Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-20T14:52:07.830Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive individual differences in the process and product of L2 writing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2023

Gisela Granena*
Affiliation:
Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain

Abstract

This paper examines how research in second language acquisition has approached the study of cognitive individual differences in the process and product of L2 writing from a theoretical and empirical perspective, paying special attention to the three empirical studies included in this special issue. The paper is divided into three sections. The first section examines the cognitive abilities that have been investigated in L2 writing research, among which working memory stands out as the most widely studied. The second section synthesizes the findings reported by the empirical studies in this issue in relation to the role of working memory in L2 writing behaviors and outcomes. The last section suggests future lines of research that can broaden the current scope of research on writing and cognitive individual differences, mostly centered on the components of the working memory system. This research has important theoretical implications, as little is known about how different cognitive individual differences are implicated in writing, as well as pedagogical implications, as the findings can inform about optimal performance and learning conditions for learners with diverse cognitive ability profiles.

Type
State of the Scholarship
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (2015). Working memory in second language learning. In Wen, Z., Mota, M. B., & McNeill, A. (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 1728). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In Diller, K. C. (Ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83118). Newbury House.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. (1959). Modern Language Aptitude Test: Form A. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Cho, M. (2018). Task complexity, modality, and working memory in L2 task performance. System, 72, 8598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, A. R., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: the importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 331335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Saults, J. S., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 42100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on interactions. Irvington.Google Scholar
Dédeyan, A., Olive, T., & Largy, P. (2006, October). Implication des composants de la mémoire de travail dans la détection des erreurs d’accord sujet-verbe: approche développementale [Paper presentation] International Conference on Cognitive Approach of Written Language Learning. Rennes, France.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2012). Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62, 189200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 44, pp. 145199). Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K.A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goo, J. (2010). Working memory and reactivity. Language Learning, 60, 712752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2013a). Cognitive aptitudes for second language learning and the LLAMA Language Aptitude Test. In Granena, G. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment (pp. 105129). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2013b). Individual differences in sequence learning ability and SLA in early childhood and adulthood. Language Learning, 63, 665703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency: Links between LLAMA and Hi-LAB. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 313336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2018). Aptitude-treatment interaction in L2 learning: A research synthesis. Studies in English Education, 23, 803830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019a). Language aptitude profiles and the effectiveness of implicit and explicit corrective feedback. In Leow, R. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language research in classroom learning (pp. 438451). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019b). Cognitive aptitudes for explicit and implicit learning. In Zhisheng, W., Skehan, P., Biedron, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. (Eds.), Language aptitude: Advancing theory, testing, research, and practice (pp. 238256). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayashi, Y. (2019). Investigating effects of working memory training on foreign language development. The Modern Language Journal, 103, 665685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 127). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In Gregg, L. & Steinberg, E. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 330). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R., & Chenoweth, N. A. (2006). Is working memory involved in the transcribing and editing of texts? Written Communication, 23, 135149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (2012). Modeling and remodeling writing. Written Communication, 29, 369388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwu, F., Pan, W., & Sun, S. (2014). Aptitude-treatment interaction effects on explicit rule learning: A latent growth curve analysis. Language Teaching Research, 18, 294319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
In’nami, Y., Hijikata, Y., & Koizumi, R. (2022). Working memory capacity and L2 reading: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44, 381406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y.-F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning: Implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38, 625635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeon, E., & Yamashita, J. (2014). L2 reading comprehension and its correlates: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 64, 160212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (1997). The architecture of working memory. In Rugg, M. D. (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 243276). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Miura, T. K., & Colflesh, G. J. H. (2007). Working memory, attention control, and the n-back task: A question of construct validity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 615622.Google ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In Levy, C. M. & Ransdell, S. (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 5771). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kellogg, R. T. (2001). Long-term working memory in text production. Memory & Cognition, 29, 4352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R. T., Whiteford, A. P., Turner, C. E., Cahil, M., & Mertens, A. (2013). Working memory in written composition: An evaluation of the 1996 model. Journal of Writing Research, 5, 159190.Google Scholar
Kormos, J. (2023). The role of cognitive factors in second language writing and writing to learn a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45, 622646.Google Scholar
Kormos, J., & Sáfár, A. (2008). Phonological short-term memory, working memory and foreign language performance in intensive language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 261271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kormos, J., & Trebits, A. (2012). The role of task complexity, modality and aptitude in narrative task performance. Language Learning, 62, 439472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S. (2015). The associations between language aptitude and second language grammar acquisition: A meta-analytic review of five decades of research. Applied Linguistics, 36, 385408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S. (2016). The construct validity of language aptitude: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 801842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S. (2017). The effects of cognitive aptitudes on the process and product of L2 interaction. In Gurzynski-Weiss, L. (Ed.), Expanding individual difference research in the interaction approach: Investigating learners, instructors, and other interlocutors (pp. 4170). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2019). Six decades of aptitude research: A comprehensive and critical review. In Wen, Z., Skehan, P., Biedron, A., Li, S., & Sparks, R. L. (Eds.), Language aptitude: Advancing theory, testing, research and practice (pp. 7896). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S., & Roshan, S. (2019). The associations between working memory and the effects of four different types of written corrective feedback. Journal of Second Language Writing, 45, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J., Hughes, M., Campbell, S., Silbert, N., Tare, M., Jackson, S., Smith, B. K., Bunting, M. F., & Doughty, C. J. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63, 530566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lu, Y. (2015). Working memory, cognitive resources and L2 writing performance. In Wen, Z., Mota, M. B., & Mcneill, A. (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 175188). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manchón, R. M., & Williams, J. (2016). L2 writing and SLA studies. In Manchón, R. M. & Matsuda, P. K. (Eds.), Handbook of second and foreign language writing (pp. 567586). De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manchón, R. M., McBride, S., Mellado, M. D., & Vasylets, O. (2023). Working memory, L2 proficiency, and task complexity: Independent and interactive effects on L2 written performance. Studies in Second Language Acquisition., 45, 737764.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R., & Yaffee, L. S. (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 83111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Freedman, M. L. (2001). Short-term retention of lexical-semantic representations: Implications for speech production. Memory, 9, 261280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA language aptitude tests. Lognostics.Google Scholar
Michel, M., Kormos, J., Brunfaut, T., & Ratajczak, M. (2019). The role of working memory in young second language learners’ written performances. Journal of Second Language Writing, 45, 3145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (1998). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In Healy, A. & Bourne, L. (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistics studies on training and retention. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monsell, S. (1996). Control of mental processes. In Bruce, V. (Ed.), Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition (pp. 93148). Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Monsell, S., & Driver, J. (2000). Banishing the control homunculus. In Monsell, S. & Driver, J. (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 332). MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, N., & Jones, D. M. (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 111121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 411421.Google ScholarPubMed
Olive, T. (2011). Working memory in writing. In Berninger, V. (Ed.), Past, present, and future contributions of cognitive writing research to cognitive psychology (pp. 485503). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Olive, T., Kellogg, R. T., & Piolat, A. (2002). The triple task technique for studying the process of writing: Why and how? In Olive, T. & Levy, M. C. (Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 3159). Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olive, T., Kellogg, R. T., & Piolat, A. (2008). Verbal, visual, and spatial working memory demands during text composition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 669687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortega, L. (2012). Epilogue: Exploring L2 writing-SLA interfaces. Journal of Second Language Writing, 21, 404415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Norgate, R., & Hadwin, J. A. (2008). Processing efficiency theory in children: Working memory as a mediator between trait anxiety and academic performance. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21, 417430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Hadwin, J. A., & Norgate, R. (2014). When does anxiety help or hinder cognitive test performance? The role of working memory capacity. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 92101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Révész, A., Michel, M., & Lee, M. (2017). Investigating IELTS Academic Writing Task 2: Relationships between cognitive writing processes, text quality, and working memory [IELTS Research Reports Online Series ISSN 2201-2982]. https://www.ielts.org/-/media/research-reports/ielts_online_rr_2017-3.ashxGoogle Scholar
Révész, A., Michel, M., & Lee, M. (2023). Exploring the relationship of working memory to the temporal distribution of pausing and revision behaviors during L2 writing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45, 680709.Google Scholar
Sadoski, M., Kealy, W. A., Goetz, E. T., & Paivio, A. (1997). Concreteness and imagery effects in the written composition of definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 518526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C., Lin, H. J., Lado, B., Bowden, H. W., & Stafford, C. A. (2014). One size fits all? Learning conditions and working memory capacity in ab initio language development. Applied Linguistics, 37, 669692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shin, J. (2020). A meta-analysis of the relationship between working memory and second language reading comprehension: Does task type matter? Applied Psycholinguistics, 41, 873900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skehan, P. (2002). Theorising and updating aptitude. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 6993). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparks, R. L., Artzer, M., Ganschow, L., Siebenhar, D., Plageman, M., & Patton, J. (1998). Differences in native-language skills, foreign-language aptitude, and foreign language grades among high-, average-, and low-proficiency foreign-language learners: Two studies. Language Testing, 15, 181216.Google Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., Ganschow, L., & Humbach, N. (2009). Long-term relationships among early first language skills, second language aptitude, second language affect, and later second language proficiency. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 725755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparks, R. L., Humbach, N., Patton, J. O. N., & Ganschow, L. (2011). Subcomponents of second-language aptitude and second-language proficiency. The Modern Language Journal, 95, 253273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 745759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres, J. (2023). Exploring working memory and language dominance in heritage bilinguals’ writing processes. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45, 710736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasylets, O., Mellado, M. D., & Plonsky, L. (2022). The role of cognitive individual differences in digital versus pen-and-paper writing. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 12, 723745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasylets, O., & Marin, J. (2021). The effects of working memory and L2 proficiency on L2 writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 52, Article 100786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vatz, K., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., & Doughty, C. J. (2013). Aptitude-treatment interaction studies in second language acquisition: Findings and methodology. In Granena, G. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude and ultimate L2 attainment (pp. 273292). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Was, C. A., & Woltz, D. J. (2007). Reexamining the relationship between working memory and comprehension: The role of available long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 86102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Was, C. A., Rawson, K. A., Bailey, H., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Content-embedded tasks beat complex span for predicting comprehension. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 910915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, Z., Biedroń, A., & Skehan, P. (2017). Foreign language aptitude theory: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. Language Teaching, 50, 131.10.1017/S0261444816000276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 258262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woltz, D. J. (2003). Implicit cognitive processes as aptitudes for learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 95104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woltz, D. J., & Was, C. A. (2006). Availability of related long-term memory during and after attention focus in working memory. Memory and Cognition, 34, 668684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woltz, D. J., & Was, C. A. (2007). Available but unattended conceptual information in working memory: Temporarily active semantic knowledge or persistent memory for prior operations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 155168.Google ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Sun, Y., Chang, P., & Li, Y. (2019). Exploring the relationship between language aptitude, vocabulary size, and EFL graduate students’ L2 writing performance. TESOL Quarterly, 53, 845856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zalbidea, J. (2017). “One task fits all”? The roles of task complexity, modality, and working memory capacity in L2 performance. Modern Language Journal, 101, 335352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zalbidea, J., & Sanz, C. (2020). Does learner cognition count on modality? Working memory effects on early L2 morphosyntactic attainment across oral and written tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 41, 11711196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabildea, J., & Issa, B. I. (2023). Exploring the potential of content-embedded working memory tasks for advancing SLA research. [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Latin American Studies, Temple University and Department of Hispanic Studies, University of Tennessee, Knoxville.Google Scholar