Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T08:11:58.864Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

LEARNING CONDITION, LINGUISTIC COMPLEXITY, AND FIRST LANGUAGE TRANSFER IN SEMIARTIFICIAL LANGUAGE LEARNING

A CONCEPTUAL REPLICATION AND EXTENSION OF TAGARELLI ET AL. (2016)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2021

Jianwu Gao
Affiliation:
Capital Normal University
Shuang Ma*
Affiliation:
Capital Normal University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shuang Ma, Department of English Language and Literature, Capital Normal University, Haidian District, Beijing100089, China. E-mail: s.ma5384@cnu.edu.cn

Abstract

This study explored the interaction between learning conditions, linguistic complexity, and first language (L1) syntactic transfer in semiartificial grammar learning by conceptually replicating and extending Tagarelli et al. (2016). We changed the L1 background, elicited production data during debriefing, and added a binary mixed-effects logistic regression analysis to compare variability at learner and item levels with group-level variation on exposure condition, linguistic complexity, and their interaction. Our results replicated those of the original study regarding the comparative efficacy of explicit instruction; however, we also found a condition × complexity interaction absent in the original study. Debriefing sentence-production data suggest that the changed L1-L2 typological distance may have leveled off the advantage of explicit instruction in the learning of the complex V2-VF structure. Finally, our mixed-effects modeling analysis revealed that variability at learner and item levels accounted for a larger proportion of the variance of the outcomes than all the predictors combined.

Type
Research Article
Open Practices
Open materials
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The experiment in this article earned an Open Materials badge for transparent practices. The materials are available at https://www.iris-database.org/iris/app/home/detail?id=york%3a938554&ref=search

We would like to thank Bronson Hui and Ryo Maie from Michigan State University for their detailed, insightful feedback on the drafts of this article. We thank Dr. Kaitlyn Tagarelli from Georgetown University and Dr. Simón Ruiz from University of Tuebingen for kindly providing the instruments that the original study adopted. We are grateful to Prof. Charlene Polio and Prof. Aline Godfroid from Michigan State University for their advice on the research design and data analysis. We thank the two anonymous reviewers and the handling editor Prof. Gregory Keating for their informative feedback. We thank our research assistants, Zizhen Wang and Yiwei Ma, and all the colleagues and students who participated in or supported the study. Finally, we would like to thank Kehao Zhang for technical support. All remaining errors are our own. This research was sponsored by China Scholarship Council.

References

REFERENCES

Andringa, S. J., de Glopper, C. M., & Hacquebord, H. I. (2011). The effect of explicit and implicit instruction on free written response task performance. Language Learning, 61, 868903. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00623.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, P., Clenton, J., & Van Herwegen, J. (2018). L1–L2 semantic and syntactic processing: The influence of language proximity. System, 78, 5464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2018.07.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulté, B., & Housen, A. (2012). Defining and operationalizing L2 complexity. In Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (Eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency: Complexity, accuracy and fluency in SLA (pp. 2146). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Graaff, R. (1997). The Experanto experiment: Effects of explicit instruction on second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 249276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (1995). Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 379410. https://doi.org/0.1017/S027226310001425X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (1998). Beyond focus on form: Cognitive perspectives on learning and practicing second language grammar. In Doughty, C., & Williams, J. (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom language acquisition (pp. 4263). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2012). Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62, 189200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00712.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2016). Of moving targets and chameleons. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 353363. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Putta, P. (2016). The effects of textual enhancement on the acquisition of two nonparallel grammatical features by Spanish-speaking learners of Italian. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 217238. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The psychology of second language acquisition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Doughty, C., & Williams, J. (1998). Pedagogical choices in focus on form. In Doughty, C. & Williams, J. (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 197261). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dryer, M. S., & Gensler, O. D. (2005). Order of object, oblique, and verb. In Dryer, M. S., & Haspelmath, M. (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures (pp. 342345). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305352. https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226310505014X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2006). Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied Linguistics, 27, 164194. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/aml015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141172. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263105050096.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H. (2009). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing, and teaching. Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 11491160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J., Weisberg, S., Price, B., Adler, D., Bates, D., Baud-Bovy, G., … R-Core. (2019). Car package version 3.0-5. https://r-forge.r-project.org/projects/car/ Google Scholar
Godfroid, A. (2016). The effects of implicit instruction on implicit and explicit knowledge development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 177215. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
González, P., & Hernández, L. Q. (2018). Inherent aspect and L1 transfer in the L2 acquisition of Spanish grammatical aspect. Modern Language Journal, 102, 611625. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goo, J., Granena, G., Yilmaz, Y., & Novella, M. (2015). Implicit and explicit instruction in L2 learning: Norris & Ortega (2000) revisited and updated. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 443483). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., & Sachs, R. (2018). Establishing evidence of learning in experiments employing artificial linguistic systems. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 153169. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S. (2017). The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 219234. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Held, L., & Bové, D. S. (2014). Applied statistical inference: Likelihood and Bayes. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (2012). Complexity, accuracy and fluency: Definitions, measurement and research. In Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (Eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency (pp. 120). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Housen, A., Pierrard, M., & Van Daele, S. (2005). Rule complexity and the efficacy of explicit grammar instruction. In Housen, A. & Pierrard, M. (Eds.), Investigations of instructed second language acquisition (pp. 235269). Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Housen, A., & Simoens, H. (2016). Introduction: Cognitive perspectives on difficulty and complexity in L2 acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 163175. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hui, B. (2020). Processing variability in intentional and incidental word learning: An extension of Solovyeva and Dekeyser (2018). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 327357. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H., & de Graaff, R. (1994). Under what conditions does explicit knowledge of a second language facilitate the acquisition of implicit knowledge? A research proposal. AILA Review, 11, 97112.Google Scholar
Hwang, H., Shin, J.-A., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2018). Late bilinguals share syntax unsparingly between L1 and L2: Evidence from crosslinguistically similar and different constructions. Language Learning, 68, 177205. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishikawa, K. (2019). Incidental and explicit learning of L2 derivational morphology and the nature of acquired knowledge. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40, 13771404. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716419000304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning: With applications in R. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kang, E. Y., Sok, S., & Han, Z. (2018). Thirty-five years of ISLA on form-focused instruction: A meta-analysis. Language Teaching Research, 23, 428523. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168818776671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1978). Giving learners a break: Native language intuitions as a source of predictions about transferability. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 15 (pp. 5992). Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.Google Scholar
Kim, K. M., & Godfroid, A. (2019). Should we listen or read? Modality effects in implicit and explicit knowledge. Modern Language Journal, 103, 648664. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12583.Google Scholar
Larson-Hall, J. (2016). A guide to doing statistics in second language research using SPSS and R (2nd ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Leow, R. P. (2019). ISLA: How implicit or how explicit should it be? Theoretical, empirical, and pedagogical/curricular issues. Language Teaching Research, 23, 476493. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168818776674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., & Cunnings, I. (2015). The utility and application of mixed-effects models in second language research. Language Learning, 65, 185207. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luke, K.-K., Liu, H. L., Wai, Y. Y., Wan, Y. L., & Tan, L. H. (2002). Functional anatomy of syntactic and semantic processing in language comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 16, 133145. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., & Kliegl, R. (1984). Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maie, R. (2020, March). Eyeing acceptability judgment task: Automaticity moderates different L2 knowledge and processing. Paper presented at the American Association for Applied Linguistics, Denver, CO.Google Scholar
Maie, R., & DeKeyser, R. M. (2020). Conflicting evidence of explicit and implicit knowledge from objective and subjective measures. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 359382. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mair, P., & Wilcox, R. R. (2020). Robust statistical methods in, R using the WRS2 package. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 464488. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01246-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsden, E., Morgan-Short, K., Thompson, S., & Abugaber, D. (2018). Replication in second language research: Narrative and systematic reviews and recommendations for the field. Language Learning, 68, 321391. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Z. F., & Godfroid, A. (2020). Emotions in incidental language learning: An individual differences approach. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 115141. https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226311900041X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 933947. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mukarami, A. (2016). Modeling systematicity and individuality in nonlinear second language development: The case of English grammatical morphemes. Language Learning, 66, 834871. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12166.Google Scholar
Neeleman, A., & Weerman, F. (1997). L1 and L2 word order acquisition. Language Acquisition, 6, 125170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417528. https://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plonsky, L., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64, 878912. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rankin, T. (2014). Variational learning in L2: The transfer of L1 syntax and parsing strategies in the interpretation of wh-questions by L1 German learners of L2 English. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4, 432461. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.4.4.02ran.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S., Kassin, S. M., Lewis, S., & Cantor, G. W. (1980). On the relationship between implicit and explicit modes in the learning of a complex rule structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 492502.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P. (2008). Implicit learning of natural language syntax (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Cambridge, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P. (Ed.). (2015). Implicit and explicit learning of languages. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 829856. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716411000580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. (1996). Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental, rule-search and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 2768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruiz, S., Tagarelli, K. M., & Rebuschat, P. (2018). Simultaneous acquisition of words and syntax: Effects of exposure condition and declarative memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1168. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R. A. (1994). Word order and nominative case in non-native language acquisition: A longitudinal study of (L1 Turkish) German interlanguage. In Hoekstra, T. & Schwartz, B. D. (Eds.), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar: Papers in honor of Kenneth Wexler from the 1991 Glow Workshop (pp. 317368). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: A meta analysis. Language Learning, 60, 263308. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00562.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y. (2017). Validity of new measures of implicit knowledge: Distinguishing implicit knowledge from automatized explicit knowledge. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 12291261. https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641700011X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2017). The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language: Insights from individual differences in cognitive aptitude. Language Learning, 67, 747790. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagarelli, K., Ruiz, S., Moreno Vega, J. L., & Rebuschat, P. (2016). Variability in second language learning: The roles of individual differences, learning conditions, and linguistic complexity. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 293316. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tao, Y., & Williams, J. N. (2018). Generalization of syntactic knowledge in semiartificial language learning. Language Learning, 68, 10011031. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vafaee, P., Suzuki, Y., & Kachinske, I. (2017). Validating grammaticality judgment tests: Evidence from two new psycholinguistic measures. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 5995. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Patten, B. (2003). Input processing in SLA. In VanPatten, B. (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary (pp. 532). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wilcox, R. R., & Schönbrodt, F. D. (2014). The WRS package for robust statistics in R (version 0.24) [Computer software]. http://r-forge.r-project.org/projects/wrs/ Google Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2005). Form-focused instruction. In Hinkel, E. (Ed.), Handbook on research in second language teaching and learning (pp. 673691). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2010). Initial incidental acquisition of word order regularities: Is it just sequence learning? Language Learning, 60, 221244. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00607.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N., & Evans, J. (1998). What kind of focus and on which forms? In Doughty, C. & Williams, J. (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 139155). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, J. N., & Kuribara, C. (2008). Comparing a nativist and emergentist approach to the initial stage of SLA: An investigation of Japanese scrambling. Lingua, 118, 522553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2007.03.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuen, K. K. (1974). The two sample trimmed t for unequal population variances. Biometrika, 61, 165170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Gao and Ma supplementary material

Gao and Ma supplementary material

Download Gao and Ma supplementary material(File)
File 201.1 KB