Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T04:34:39.917Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

L2 Working Memory Capacity and L2 Reading Skill

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2008

Michael Harrington
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Mark Sawyer
Affiliation:
International University of Japan

Abstract

Working memory capacity refers to the ability to store and process information simultaneously in real time and has been shown to correlate highly with first language (L1) reading skill. This study examines the sensitivity of second language (L2) working memory capacity to differences in reading skill among advanced L2 learners. The index of working memory capacity used was the reading span test (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). Subjects with larger working memory capacities scored higher on measures of reading skill, in contrast with the lack of strong correlations between measures of passive short-term storage (memory for strings of random words or digits) and the same reading measures. This result is consistent with an interpretation of the reading span test as an index of working memory capacity, in which capacity is defined functionally in terms of a trade-off between active processing and storage. Issues involved in investigating working memory capacity are discussed and the role of capacity limitations in models of L2 comprehension is considered.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, J. A., & Collins, A. (1985). A schema-theoretic view of reading. In Singer, H. & Ruddell, R. B. (Eds.), Theoretic models and processes of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon.Google ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1989). The role of working memory in language comprehension. In Klahr, D. & Kotovsky, K. (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 3168). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carrell, P., Devine, J., & Eskey, D. (1988). Interactive approaches to second language reading. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cziko, G. (1980). Language competence and reading strategies: A comparison of first- and second-language oral reading errors. Language Learning, 30, 101116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1983). Individual differences in integrating information between and within sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 561583.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Green, I. (1986). Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Tardif, T. (1987). Working memory and reading skill re-examined. In Coltheart, M. (Ed.), Attention and performance XII (pp. 491508). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Favreau, M., & Segalowitz, N. (1983). Automatic and controlled processes in reading a second language. Memory and Cognition, 11, 565574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, G. V., & Hopkins, K. H. (1984). Statistical methods in education and psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1978). Mechanics of verbal ability, Psychological Review, 85, 109130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, D. R. (1977). Individual differences in immediate serial recall: A matter of mnemonics? Cognitive Psychology, 9, 403411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magiste, E. (1986). Selected issues in second and third language learning. In Vaid, J. (Ed.), Language processing in bilinguals: Psycholinguistic and neuropsychological perspectives (pp. 97121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Masson, M. E., & Miller, J. A. (1983). Working memory and individual differences in comprehension and Memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 314318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1987). Theories of second language learning. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
McLeod, B., & McLaughlin, B. (1986). Restructuring of automaticity? Reading in a second language. Language Learning, 36, 109124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits of our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prefetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pienemann, M., & Johnston, M. (1987). Factors influencing the development of language proficiency. In Nunan, D. (Ed.), Applying second language acquisition research (pp. 45141). Adelaide, Australia: National Curriculum Resource Centre.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, N. (1986). Skilled reading in a second language. In Vaid, J. (Ed.), Language processing in bilinguals: Psycholinguistic and neuropsychological perspectives (pp. 319) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in second language learning. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar