Skip to main content
    • Aa
    • Aa

Responsibility and Decision Making in the Era of Neural Networks*

  • William Bechtel (a1)
  • DOI:
  • Published online: 01 January 2009

Many of the mathematicians and scientists who guided the development of digital computers in the late 1940s, such as Alan Turing and John von Neumann, saw these new devices not just as tools for calculation but as devices that might employ the same principles as are exhibited in rational human thought. Thus, a subfield of what came to be called computer science assumed the label artificial intelligence (AI). The idea of building artificial systems which could exhibit intelligent behavior comparable to that of humans (which could, e.g., recognize objects, solve problems, formulate and implement plans, etc.) was a heady prospect, and the claims made on behalf of AI during the 1950s and 1960s were impossibly ambitious (e.g., having a computer capture the world chess championship within a decade). Despite some theoretical and applied successes within the field, serious problems soon became evident (of which the most notorious is the frame problem, which involves the difficulty in determining which information about the environment must be changed and which must be kept constant in the face of new information). Instead of fulfilling the goal of quickly producing artificial intelligent agents which could compete with or outperform human beings, by the 1970s and 1980s AI had settled into a pattern of slower but real progress in modeling or simulating aspects of human intelligence. (Examples of the advances made during this period were the development of higher-level structures for encoding information, such as frames or scripts, which were superior to simple prepositional encodings in supporting reasoning or the understanding of natural [as opposed to computer or other artificial] language texts, and the development of procedures for storing information about previously encountered cases and invoking these cases in solving new problems.)

Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

Oliver G. Selfridge and Ulric Neisser , “Pattern Recognition by Machine,” Scientific American, vol. 203 (081960), pp. 6068.

William Bechtel and Adele A. Abrahamsen , Connectionism and the Mind: An Introduction to Parallel Processing in Networks (Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1991).

William Bechtel , “Attributing Responsibility to Computer Systems,” Metaphilosophy, vol. 16 (1984), pp. 296306.

Daniel C. Dennett , “Intentional Systems,” Journal of Philosophy, vol. 68 (1971), pp. 87106.

John R. Searle , “Minds, Brains, and Programs,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 3 (1980) pp. 417–24.

John R. Anderson , Cognitive Psychology and Its Applications, 3d ed. (San Francisco: Freeman, 1990).

William Bechtel , “Natural Deduction in Connectionist Systems,” Synthese, vol. 101, no. 3 (1994), pp. 433–63.

William Ramsey , Stephen P. Stich , and J. Garon , “Connectionism, Eliminativism, and the Future of Folk Psychology,” Philosophical Perspectives, vol. 4 (1990), pp. 499533.

Eleanor Rosch and Carolyn B. Mervis , “Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 7 (1975), pp. 573605

Kim Plunkett and Virginia Marchman , “U-shaped Learning and Frequency Effects in a Multilayered Perceptron,” Cognition, vol. 38 (1991), pp. 160.

Geoffrey E. Hinton and Timothy Shallice , “Lesioning an Attractor Network: Investigations of Acquired Dyslexia,” Psychological Review, vol. 98 (1991), pp. 7495.

Mark F. St. John and James L. McClelland , “Learning and Applying Contextual Constraints in Sentence Comprehension,” Artificial Intelligence, vol. 46 (1990), pp. 217–57

S. Nolfi , Jeffrey L. Elman , and D. Parisi , “Learning and Evolution in Neural Networks,” Adaptive Behavior, vol. 3, no. 1 (1994), pp. 528

S. Nolfi , O. Miglino , and D. Parisi , “Phenotypic Plasticity in Evolving Neural Networks,” Proceedings of the First Conference from Perception to Action, ed. D. P. Gaussier and J. D. Nicoud (Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Press, 1994).

William Bechtel , “The Case for Connectionism,” Philosophical Studies, vol. 71 (1993), pp. 119–54.

Jeffrey L. Elman , “Distributed Representations, Simple Recurrent Networks, and Grammatical Structure,” Machine Learning, vol. 7 (1991), pp. 195225.

Donald Davidson , “Rational Animals,” Dialectica, vol. 36 (1982), pp. 318–27.

Richard E. Nisbett and Timothy D. Wilson , “Telling More Than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes,” Psychological Review, vol. 84 (1974), pp. 231–59.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Social Philosophy and Policy
  • ISSN: 0265-0525
  • EISSN: 1471-6437
  • URL: /core/journals/social-philosophy-and-policy
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *