Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-09T09:04:45.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Insular activation and functional connectivity in firefighters with post-traumatic stress disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 March 2022

Deokjong Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Yongin Severance Hospital, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Yongin, South Korea; and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
Jung Eun Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Seongnam Saran Hospital, Seongnam, South Korea
Junghan Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea; and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
Changsoo Kim
Affiliation:
Department of Preventive Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea; and Department of Public Health, Yonsei University Graduate School, Seoul, South Korea
Young-Chul Jung*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea; and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
*
Correspondence: Young-Chul Jung. Email: eugenejung@yuhs.ac
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Firefighters are frequently exposed to stressful situations and are at high risk of developing post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Hyperresponsiveness to threatening and emotional stimuli and diminishment of executive control have been suggested as manifestations of PTSD.

Aims

To examine brain activation in firefighters with PTSD by conducting an executive control-related behavioural task with trauma-related interferences.

Method

Twelve firefighters with PTSD and 14 healthy firefighters underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while performing a Stroop match-to-sample task using trauma-related photographic stimuli. Seed-based functional connectivity analysis was conducted using regions identified in fMRI contrast analysis.

Results

Compared with the controls, the participants with PTSD had longer reaction times when the trauma-related interferences were presented. They showed significantly stronger brain activation to interfering trauma-related stimuli in the left insula, and had weaker insular functional connectivity in the supplementary motor area and the anterior cingulate cortex than the controls. They also showed a significant correlation between left insula–supplementary motor area connectivity strength and the hyperarousal subscale of the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale.

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that trauma-related stimuli elicit excessive brain activation in the left insula among firefighters with PTSD. Firefighters with PTSD also appear to have weak left insular functional connectivity with executive control-related brain regions. This aberrant insular activation and functional connectivity could be related to the development and maintenance of PTSD symptoms in firefighters.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) presents with a variety of psychiatric symptoms, including intrusion, avoidance and hyperarousal, which arise after experiencing one or multiple traumatic events. The psychiatric symptoms of PTSD harm several domains of cognitive functioning; for example, the negative impact of PTSD on attentional and executive functioning is well established.Reference Qureshi, Long, Bradshaw, Pyne, Magruder and Kimbrell1 Impaired executive function is further associated with both the development and maintenance of PTSD and its clinical manifestations.Reference Polak, Witteveen, Reitsma and Olff2 Interestingly, research has indicated that difficulty with executive function in people with PTSD is more prominent in trauma-related contextsReference Aupperle, Melrose, Stein and Paulus3 and that individuals with PTSD have difficulty exercising top-down executive control while engaging with stress-related stimuli.Reference Pineles, Shipherd, Mostoufi, Abramovitz and Yovel4

Functional brain imaging studies have identified functional abnormalities in several areas of the brains of people with PTSD. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of PTSD have revealed reduced functional activation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), both of which are related to top-down executive control,Reference Shin, Whalen, Pitman, Bush, Macklin and Lasko5,Reference Shin, Wright, Cannistraro, Wedig, McMullin and Martis6 as well as increased functional activation of the amygdala and the insula, reflecting hyperresponsiveness to stimuli.Reference Armony, Corbo, Clément and Brunet7,Reference Hopper, Frewen, Van der Kolk and Lanius8 These findings are particularly associated with hyperresponsivity when assessing the salience of emotional or threating stimuli. Although fMRI studies have provided important evidence for clarifying the neurobiological mechanisms of PTSD, their results are complex, inconsistent and dependent on the design of the experiments (e.g. which people participated, which tasks were undertaken and which stimuli participants were exposed to).

Firefighters are often faced with stressful situations, including threatening and traumatic events. This psychosocial work environment exposes firefighters to stressors associated with the development of PTSD.Reference Shomstein9 Research has shown that firefighters experience PTSD symptoms more often than the general populationReference Berger, Coutinho, Figueira, Marques-Portella, Luz and Neylan10 and that the physical and psychological stressors they face in their work environments have a negative impact on their cognitive functioning.Reference Taylor, Watkins, Marshall, Dascombe and Foster11 Given that PTSD affects executive control – an important domain of cognitive function and a crucial resilience factor in stress reactionsReference Maier, Amat, Baratta, Paul and Watkins12 – assessing executive control in firefighters with PTSD and the brain activation associated with it is important in identifying the mechanisms of PTSD.

Aims

The purpose of this study was to identify the pathophysiology of PTSD by analysing functional brain activation patterns in firefighters with PTSD. We applied a Stroop match-to-sample task to evaluate the functional activation related to executive control in these firefighters. Photographic stimuli related to the participants’ occupational environment were inserted into the Stroop task as trauma-related interference while they conducted the task. To investigate functional brain activation related to the trauma-related interference stimuli, we conducted functional brain contrast analysis in firefighters with PTSD and compared the results with those of firefighters without PTSD.

Method

Participants

Firefighters who had not received psychiatric treatment but complained of PTSD-related symptoms were recruited for voluntary participation. This study's participants were previously included in the Firefighter Research on the Enhancement of Safety and Health (FRESH) cohortReference Kim, Kim, Choi, Bae, Jang and Lee13 and had therefore been assessed for their physical and psychological health status. All participants completed the Korean version of the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS) for PTSD screening.Reference Foa, Cashman, Jaycox and Perry14 A board-certified psychiatrist evaluated all participants using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) and confirmed whether each participant met the criteria for PTSD diagnosis.Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams15 Individuals who received a PDS score of 15 or higher and met the PTSD diagnostic criteria in the clinical interview were assigned to the PTSD group; 1 male participant had a high PDS score but was excluded because he did not meet the diagnostic criteria. Participants who scored less than 15 points on the PDS and did not meet the PTSD criteria in the clinical interview were classified as controls. Afterwards, the participants’ PTSD-related features were assessed according to the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) through clinical interviews conducted by a psychiatrist.Reference Blake, Weathers, Nagy, Kaloupek, Gusman and Charney16 The presence of comorbid psychiatric disorders was assessed via the SCID-I.Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams15 Participants were excluded if they had a current or past non-PTSD psychiatric illness, history of psychiatric medication use, traumatic brain injury, neurological disease, visual defect or contraindication for MRI. One female was excluded owing to her history of major depressive disorder. This study's participants ultimately included 26 right-handed men and women (control group, n = 14; PTSD group, n = 12). The groups each included one woman; the rest were men. All participants completed a series of questionnaires after PTSD screening, including the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-D),Reference Radloff17 the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI),Reference Ulusoy, Sahin and Erkmen18 the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)Reference Bush, Kivlahan, McDonell, Fihn and Bradley19 and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI).Reference Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Hoch, Yeager and Kupfer20 Each participant's full-scale IQ was measured using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV).Reference Wechsler21

Ethics statement

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by the Ethics Committee of Severance Hospital, Yonsei University Health System in Seoul, Korea (approval no. 4-2016-0187). Written informed consent was obtained after a full description of the scope of the study was given to all participants.

Stroop match-to-sample task using trauma-related stimuli

We used the Stroop match-to-sample task to explore the brain's response to executive control performance when interfering stimuli were introduced,Reference Lee, Lee, Chun, Cho, Kim and Jung22 following the same task structure as was used in our previous study of eating disorders (Fig. 1).Reference Lee, Namkoong and Jung23 The detailed conditions of the task, such as the compositions of Run 1 and Run 2 and the length of time the stimulus was presented, were all identical to those of the previous study. In Run 1, participants responded according to the written colour of a target word; in Run 2, participants responded according to the colour that a target word referred to. Accuracy and reaction time were measured for each participant. However, unlike our previous study, which used photographs of food, this study compared the ‘trauma-related condition’ with the ‘neutral condition’ using trauma-related and neutral stimuli respectively. The trauma-related stimuli consisted of photographs of firefighting or emergency rescues, whereas the neutral stimuli consisted of photographs of daily life. The photographs were selected from 55 trauma-related and 55 daily life-related photographs through a preliminary questionnaire survey. The six trauma-related photos with the most negative valence and highest tension were selected as the trauma-related stimuli, and the six neutral photos with the most positive valence and lowest tension were selected as the neutral stimuli.

Fig. 1 Stroop match-to-sample task.

(a) Examples for trauma-related and neutral stimuli presentations. (b) Examples of the experimental paradigm.

Image acquisition and pre-processing

MRI data were acquired using a 3 T Siemens Magnetom MRI scanner (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with an 8-channel head coil. The T 2-weighted gradient echo-planar pulse sequence was as follows: echo time TE = 30 ms, repetition time TR = 2200 ms, flip angle 90°, field of view 240 mm, matrix 64 × 64, slice thickness 4 mm. The T 1-weighted spoiled gradient echo sequence was as follows: TE = 2.19 ms, TR = 1780 ms, flip angle 9°, field of view 256 mm, matrix 256 × 256, slice thickness 1 mm. Spatial pre-processing and statistical analyses of the functional images were conducted using the Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 software for Windows (SPM12; Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging). To ensure that there was no sudden head movement and that the maximum head movement in each axis was <3 mm, estimates of the realignment parameters for each participant were visually examined. Functional images for each participant were realigned and registered to structural images, which were segmented according to grey matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid. The images were then normalised into standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Spatial parameters were entered into the realigned and unwrapped functional images to re-sample them to a 2 mm isotropic voxel size. Smoothing was applied using an 8 mm full-width at half-maximum kernel.

fMRI contrast analysis and functional connectivity analysis

Individual statistics were computed using a general linear model approach in SPM12. In the first-level analysis, blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast images for the ‘trauma-related condition’ blocks and ‘neutral condition’ blocks were generated for each participant. The resulting set of contrast images was then applied into a second-level analysis using a full-factorial model. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted for brain voxels with the group (PTSD group and control group) as the between-group factor and the task condition (trauma-related and neutral conditions) as the within-group factor to compare the functional connectivity estimates between the PTSD and control groups. Several brain regions were set as regions of interest (ROIs) to conduct the fMRI contrast analysis: both sides of the MPFC, the ACC, the amygdala and the insula. These regions were delineated by the Automated Anatomical Labeling atlasReference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landeau, Papathanassiou, Crivello, Etard and Delcroix24 provided in the Wake Forest University PickAtlas toolbox.Reference Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft and Burdette25 Initially, a voxel-wise cluster-defining threshold of uncorrected P < 0.001 was applied. Then, we reported significant clusters with a cluster-level extent threshold correction of false-wise error rate of P < 0.05.

Seed-based functional connectivity analysis was performed using the regions identified in the fMRI contrast analysis. The CONN-fMRI functional connectivity toolbox (www.nitrc.org/projects/conn) was used to create individual seed-to-voxel functional connectivity maps. The waveform of each brain voxel was filtered using a bandpass filter (0.008 Hz < f < 0.09 Hz) to minimise the effect of low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise. Signals from ventricular regions and white matter were eliminated from the data using linear regression. Correlation coefficients were extracted and converted to z-values to estimate functional connectivity strengths using Fisher's r-to-z transformation. Then, the functional connectivity strength estimates at each voxel were compared between groups using ANOVA. Statistical inferences for the whole-brain analyses were set to a voxel-wise cluster-defining threshold of uncorrected P < 0.001, with a cluster-level extent threshold correction of false-wise error rate of P < 0.05.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 24.0 statistical software for Windows (SPSS, Chicago IL, USA) and the threshold for statistical significance was set at P < 0.05. Demographic, clinical and behavioural variables were compared using a two-sample t-test. We conducted correlation analyses to examine the relationship between brain functional characteristics and clinical features. The functional connectivity strength was calculated by extracting the Fisher-transformed z-values of functional connectivity between the left insula seed and the target regions that were identified through functional connectivity analysis. Age- and BAI-controlled partial correlation analyses were conducted to determine whether the functional connectivity estimates correlated with the scores on the CAPS subscales and/or behavioural variables of the Stroop match-to-sample task.

Results

Clinical characteristics

There were no statistically significant differences between the PTSD and control groups in age, gender or IQ (Table 1). The PTSD group had higher PDS and CAPS scores than the control group (PDS: P < 0.0001; CAPS: P < 0.001). The PTSD group had higher scores than the control group on all subscales of the CAPS (intrusion: P < 0.001; avoidance: P < 0.001; hyperarousal: P < 0.001). There were no statistically significant differences between the PTSD and control groups in the CES-D, AUDIT and PSQI results. The PTSD group had higher BAI scores than the controls (P = 0.010). The comparison of behavioural performance on the Stroop match-to-sample task showed a difference only in reaction times in the trauma-related condition (P = 0.044; Table 2).

Table 1 Demographic and clinical variables of participants

AUDIT, Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; BAI, Beck Anxiety Inventory; CAPS, Clinician-Administered Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Scale; CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale; PDS, Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

a. Data on gender are expressed as n (%).

Table 2 Behavioural performance results

PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

Brain activation associated with interfering trauma-related stimuli

In the task-based fMRI analyses, the interaction effects for the group × task condition were significant in the left insula (peak MNI coordinates: −36, −6, −10; cluster size k = 425; peak z = 4.56; P = 0.013, cluster-corrected; Fig. 2(a)). Post hoc analyses revealed that the PTSD group exhibited significantly stronger BOLD responses to interfering trauma-related stimuli in the left insula. The main effects of grouping (PTSD group and control group) were significant in the frontal eye field (FEF) (peak MNI coordinates: −10, 42, 52; k = 1007; peak z = 5.70; P < 0.001, cluster-corrected; Fig. 2(b)). Post hoc analyses revealed that the PTSD group had weaker BOLD activity in the FEF than the control group. In contrast, there was no significant cluster in which the main effects of the task were significant.

Fig. 2 Region of interest (ROI)-based functional magnetic resonance imaging contrast analysis.

Both sides of the medial prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, the amygdala and the insula were set as ROIs. Statistical inference was set as an uncorrected P-value height threshold of 0.001 in conjunction with an extent threshold correction of false-wise error rate of P < 0.05. (a) The interaction effects for the group-by-task condition were significant in the left insula. (b) The main effects of the group were significant in the frontal eye field.

Functional connectivity analysis in the left insula

One left insula seed consisted of a 5 mm radius sphere centred on the coordinates x = −36, y = −6, z = 10, where the interaction effects were significant. The PTSD group had weaker insular functional connectivity with the supplementary motor area (peak MNI coordinates: 4, −4, 70; k = 258; peak z = 4.14; P = 0.007, cluster-corrected; Fig. 3(a)) and the ACC (peak MNI coordinates: −6, 20, 30; k = 189; peak z = 3.93; P = 0.031, cluster-corrected; Fig. 3(b)) than the control group. In contrast, there was no significant cluster in which the PTSD group had stronger insular functional connectivity than the controls.

Fig. 3 Left-insula-based functional connectivity analysis.

The statistical inference was set as an uncorrected P-value height threshold of 0.001 in conjunction with an extent threshold correction of false-wise error rate of P < 0.05. (a) Compared with controls, participants with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) showed significantly weaker functional connectivity between the left insula and the supplementary motor area. (b) Compared with controls, participants with PTSD showed significantly weaker functional connectivity between the left insula and the anterior cingulate cortex.

Correlation between functional connectivity and clinical variables

A significant correlation was shown for functional connectivity between the left insula and the supplementary motor area such that the smaller the functional connectivity strength, the higher the CAPS hyperarousal subscale scores in the PTSD group (r = −0.699, P = 0.025; Fig. 4). Left insula–supplementary motor area connectivity strength was not significantly correlated with the other CAPS subscale scores (intrusion: P = 0.675; avoidance: P = 0.793). The correlation tests showed no statistical significance for functional connectivity between the left insula and ACC (intrusion: P = 0.641; avoidance: P = 0.553; hyperarousal: P = 0.710). There was no significant correlation between left insula–supplementary motor area connectivity and the CAPS hyperarousal subscale scores in the control group (P = 0.214).

Fig. 4 Partial correlation analyses after controlling for age and Beck Anxiety Inventory score.

Non-standardised residuals were used to make scatter plots. Participants with PTSD exhibited a negative correlation between functional connectivity in the left insula–supplementary motor area and score on the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale hyperarousal subscale (r = −0.699, P = 0.025).

Discussion

Insular activation

In this study, we evaluated brain activation in firefighters with and without PTSD during a behavioural Stroop match-to-sample task involving executive control. Participants with PTSD did not show any significant differences in accuracy of task completion compared with the controls. However, the PTSD group had longer reaction times when presented with trauma-related interference. This may reflect a stronger psychological response to trauma-related interferences in those with PTSD. Furthermore, the PTSD group showed greater functional activation of the left insular regions in response to interfering trauma-related stimuli while exercising executive control. We suggest that this insular hyperactivation may be related to the longer response times during exposure to trauma-related stimuli. However, to elucidate the link between the present findings and the clinical aspects of PTSD, further studies including more integrated behavioural assessments are needed.

The observed hyperactivation of the left insula in response to trauma-related interferences in the Stroop match-to-sample task might reflect hyperresponsiveness to trauma-related stimuli in the PTSD group. Our findings are consistent with those of previous studies that reported hyperactivation of the insular regions in PTSD during exposure to emotional stimuli.Reference Bruce, Buchholz, Brown, Yan, Durbin and Sheline26,Reference Simmons, Paulus, Thorp, Matthews, Norman and Stein27 The insula is responsible for salience processing of emotional stimuli and linking it with cognitive control processing.Reference Menon and Uddin28 We suggest that the PTSD group might experience trauma-related stimuli as more highly salient interferences while performing executive control. Although the insula is commonly examined in PTSD-related studies, the locations of brain clusters where functional alterations occur vary depending on the context in which traumatic stimuli are presented in task-based fMRI.Reference Hughes and Shin29 For instance, previous studies on PTSD reported functional insular alterations on the right rather than the left in response to passively presented trauma-related scripts.Reference Cisler, Steele, Lenow, Smitherman, Everett and Messias30,Reference Lindauer, Booij, Habraken, Van Meijel, Uylings and Olff31 The salience network in which the insula participates has been suggested to be bilateral.Reference Uddin32 However, evidence suggests that the left insula is more prominently connected with the executive control networkReference Seeley, Menon, Schatzberg, Keller, Glover and Kenna33 and correlated with behavioural adaptation for salience information.Reference Späti, Chumbley, Brakowski, Dörig, Grosse Holtforth and Seifritz34 We consider that this study's presentation of trauma-related stimuli (i.e. inserted as interference during a Stroop task) is consistent with the findings of left-insular alterations.

Functional connectivity

In functional connectivity analysis with the left insular seed region, the PTSD group exhibited weak functional connectivity of the left insula with the supplementary motor area and ACC. These findings coincide with a previous insula-based functional connectivity study of PTSD that reported weak functional connectivity between the left insula and the ACC.Reference Zhang, Xie, Chen, Li, Guo and Chen35 The ACC is associated with top-down executive functionsReference Carter, Botvinick and Cohen36 involving conflict monitoring and decision-making. The supplementary motor area may also be involved in proper cognitive control.Reference Nachev, Kennard and Husain37 The weak insular functional connectivity in the supplementary motor area and the ACC observed in this study may reflect diminished cognitive control of salience processing in PTSD. Moreover, we showed that the lower functional connectivity between the left insula and the supplementary motor area was correlated with higher scores on PTSD symptom scales, particularly the CAPS hyperarousal subscale. Others have shown that impaired executive control contributes to dysfunctional coping strategies and other manifestations of PTSDReference Aupperle, Melrose, Stein and Paulus3 and have suggested an inverse relationship between cognitive task performance and hyperarousal symptoms.Reference Vasterling, Brailey, Constans and Sutker38 Taken together, our findings suggest that the insular functional connectivity characteristics of firefighters with PTSD have a close relationship with their clinical features.

In addition to functional alterations in the insula, the PTSD group showed lower functional activity in the FEF than the controls in the task-based fMRI analysis. As one of the core brain regions of the dorsal attention network, the FEF may be involved in top-down attentional control,Reference Esterman, Liu, Okabe, Reagan, Thai and DeGutis39 which plays an important role in the adaptive regulation of emotional responses in individuals with PTSD.Reference White, Costanzo, Blair and Roy40 The FEF is also involved in the modulation of eye movement and may be one of the neural correlates of eye-movement desensitisation and reprocessing therapy for PTSD.Reference Harricharan, McKinnon, Tursich, Densmore, Frewen and Théberge41 Despite a lack of significance in the group × task interaction effects, the group differences in BOLD activity in the FEF suggest that the FEF may contribute to the neurobiological pathophysiology of PTSD.

Strengths and limitations

This study performed task-based fMRI to observe the differences in responses to interference during executive control tasks, not simply in response to trauma-related stimuli. Although many studies have examined the increased responsiveness of the insula to trauma-related stimuli, task-based fMRI studies exploring responses to implicitly inserted trauma-related interference are relatively scarce.Reference Hughes and Shin29 Furthermore, this study was conducted for a special occupational group of firefighters who had not yet received psychiatric treatment.

However, this study has several limitations. First, the sample was too small to fully investigate functional brain characteristics of PTSD. The results of this study, which were not equally reproduced for all brain regions identified in previous studies, may be affected by this small sample size. Second, the homogeneity of the trauma experienced by participants in this study was not well controlled, and differences in trauma experiences may have affected the psychological responses to the photographic stimuli used in the study. Future studies that use larger samples of firefighters and control for types of trauma are needed to elaborate on the neuroimaging results of this study.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all the FRESH cohort participants, without whom this study would not have been possible.

Author contributions

D.L. and C.K. made substantial contributions to the analysis and interpretation of the data. D.L. drafted the manuscript. J.E.L and J.L. critically revised the manuscript to convey important intellectual content. Y.-C.J. conceived, designed and directed the study. All authors participated sufficiently in the work and take public responsibility for appropriate portions of the content.

Funding

This study was funded by the Fire Fighting Safety & 119 Rescue Technology Research and Development Program funded by the Korean National Fire Agency (‘MPSS-Firesafety-2015-80’).

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Qureshi, SU, Long, ME, Bradshaw, MR, Pyne, JM, Magruder, KM, Kimbrell, T, et al. Does PTSD impair cognition beyond the effect of trauma? J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2011; 23: 1628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polak, AR, Witteveen, AB, Reitsma, JB, Olff, M. The role of executive function in posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review. J Affect Disord 2012; 141: 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aupperle, RL, Melrose, AJ, Stein, MB, Paulus, MP. Executive function and PTSD: disengaging from trauma. Neuropharmacology 2012; 62: 686–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pineles, SL, Shipherd, JC, Mostoufi, SM, Abramovitz, SM, Yovel, I. Attentional biases in PTSD: more evidence for interference. Behav Res Ther 2009; 47: 1050–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, LM, Whalen, PJ, Pitman, RK, Bush, G, Macklin, ML, Lasko, NB, et al. An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50: 932–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, LM, Wright, CI, Cannistraro, PA, Wedig, MM, McMullin, K, Martis, B, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 273–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armony, JL, Corbo, V, Clément, M-H, Brunet, A. Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 1961–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopper, JW, Frewen, PA, Van der Kolk, BA, Lanius, RA. Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: Symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. J Trauma Stress 2007; 20: 713–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shomstein, S. Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex: top-down and bottom-up attentional control. Front Integr Neurosci 2012; 6: 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, W, Coutinho, ESF, Figueira, I, Marques-Portella, C, Luz, MP, Neylan, TC, et al. Rescuers at risk: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis of the worldwide current prevalence and correlates of PTSD in rescue workers. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2012; 47: 1001–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, L, Watkins, SL, Marshall, H, Dascombe, BJ, Foster, J. The impact of different environmental conditions on cognitive function: a focused review. Front Physiol 2016; 6: 372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, SF, Amat, J, Baratta, MV, Paul, E, Watkins, LR. Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2006; 8: 397406.Google ScholarPubMed
Kim, YT, Kim, WJ, Choi, JE, Bae, M-j, Jang, H, Lee, CJ, et al. Cohort profile: firefighter research on the enhancement of safety and health (FRESH), a prospective cohort study on Korean firefighters. Yonsei Med J 2020; 61: 103–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, EB, Cashman, L, Jaycox, L, Perry, K. The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychol Assess 1997; 9: 445–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JB. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1995.Google Scholar
Blake, DD, Weathers, FW, Nagy, LM, Kaloupek, DG, Gusman, FD, Charney, DS, et al. The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. J Trauma Stress 1995; 8: 7590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977; 1: 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ulusoy, M, Sahin, NH, Erkmen, H. The Beck Anxiety Inventory: psychometric properties. J Cogn Psychother 1998; 12: 163–72.Google Scholar
Bush, K, Kivlahan, DR, McDonell, MB, Fihn, SD, Bradley, KA. The AUDIT alcohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): an effective brief screening test for problem drinking. Arch Intern Med 1998; 158: 1789–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buysse, DJ, Reynolds, CF III, Monk, TH, Hoch, CC, Yeager, AL, Kupfer, DJ. Quantification of subjective sleep quality in healthy elderly men and women using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Sleep 1991; 14: 331–8.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). NCS Pearson, 2008.Google Scholar
Lee, J, Lee, S, Chun, JW, Cho, H, Kim, D-J, Jung, Y-C. Compromised prefrontal cognitive control over emotional interference in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2015; 18: 661–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, JE, Namkoong, K, Jung, Y-C. Impaired prefrontal cognitive control over interference by food images in binge-eating disorder and bulimia nervosa. Neurosci Lett 2017; 651: 95101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N, Landeau, B, Papathanassiou, D, Crivello, F, Etard, O, Delcroix, N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 2002; 15: 273–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maldjian, JA, Laurienti, PJ, Kraft, RA, Burdette, JH. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 2003; 19: 1233–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, SE, Buchholz, KR, Brown, WJ, Yan, L, Durbin, A, Sheline, YI. Altered emotional interference processing in the amygdala and insula in women with post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuroimage Clin 2013; 2: 43–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, AN, Paulus, MP, Thorp, SR, Matthews, SC, Norman, SB, Stein, MB. Functional activation and neural networks in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to intimate partner violence. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64: 681–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V, Uddin, LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 2010; 214: 655–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, KC, Shin, LM. Functional neuroimaging studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Expert Rev Neurother 2011; 11: 275–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisler, JM, Steele, JS, Lenow, JK, Smitherman, S, Everett, B, Messias, E, et al. Functional reorganization of neural networks during repeated exposure to the traumatic memory in posttraumatic stress disorder: an exploratory fMRI study. J Psychiatr Res 2014; 48: 4755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindauer, R, Booij, J, Habraken, J, Van Meijel, E, Uylings, H, Olff, M, et al. Effects of psychotherapy on regional cerebral blood flow during trauma imagery in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder: a randomized clinical trial. Psychol Med 2008; 38: 543–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uddin, LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015; 16: 5561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeley, WW, Menon, V, Schatzberg, AF, Keller, J, Glover, GH, Kenna, H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 2349–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Späti, J, Chumbley, J, Brakowski, J, Dörig, N, Grosse Holtforth, M, Seifritz, E, et al. Functional lateralization of the anterior insula during feedback processing. Hum Brain Mapp 2014; 35: 4428–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y, Xie, B, Chen, H, Li, M, Guo, X, Chen, H. Disrupted resting-state insular subregions functional connectivity in post-traumatic stress disorder. Medicine 2016; 95(27): e4083.Google ScholarPubMed
Carter, CS, Botvinick, MM, Cohen, JD. The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex to executive processes in cognition. Rev Neurosci 1999; 10: 4958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nachev, P, Kennard, C, Husain, M. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9: 856–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vasterling, JJ, Brailey, K, Constans, JI, Sutker, PB. Attention and memory dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychol 1998; 12: 125–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esterman, M, Liu, G, Okabe, H, Reagan, A, Thai, M, DeGutis, J. Frontal eye field involvement in sustaining visual attention: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 2015; 111: 542–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, SF, Costanzo, ME, Blair, JR, Roy, MJ. PTSD symptom severity is associated with increased recruitment of top-down attentional control in a trauma-exposed sample. Neuroimage Clin 2015; 7: 1927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harricharan, S, McKinnon, MC, Tursich, M, Densmore, M, Frewen, P, Théberge, J, et al. Overlapping frontoparietal networks in response to oculomotion and traumatic autobiographical memory retrieval: implications for eye movement desensitization and reprocessing. Eur J Psychotraumatol 2019; 10(1): 1586265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Stroop match-to-sample task.(a) Examples for trauma-related and neutral stimuli presentations. (b) Examples of the experimental paradigm.

Figure 1

Table 1 Demographic and clinical variables of participants

Figure 2

Table 2 Behavioural performance results

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Region of interest (ROI)-based functional magnetic resonance imaging contrast analysis.Both sides of the medial prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, the amygdala and the insula were set as ROIs. Statistical inference was set as an uncorrected P-value height threshold of 0.001 in conjunction with an extent threshold correction of false-wise error rate of P < 0.05. (a) The interaction effects for the group-by-task condition were significant in the left insula. (b) The main effects of the group were significant in the frontal eye field.

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Left-insula-based functional connectivity analysis.The statistical inference was set as an uncorrected P-value height threshold of 0.001 in conjunction with an extent threshold correction of false-wise error rate of P < 0.05. (a) Compared with controls, participants with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) showed significantly weaker functional connectivity between the left insula and the supplementary motor area. (b) Compared with controls, participants with PTSD showed significantly weaker functional connectivity between the left insula and the anterior cingulate cortex.

Figure 5

Fig. 4 Partial correlation analyses after controlling for age and Beck Anxiety Inventory score.Non-standardised residuals were used to make scatter plots. Participants with PTSD exhibited a negative correlation between functional connectivity in the left insula–supplementary motor area and score on the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale hyperarousal subscale (r = −0.699, P = 0.025).

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.