”

  • “The chances of getting space in a boarding house will decline due to the presence of other religious groups.”

  • “The migration of people of different religious groups to my community is a threat to my own religious group.”

  • “The religious practices of people from other religious groups threaten our own way of life.”

  • “I am afraid that customs of my group will be lost due to the presence of other religious groups.”

The items are measured on five-point scales ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree).

National identification

I am worried that job prospects for members of my group would decline due to the presence of other religious groups.

  • “I would rather be a citizen of Indonesia than of any other country in the world.”

  • “My most important characteristics come from my nationality.”

  • “I should respect my nation and its tradition.”

  • “I should always put national interest above ethno-religious group interest.”

  • “Renewing national ideas is our national task.”

The items are measured on five-point ordinal scales ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree).

Table A1. OLS regression analyses of perceived group threat, separate per country

Table A2. OLS Regression analyses of perceived group threat, separate for religious majorities and minorities

Footnotes

1. The exact numbers in the latest national cenuses are: Christians 60% and Muslims 40% (Ambon city); Christians 9% and Muslims 91% (greater Yogyakarta) (Statistics Indonesia 2010); Christians 8% and Muslims 90% (BARMM); and Christians 95% and Muslims 0.6% (Metro-Manila) (NSO 2010).

2. For a detailed account of a situation in Ambon and Mindanao see the reports of the International Crisis Group (ICG 2002; 2012a; 2012b; 2019; 2020a; 2020b).

3. In total, 59 additional respondents were added in Metro-Manila sample, of which 29 came from the pilot survey and 30 were contacted via religious organizations and mosques.

4. The missing values are mainly due to missing information on the quality of contact (9%), household income (2%), and parental education (1%).

5. Besides ethnic and religious identification, national identification is another factor contributing to support for political action (Hayes and McAllister Reference Hayes and McAllister2001; Kanas and Martinovic Reference Kanas and Martinovic2017a). National identification may be particularly important for aggravated minorities, for whom identifying with both ethnic and religious in-group and national society can contribute to the feeling of being part of the larger national society and legitimize political action on behalf of the in-group (e.g., Klandermans, Van der Toorn, and Van Stekelenburg, Reference Klandermans, Van der Toorn and Van Stekelenburg2008). However, in our context, to the extent that ethnic and religious majorities experience shared grievances, dual identification may promote political action among majority group members as well. We re-estimated our models with national identification as a control variable for the Indonesian sample which includes measures of national identification (see the Appendix). While there is some evidence that national identification is negatively related to support for out-group violence (b = −0.063, p = 0.088), the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence remains negative and statistically significant. These findings suggest that at least in Indonesia, the positive relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence cannot be explained by higher levels of national identification among aggravated communities.

6. To see whether the estimated relationship are influenced by the factor of youth, we re-estimated analyses with age of respondents as a control variable. Additional results show that while age has a negative effect on support for out-group violence in Indonesia, this effect becomes insignificant after the perception of threat and the quality of contact are included in the analyses (cf. Model 2). In addition, age has no significant effect on support for violence in the Philippines and among religious majorities and minorities. More importantly, controlling for age does not affect our conclusions regarding the role of ethno-religious identification in support for violence.

7. We also examined whether the relationship between ethno-religious identification and support for violence is significantly stronger among religious majorities than minorities (cf. Model 1, Table 3), by estimating the interaction between ethno-religious identification and majority status in the combined model (cf., Table 2). The results show that the interaction is positive and marginally significant (p < 0.09) in the Filipino sample.

t-statistics in parentheses; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, two-tailed test.

Source: ERCI 2012, ERCP 2012.

t-statistics in parentheses; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, two-tailed test.

Source: ERCI 2012, ERCP 2012.

References

Allport, G. W. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Ashmore, R.D., Deaux, K., and McLaughlin-Volpe, T.. 2004. “An Organizing Framework for Collective Identity: Articulation and Significance of Multidimensionality.” Psychological Bulletin 130:80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Strategic Policy Institute. 2019. The Marawi Crisis. Urban Conflict and Information Operations. Barton: ASPI.Google Scholar
Bauman, C., and Leech, T.. 2012. “Political Competition, Relative Deprivation, and Perceived Threat: A Research Note on Anti-Christian Violence in India.” Ethnic and Racial Studies 35:21952216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumer, H. 1958. “Race Prejudice as A Sense of Group Position.” Pacific Sociological Review 1:37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. 1999. “The Psychology of Prejudice: In-Group Love or out-Group Hate?Journal of Social Issues 55:429444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cairns, E., Kenworthy, J., Campbell, A., and Hewstone, M.. 2006. “The Role of in-Group Identification, Religious Group Membership and Intergroup Conflict in Moderating in-Group and out-Group Affect.” British Journal of Social Psychology 45:701716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gijsberts, M., Hagendoorn, L., and Scheepers, P.. 2004. Nationalism and Exclusion of Migrants: Cross National Comparisons. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Hall, D. L., Matz, D. C., and Wood, W. 2010. “Why Don't We Practice What we Preach? A Meta-Analytic Review of Religious Racism.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 14:126139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F. 2018. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis. A Regression-Based Approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. C., and McAllister, I.. 2001. “Sowing Dragon's Teeth: Public Support for Political Violence and Paramilitarism in Northern Ireland.” Political Studies 49:901922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinkle, S., and Brown, R.. 1990. “Intergroup Comparisons and Social Identity: Some Links and Lacunae.” Social Identity Theory: Constructive and Critical Advances 48:70.Google Scholar
Hutchcroft, P. D. 2015. Mindanao. The Long Journey to Peace and Prosperity. Singapore: World Scientific Publishing.Google Scholar
International Crisis Group Asia Briefing 133. 2012a. Indonesia: Cautious Calm in Ambon. Jakarta/Brussels: International Crisis Group.Google Scholar
International Crisis Group Asia Report 250. 2012b. Philippines: Breakthrough in Mindanao. Jakarta/Brussels: International Crisis Group.Google Scholar
International Crisis Group Asia Report 301. 2019. The Philippines: Militancy and the New Bangsamoro. Jakarta/Brussels: International Crisis Group.Google Scholar
International Crisis Group Asia Report 302. 2020a. The Philippines: Tackling Clan Politics in the Bangsamoro. Jakarta/Brussels: International Crisis Group.Google Scholar
International Crisis Group Asia Report 306. 2020b. Southern Philippines: Tackling Clan Politics in the Bangsamoro. Jakarta/Brussels: International Crisis Group.Google Scholar
International Crisis Group Asia Report 31. 2002. Indonesia: The Search for Peace in Maluku. Executive Summary and Recommendations. Jakarta/Brussels: International Crisis Group.Google Scholar
Jackson, L. M., and Hunsberger, B. 1999. “An Intergroup Perspective on Religion and Prejudice.” Journal for the scientific study of religion 38:509523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A., and Martinovic, B.. 2017a. “Political Action in Conflict and Non-Conflict Regions in Indonesia: The Role of Religious and National Identifications.” Political Psychology 38:209225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A., Scheepers, P., and Sterkens, C.. 2015. “Interreligious Contact, Perceived Group Threat, and Perceived Discrimination: Predicting Negative Attitudes Among Religious Minorities and Majorities in Indonesia.” Social Psychology Quarterly 78:102126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A., Scheepers, P., and Sterkens, C.. 2016. “Religious Identification and Interreligious Contact in Indonesia and the Philippines: Testing the Mediating Roles of Perceived Group Threat and Social Dominance Orientation and the Moderating Role of Context.” European Journal of Social Psychology 46:700715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanas, A., Scheepers, P., and Sterkens, C.. 2017b. “Positive and Negative Contact and Attitudes Towards the Religious out-Group: Testing the Contact Hypothesis in Conflict and non-Conflict Regions of Indonesia and the Philippines.” Social Science Research 63:95110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klandermans, B., Van der Toorn, J., and Van Stekelenburg, J.. 2008. “Embeddedness and Identity: How Immigrants Turn Grievances Into Action.” American Sociological Review 73:9921012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Licata, L., Klein, O., Saade, W., Azzi, A. E., and Branscombe, N. R.. 2012. “Perceived out-Group (dis)Continuity and Attribution of Responsibility for the Lebanese Civil War Mediate Effects of National and Religious Subgroup Identification on Intergroup Attitudes.” Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 15:179192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., and Lockwood, C. M. 2000. “Equivalence of the Mediation, Confounding and Suppression Effect.” Prevention Science 1:173181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magdalena, F. V. 1977. “Intergroup Conflict in the Southern Philippines: An Empirical Analysis.” Journal of Peace Research 14:299313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maoz, I., and McCauley, C.. 2009. “Threat Perceptions and Feelings as Predictors of Jewish-Israeli Support for Compromise with Palestinians.” Journal of Peace Research 46:525539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaren, L. M. 2003. “Anti-Immigrant Prejudice in Europe: Contact, Threat Perception, and Preferences for the Exclusion of Migrants.” Social Forces 81:909936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montiel, C. J., and Macapagal, M. E. J. 2006. “Effects of Social Position on Societal Attributions of an Asymmetric Conflict.” Journal of Peace Research 43:219227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskalenko, S., and McCauley, C.. 2009. “Measuring Political Mobilization: The Distinction Between Activism and Radicalism.” Terrorism and Political Violence 21:239260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskalenko, S., McCauley, C., and Rozin, P.. 2006. “Group Identification Under Conditions of Threat: College Students’ Attachment to Country, Family, Ethnicity, Religion, and University Before and After September 11.” Political Psychology 27:7797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mujiburrahman. 2006. Feeling Threatened: Muslim-Christian Relations in Indonesia's New Order. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
NSO National Statistics Office, Republic of the Philippines. 2010. Retrieved in January 2012, from: www.census.gov.ph.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., and Tropp, L. R.. 2006. “A Meta-Analytic Test of Intergroup Contact Theory.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90:751783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F., Tropp, L. R., Wagner, U., and Christ, O.. 2011. “Recent Advances in Intergroup Contact Theory.” International Journal of Intercultural Relations 35:271280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center. 2019. A Closer Look at How Religious Restrictions Have Risen Around the World. Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F.. 2008. “Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediator Models.” Behavior Research Methods 40:879891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putra, I. E., and Wagner, W.. 2017. “Prejudice in Interreligious Context: The Role of Metaprejudice and Majority–Minority Status.” Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology 27:226239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quillian, L. 1995. “Prejudice as A Response to Perceived Group Threat: Population Composition and Anti-Immigrant and Racial Prejudice in Europe.” American Sociological Review 60:586611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., and Gaertner, S. L.. 2006. “Intergroup Threat and Outgroup Attitudes: A Meta-Analytic Review.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 10:336353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roccas, S., Klar, Y., and Liviatan, I.. 2006. “The Paradox of Group-Based Guilt: Modes of National Identification, Conflict Vehemence, and Reactions to the in-Group's Moral Violations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91:698711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roccas, S., Sagiv, L, Schwartz, S., Halevy, N., and Eidelson, R.. 2008. “Toward A Unifying Model of Identification with Groups: Integrating Theoretical Perspectives.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 12:280306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouhana, N. N., and Bar-Tal, D.. 1998. “Psychological Dynamics of Intractable Ethnonational Conflicts. The Israeli- Palestinian Case.” American Psychologist 53:761770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowatt, W. C., Ottenbreit, A., Nesselroade, K. P., and Cunningham, P. A.. 2002. “On Being Holier-than-Thou or Humbler-than-Thee: A Social-Psychological Perspective on Religiousness and Humility.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 41:227237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rydgren, J., Sofi, D., and Hällsten, M.. 2013. “Interethnic Friendship, Trust, and Tolerance: Findings From Two North Iraqi Cities.” American Journal of Sociology 118:16501694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, P., and Eisinga, R. M. 2015. “Religiosity and Prejudice Against Minorities.” In International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences, ed. J. Wright, 2nd edition. London, United Kingdom: Elsevier Health Sciences, 381383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheepers, P., Gijsberts, M., and Coenders, M.. 2002. “Ethnic Exclusionism in European Countries: Public Opposition to Civil Rights for Legal Migrants as a Response to Perceived Ethnic Threat.” European Sociological Review 18:1734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlueter, E., and Wagner, U. 2008. “Regional Differences Matter Examining the Dual Influence of the Regional Size of the Immigrant Population on Derogation of Immigrants in Europe.” International Journal of Comparative Sociology 49:153173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, K., Tausch, N., Hewstone, M., Hughes, J., and Cairns, E.. 2008. “The Effects of Living in Segregated vs. Mixed Areas in Northern Ireland: A Simultaneous Analysis of Contact and Threat Effects in the Context of Micro-Level Neighbourhoods.” International Journal of Conflict and Violence 2:5671.Google Scholar
Schneider, S. L. 2008. “Anti-Immigrant Attitudes in Europe: Out-Group Size and Perceived Ethnic Threat.” European Sociological Review 24:5367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulze, K. E. 2017. “The “Ethnic” in Indonesia's Communal Conflicts: Violence in Ambon, Poso, and Sambas.” Ethnic and Racial Studies 40:20962114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Setiawan, T., Scheepers, P. L. H., de Jong, E. B. P., and Sterkens, C. J. A.. 2021. “Support for Interreligious Conflict in Indonesia: Tests of Theories on Interethnic Threat and Distrust Versus Contact.” Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology 15:114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spears, R. 2011. “Group Identities: The Social Identity Perspective.” In Handbook of Identity Theory and Research, eds. Schwartz, S. J., Luyckx, K. and Vignoles, V. L.. New York: Springer, 201224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Statistics Indonesia. 2010. Retrieved in October 2015, from: http://sp2010.bps.go.id.Google Scholar
Stephan, W. G., Boniecki, K. A., Ybarra, O., Bettencourt, A., Ervin, K. S., Jackson, L. A., McNatt, P. S., and Renfro, C. L.. 2002. “The Role of Threats in the Racial Attitudes of Blacks and Whites.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28:12421254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W.G., Ybarra, O., and Morrison, K. R.. 2009. “Intergroup Threat Theory.” In Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 4455.Google Scholar
Sterkens, C., Abanes, M., Camacho, A., Kanas, A., Thijs, P. E., and Scheepers, P.. 2014a. Ethno-religious Conflicts in the Philippines 2012 (ERCP 2012). Documentation of Surveys on Ethno-Religious Identity and Latent Intergroup Conflict (DANS Data Guide 13), eds. Nelson, T. D.. Amsterdam: Pallas Publications—Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Sterkens, C., Kanas, A., Pamungkas, C., Subagya, T., Thijs, P. E., and Scheepers, P.. 2014b. Ethno-religious Conflicts in Indonesia 2012 (ERCI 2012). Documentation of Surveys on Ethno-Religious Identity and Latent Intergroup Conflict (DANS Data Guide 12). Amsterdam: Pallas Publications—Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Subagya, T. 2015. Support for Ethno-Religious Violence in Indonesia. Yogyakarta: USD press.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H. 1982. “Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations.” Annual Review of Psychology 33:139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C.. 1979. “An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Conflict,” In The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, ed. Austin, W. G. and Worchel, S., Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole, 3347.Google Scholar
Tausch, N., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J., Cairns, E., and Christ, O.. 2007. “Cross-community Contact, Perceived status Differences and Intergroup Attitudes in Northern Ireland: The Mediating Roles of Individual-Level vs. Group Level Threats and the Moderating Role of Social Identification.” Political Psychology 28:5368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tausch, N., Hewstone, M., and Roy, R.. 2009. “The Relationships Between Contact, Status and Prejudice: An Integrated Threat Theory Analysis of Hindu–Muslim Relations in India.” Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology 19:8394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bruinessen, M. 2018. “Indonesian Muslims in a Globalizing World.” RSIS Working Paper 311 (Issue May): 123.Google Scholar
Verkuyten, M. 2007. “Religious Group Identification and Inter-Religious Relations: A Study among Turkish-Dutch Muslims.” Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 10:341357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verkuyten, M., and Yildiz, A. A.. 2007. “National (dis)Identification, and Ethnic and Religious Identity: A Study among Turkish-Dutch Muslims.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33:14481462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verkuyten, M., and Yildiz, A. A.. 2010. “Religious Identity Consolidation and Mobilization among Turkish Dutch Muslims.” European Journal of Social Psychology 40:436447.Google Scholar
Ysseldyk, R. S., Haslam, A., Matheson, K., and Anisman, H.. 2012. “Love Thine Enemy? Evidence That (ir)Religious Identification can Promote Outgroup Tolerance Under Threat.” Group Processes Intergroup Relations 15:105117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ysseldyk, R., Matheson, K., and Anisman, H.. 2010. “Religiosity as Identity: Toward an Understanding of Religion From a Social Identity Perspective.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 14:6071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zartman, William, and Hopmann, P. Terrence. 2011. Mindanao: Understanding Conflict 2011. Johns Hopkins University: Johns Hopkins University School for Advanced International Studies.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Ethno-religious identification and support for out-group violence.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 2

Table 2. OLS regression analyses of support for out-group violence, separate per country

Figure 3

Table 3. OLS Regression analyses of support for out-group Violence, separate for religious majorities and minorities

Figure 4

Table A1. OLS regression analyses of perceived group threat, separate per country

Figure 5

Table A2. OLS Regression analyses of perceived group threat, separate for religious majorities and minorities