Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T08:21:40.055Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

THE INFLUENCE OF ORTHOGRAPHY ON ORAL VOCABULARY ACQUISITION IN LEARNERS OF CHINESE AS A SECOND LANGUAGE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2021

Jie Zhang*
Affiliation:
University of Houston
Hong Li*
Affiliation:
Beijing Normal University
Yang Liu*
Affiliation:
Western Kentucky University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hong Li, Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education (Beijing Normal University), Institute of Children’s Reading and Learning, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Xin Jie Kou Wai Street #19, Beijing100875, PR China. E-mail: psy.lihong@bnu.edu.cn

Abstract

The present study investigated the effects of exposure to Chinese orthography on learning phonological forms of new words in learners of Chinese as a second language. A total of 30 adult learners of Chinese studied spoken label and picture associations presented either with phonologically accurate characters, characters with partial phonological information, or no orthography. Half the phonologically accurate or partially accurate characters were semantically transparent or opaque. Spoken labels were recalled without orthography presence. Results showed that exposure to phonologically accurate and semantically transparent characters during learning did not enhance the recall of the spoken labels compared to no orthography. But exposure to characters with partial phonological information and semantically opaque characters significantly hindered vocabulary learning. The implications for Chinese as a second language vocabulary acquisition and instruction are discussed.

Type
Research Report
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research project was supported by grants from Western Kentucky University Office of Research to JZ. This work was also supported by grants from the Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China (17YJA190009) to HL. We acknowledge all participating teachers, students, and parents. We also thank the undergraduates from Western Kentucky University for their assistance in data collection.

References

Bassetti, B. (2006). Orthographic input and phonological representations in learners of Chinese as a foreign language. Written Language and Literacy, 9, 95114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A., & Keijzer, R. (2000). What is hard to learn is easy to forget: The roles of word concreteness, cognate status, and word frequency in foreign language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Language Learning, 50, 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In Gough, P., Ehri, L., & Treiman, R. (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107143). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L., & Wilce, L. (1979). The mnemonic value of orthography among beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 2640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P. (2015). Orthography plays a limited role when learning the phonological forms of new words: The case of Spanish and English learners of novel Dutch words. Applied Psycholinguist, 36, 722. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641400040X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Simon, E., & Mulak, K. (2014). Learning words in a new language: Orthography doesn’t always help. Bilingualism, 17, 384395. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fu, Y. H. (1989). A basic research on structure and its component of Chinese character. In Chen, Y. (Ed.). Information analysis of used character in modern Chinese language (pp. 154186). Shanghai Educational (in Chinese).Google Scholar
Han, J. I., & Kim, J. Y. (2017). The influence of orthography on the production of alphabetic, second-language allophones by speakers of a non-alphabetic language. Psycholinguist Research, 46, 963982. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9474-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, J. I., & Oh, S. (2018). The role of phonetic similarity and orthographic information in asymmetrical lexical encoding in second language. Journal of Psycholinguist Research, 47, 10151033. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-018-9574-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes-Harb, R., Brown, K., & Smith, B. L. (2018). Orthographic input and the acquisition of German final devoicing by native speakers of English. Language Speech, 61, 547564. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830917710048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes-Harb, R., & Cheng, H. (2016). The influence of the pinyin and zhuyin writing systems on the acquisition of Mandarin word forms by native English speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes-Harb, R., Nicol, J., & Barker, J. (2010). Learning the phonological forms of new words: Effects of orthographic and auditory input. Language and Speech, 53, 367381. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830910371460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, H., Zhang, J., Ehri, L. C., Chen, Y., Ruan, X., & Dong, Q. (2016). The role of orthography in oral vocabulary learning in Chinese children. Reading and Writing, 29, 13631381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, R., & Norbury, C. F. (2014). Orthography facilitates vocabulary learning for children with autism spectrum disorders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 13171334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathieu, L. (2016). The influence of foreign scripts on the acquisition of a second language phonological contrast. Second Language Research, 32, 145170. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658315601882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mengoni, S. E., Nash, H., & Hulme, C. (2013). The benefit of orthographic support for oral vocabulary learning in children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 40, 221243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagy, W.E., Herman, P., & Anderson, R. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., McCormick, S. F., Bayliss, L., & Davis, C. J. (2011). Orthography influences the perception and production of speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 37, 15881594. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024833.Google Scholar
Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Nation, K. (2009). Orthographic facilitation in oral vocabulary acquisition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 19481966. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802696104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, J., & Ehri, L. C. (2008). The mnemonic value of orthography for vocabulary learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 175191. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, E., Chambless, D., & Kickhöfel Alves, U. (2010). Understanding the role of orthography in the acquisition of a non-native vowel contrast. Language Sciences, 32, 380394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Showalter, C. E. (2012). The influence of novel orthographic information on second language word learning: The case of native English speakers learning Arabic. Unpublished master’s thesis, The University of Utah, Provo.Google Scholar
Showalter, C. E., & Hayes-Harb, R. (2013). Unfamiliar orthographic information and second language word learning: A novel lexicon study. Second Language Research, 29, 185200. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658313480154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Showalter, C. E., & Hayes-Harb, R. (2015). Native English speakers learning Arabic: The influence of novel orthographic information on second language phonological acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 2342. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 2747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanborn, M. S. L., & de Glopper, K. (1999). Incidental word learning while reading: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 69, 261285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tong, X., & McBride, C. (2014). Chinese children’s statistical learning of orthographic regularities: Positional constraints and character structure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 291308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, J., Li, H., Dong, Q., Xu, J., & Sholar, E. (2016). Implicit use of radicals in learning characters for nonnative learners of Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 507527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, J., Li, H., Liu, Y., & Chen, Y. (2020). Orthographic facilitation in oral vocabulary learning: Effects of language backgrounds and orthographic type. Reading and Writing, 33, 187206. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09955-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, J., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar