Hostname: page-component-6b989bf9dc-md2j5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-15T04:39:34.245Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Influence of mealtime habits on the risk of weight gain and obesity in Mexican adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2016

Libia Darina Dosamantes-Carrasco
Affiliation:
École Interdisciplinaire Sciences-Santé, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, University of Lyon, Lyon, France Unidad de Investigación Epidemiológica y en Servicios de Salud, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Cuernavaca, México
Pablo Méndez-Hernández*
Affiliation:
Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Autónoma de Tlaxcala, Calle Ciencias de la Salud sur No. 11, Guardia, Zacatelco, CP 90750, Tlaxcala, México Departamento de Calidad y Educación en Salud, Secretaría de Salud de Tlaxcala, Ignacio Picazo Norte # 25, Colonia Centro, CP 90800, Tlaxcala, México
Yvonne N Flores
Affiliation:
Unidad de Investigación Epidemiológica y en Servicios de Salud, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Cuernavaca, México UCLA Department of Health, School of Public Health and Jonsson Comprehensive Cancer, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Carole Siani
Affiliation:
Aix Marseille University, INSERM, IRD, SESSTIM UMRS912, Marseille, France University of Lyon, University Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Lyon, France
Edgar Denova-Gutiérrez
Affiliation:
Unidad de Investigación en Epidemiología Clínica, Hospital Infantil de México ‘Federico Gómez’, Ciudad de México, México
Katia Gallegos-Carrillo
Affiliation:
Unidad de Investigación Epidemiológica y en Servicios de Salud, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Cuernavaca, México
Paula Ramírez
Affiliation:
Unidad de Investigación Epidemiológica y en Servicios de Salud, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Cuernavaca, México
Berenice Rivera-Paredez
Affiliation:
Unidad de Investigación Epidemiológica y en Servicios de Salud, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Cuernavaca, México
Eduardo Salazar-Martínez
Affiliation:
Centro de Investigación en Salud Poblacional, Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública, Cuernavaca, México
Jorge Salmerón
Affiliation:
Unidad de Investigación Epidemiológica y en Servicios de Salud, Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social, Cuernavaca, México Centro de Investigación en Salud Poblacional, Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública, Cuernavaca, México
*
*Corresponding author: Email pmendezh@hotmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To prospectively examine the extent to which mealtime habits influences the risk of weight gain and obesity in Mexican adults.

Design

We performed a prospective cohort study. The Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) scale was used for assessing participants’ MHQ; the outcomes of interest were gain ≥5 % of body weight, developing overweight/obesity and abdominal obesity, after 7 years of follow-up. In order to estimate the independent effect of MHQ on anthropometric indicators, generalized linear models were computed to obtain adjusted relative risks (95 % CI).

Setting

The state of Morelos, Mexico.

Subjects

Mexican adults (n 837) aged 18–70 years participating in a cohort study.

Results

Compared with participants classified in the higher MHQ category, individuals in the middle and lower MHQ groups had a 4·1 (2·5, 6·7) and 6·2 (3·9, 9·7) fold greater risk of gain ≥5 % of body weight, respectively; 6·6 (2·8, 15·5) and 8·6 (3·7, 19·8) fold greater risk of becoming overweight/obese, respectively; and 3·8 (2·0, 7·3) and 5·3 (2·8, 9·8) fold greater risk of developing abdominal obesity, respectively.

Conclusions

This study provides evidence about the influence of a set of mealtime habits on obesity indicators, showing that greater adherence to unadvisable mealtime habits increases the risk of developing unhealthy anthropometric indicators. Since the meal is one of the most important sources of food intake, and consequently weight status, the MHQ scale can be a useful population tool to predict weight gain and obesity.

Type
Research Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2016 

The social scientific community suggests that the global epidemic of obesity cannot be explained by focusing solely on food products and amount of food intake, showing that what people do while eating can predict food consumption and weight status( Reference Wansink 1 ). For instance, distractions during eating meals, such as watching television (TV), reading, working, listening to a detective story, listening to music or playing video games, can increase how much food is consumed. These behaviours have also been associated with higher intakes of fat, fast foods and soft drinks, but lower intakes of fruit and vegetables, resulting in a greater likelihood of becoming overweight( Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Slama 2 Reference Hetherington, Anderson and Norton 10 ). Other sources of distractions are social interactions, which inhibit consumption monitoring and make people more vulnerable to mindless eating, increasing energy intake and weight gain risk( Reference Wansink 1 , Reference Martínez-Díaz 11 , Reference Raulio, Roos and Mukala 12 ). In fact, researchers have shown that non food-related environmental stimuli, such as TV and auditory stimuli, are able to provoke a significant increase of meal size and energy intake( Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Slama 2 ).

Some other behaviours that influence food intake and body weight are related with a lack of sufficient time to eat: rushing meals has been associated with greater soft drink, fast food and fat consumption, as well as with a lower intake of healthful foods( Reference Larson, Nelson and Neumark-Sztainer 13 ); skipping breakfast has been related with higher rates of obesity and elevated fasting blood insulin and cholesterol levels( Reference Smith, Gall and McNaughton 14 , Reference Timlin and Pereira 15 ); and eating out of home has been associated with a diet high in energy and fat( Reference Lachat, Nago and Verstraeten 16 ), and lower vegetable and fruit consumption( Reference Crawford, Ball and Mishra 17 ), which leads to a higher risk of obesity( Reference Kruger, Blanck and Gillespie 18 ).

The eating environment, which includes the ambience atmospherics, social interactions and distractions, may lead to decreased consumption monitoring, resulting in an increase in food consumption( Reference Wansink 1 ). The potential stimulatory influence of different environmental factors can make people more vulnerable to mindless eating. For instance, the sensory stimulation of being with family, friends or colleagues provokes eating, which influences meal size and increases energy intake regardless of hunger or need status at the start of the meal( Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Slama 2 , Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Airinei 19 , Reference De Castro 20 ).

The Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) scale was previously assessed in a cross-sectional study in Mexican adults participating in the Health Workers Cohort Study (HWCS) and computes the contribution of eight advisable and unadvisable mealtime items. As expected, lower MHQ score was related to higher rates of overweight and abdominal obesity, unhealthy dietary patterns, lower intakes of fruits and vegetables, and lower levels of leisure-time physical activity( Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 ). In the present research we attempt to highlight how a set of behaviours that people do at the mealtimes can influence some key health anthropometric indicators. Thus, in the present study we sought to estimate the risk of gaining body weight, becoming overweight/obese and developing abdominal obesity after 7 years of follow-up, according to Mexican adults’ mealtime habits quality.

Materials and methods

Study population

This is a prospective cohort study composed of 1725 active and retired health workers aged 18–70 years at baseline, who participated in two data collection periods of the HWCS: baseline assessment at 2004–2006 and follow-up assessment at 2010–2012. The HWCS is an ongoing cohort study to assess relationships between lifestyle and health, being conducted at the Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social (Mexican Social Security Institute) and the Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública (National Institute of Public Health), both located in Cuernavaca, Morelos state, Mexico( Reference Gallegos-Carrillo, Flores and Denova-Gutiérrez 22 ). We excluded those participants who did not complete all questions related to mealtime habits at both data collection periods (n 653) and those with health conditions that affect weight or mealtime habits in either assessment, such as diabetes or taking diabetes medications (n 144), determination of plasma glucose ≥125 mg/dl (n 34), cancer (n 28), kidney failure (n 10), CVD (n 10) and pregnancy (n 9); thus, the study population consisted of 837 participants (209 men and 628 women). This research was performed in accordance with the Helsinki declaration on human studies( 23 ); each person signed an approved informed consent form prior to entering the study. The Ethics Committee of the Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social from the state of Morelos approved the study protocol.

Assessment of mealtime habits quality

Originally, the MHQ scale was constructed using eight items in binary response format and was validated among 7472 Mexican health workers who were participating in the cohort study( Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 ). In order to examine a wider range of possible mealtime behaviours( Reference Spector 24 ), the MHQ scale was transformed to a multiple-choice response format in the follow-up assessment (Table 1).

Table 1 Factor loadings of the mealtime habit items, comparing binary responses at baseline and multiple-choice responses at follow-up assessment; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

TV, television.

First, mealtime items were categorized into two groups, advisable and unadvisable for good health, based on published studies that report how certain mealtime circumstances can influence diet and body weight( Reference Wansink 1 , Reference Thorp, McNaughton and Owen 5 , Reference Cleland, Schmidt and Dwyer 8 , Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 ). Then, the following six specific meal situations were examined in the present study.

  1. 1. Availability of time to eat was measured using four items from the baseline assessment: ‘I take my time to finish my meal’, ‘I rush my meals to avoid exceeding the available time to eat’, ‘I eat in huge mouthfuls’ and ‘I eat slowly’. In order to avoid duplication, these four items were replaced with two questions at the follow-up evaluation: ‘How fast do you eat your meals?’, since this question has been previously associated with lack of time to eat and obesity( Reference Larson, Nelson and Neumark-Sztainer 13 , Reference Lachat, Nago and Verstraeten 16 ), and additionally we included the question ‘Do you skip any meal a day?’, since skipping meals has been related with obesity( Reference Smith, Gall and McNaughton 14 , Reference Timlin and Pereira 15 , Reference Goon and Islam 25 ).

  2. 2. Distraction from eating has been related to increased food intake and obesity( Reference Ogden, Coop and Cousins 3 Reference Moray, Fu and Brill 9 ). This mealtime situation was measured using two items from the baseline assessment: ‘I’m distracted: I talk, watch TV or read’ and ‘I take advantage of mealtimes to accomplish work activities’. These two items were combined into one question for the follow-up evaluation: ‘Are you distracted while eating, either by watching TV, working, reading, chatting or solving everyday problems?’

  3. 3. Food selection, which is closely related to energy intake and BMI( Reference Robinson, Aveyard and Daley 4 ), was measured at baseline with the item ‘I choose what I eat’. However, in order to be more specific about the type and amount of food selected by participants, this question was divided into two questions for the follow-up assessment: ‘Do you choose the type of food you eat, thinking about your health?’ and ‘Do you choose the amount of food that you eat?’

  4. 4. Familial and cultural eating habits have been related to increased food intake( Reference Wansink 1 , Reference De Castro 20 ). This was assessed at baseline with the item ‘I eat all my food, without leaving anything on the plate’; while at the follow-up it was changed to ‘Do you eat all your food, without leaving anything on the plate?’

The two following mealtime situations, which have been shown to influence food intake, were incorporated at the follow-up assessment.

  1. 5. Environmental and social context of eating, which was measured with the items ‘Do you eat together with friends, family or colleges?’( Reference Wansink 1 , Reference De Castro 20 ) and ‘How many times a week do you eat the main meal at home?’( Reference Larson, Nelson and Neumark-Sztainer 13 Reference Lachat, Nago and Verstraeten 16 ).

  2. 6. Enjoy eating, which was assessed with the question ‘Do you enjoy eating?’, because greater pleasure with food has been associated with fewer food anxieties, less dieting behaviours and lower BMI( Reference Smith and Hawks 26 ) (Table 1).

Next, in order to evaluate the equivalence between the two versions of the MHQ instrument, we used parallel-form reliability( Reference DeVellis 27 ). Both formats of the MHQ questionnaire were tested in a cross-sectional study about lifestyle and stated preferences regarding health promotion programmes to prevent chronic diseases among 425 employees of a Mexican public university( Reference Mendez-Hernandez, Dosamantes-Carrasco and Siani 28 , Reference Méndez-Hernández, Siani and Lamure 29 ). Thus, respondents answered both formats of the MHQ questionnaire at the same time: the binary items were placed at the beginning of the questionnaire and the multiple-choice items at the end.

In order to compare if the multiple-choice format of the MHQ questionnaire was compatible with the MHQ in binary format, we transformed the multiple-choice format into binary responses. Participants were categorized into ‘yes’ when they answered ‘almost always’ or ‘always’ in the multiple-choice version, and were categorized into ‘no’ when they responded ‘almost never’ or ‘never’. Participants who selected the option ‘sometimes’ in the Likert format were eliminated from the analysis, because typically the Likert scale midpoints indicate ambivalence or indifference and they are not indicative of a tendency( Reference DeVellis 27 ). The binary format and the dichotomized Likert scale were very similar in the following ways: (i) the percentage of participants who answered positively or negatively – in both instruments – was very similar (P≥0·726 in all items); (ii) the overall mean scores and sd of the MHQ were very similar for both formats, a mean of 5·13 for the binary and 5·12 for the dichotomized-Likert format (P=0·997); and (iii) the overall reliability coefficient between two scales was 0·946 (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Table 1).

Assessment of dietary patterns

Dietary patterns and energy intake were obtained using a semi-quantitative FFQ validated in a Mexican population( Reference Hernández-Avila, Romieu and Parra 30 ). This questionnaire included data on the consumption frequency of 116 food items commonly consumed over the past year, ranging from never to ≥6 times/d.

We used the three major dietary patterns that were previously identified in a cross-sectional study of 9467 Mexican adults participating in the HWCS( Reference Denova-Gutiérrez, Castañón and Talavera 31 ) to assess changes in adherence to certain dietary patterns that have been associated with anthropometric indicators and adiposity( Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 , Reference Gallegos-Carrillo, Flores and Denova-Gutiérrez 22 , Reference Denova-Gutiérrez, Castañón and Talavera 31 ) . Briefly, in both assessments, food items were classified into twenty-eight food groups based on similarity in nutrients, lipid content profile, sugar content, proportion of dietary fibre and commonly consumed foods. Three dietary patterns were derived: the prudent pattern is typified by greater intakes of processed vegetable juices, potatoes, fresh fruits, fresh vegetables and legumes, and a lower intake of pastries; the Western pattern is characterized by higher intakes of pastries, refined cereals, corn tortillas and soft drinks, and lower intakes of whole cereals, seafood and full-fat dairy products; and the high animal protein/fat dietary pattern is typified by greater intakes of red meat, processed meat, margarine and eggs, and lower intakes of fruits and wholegrain cereals. The factor score for each dietary pattern was constructed by summing the standardized percentages of energy intake of food groups, weighted by their factor loading. We created panel data for longitudinal analysis by obtaining a personal-level data set of the participants’ adherence to the three dietary patterns, for both stages of the study. Then, individuals were classified into tertiles, with the highest tertile reflecting greater adherence to each dietary pattern.

Anthropometric assessment

At the baseline and follow-up assessment, weight was measured with a previously calibrated electronic TANITA scale (model BC-533; Tokyo, Japan) on participants who had fasted overnight and were wearing minimal clothing. Height was measured using a conventional stadiometer while the participants were standing barefoot, with their shoulders in a normal position. Waist circumference was measured to the nearest 0·1 cm at the high point of the iliac crest at the end of normal expiration, with a steel measuring tape. In order to analyse waist circumference, we considered the categories proposed by the National Cholesterol Education Program Adult Treatment Panel III: normal waist circumference was defined if women presented <88 cm and men <102 cm; and abdominal obesity was defined by waist circumference ≥88 cm for women and ≥102 cm for men( 32 ). All measurement procedures were performed by nurses trained to use standardized procedures. BMI (kg/m2) was calculated as the ratio of weight to height squared. In order to analyse BMI, we used the categories proposed by the WHO: normal weight, BMI=18·5–24·9 kg/m2; overweight, BMI=25·0–29·9 kg/m2; obesity, BMI≥30·0 kg/m2 ( 33 ).

The anthropometric outcomes of interest after 7 years of follow-up were: gaining ≥5 % of body weight, since this cut-off point represents an increased risk for developing type 2 diabetes( Reference Colditz, Willett and Rotnitzky 34 ); becoming overweight or obese( 35 ); and developing abdominal obesity, because this is a more accurate predictor of all-cause mortality( Reference Katzmarzyk, Mire and Bray 36 ), cardiac and/or metabolic disease risk than excess weight gain evaluated by BMI( Reference Maru, Van Der Schouw and Gimbrère 37 ).

Depression

Depressive symptoms were assessed using a Spanish-language depression scale, a version of a twenty-item questionnaire created by the Center for Epidemiologic Studies( Reference Radloff 38 ). We defined probable clinical depression using the mean plus one sd as a cut-off point; this criterion has been previously used to define depressive symptoms in a Mexican population( Reference Gallegos-Carrillo, Flores and Denova-Gutiérrez 22 ). To determine which participants had depressive symptoms at baseline and follow-up assessments, we generated a continuous scale from 0 to 60 points and a score of ≥16 suggested probable clinical depression( Reference Gallegos-Carrillo, Flores and Denova-Gutiérrez 22 , Reference Radloff 38 ).

Physical activity and smoking

Leisure-time physical activity was assessed using a validated physical activity questionnaire( Reference Wolf, Hunter and Colditz 39 ), validated in Spanish( Reference Martínez-González, López-Fontana and Varo 40 ), and adapted for the Mexican urban population of the HWCS( Reference Méndez-Hernández, Flores and Siani 41 ). We defined two categories of physical activity level: <30 min/d and ≥30 min/d, based on the minimum amount of time of physical activity recommended for health in Mexican adults( Reference Fernández-García and Hernández-Tezoquipa 42 ). Smoking status was assessed using the categorization proposed by the WHO: current, past and never( 43 ).

Demographic data

We categorized marital status as never married, married, divorced or widowed. Educational attainment was categorized into elementary and secondary education, high school education, and bachelor’s degree or higher.

Statistical analysis

Factor analysis at baseline assessment (2004–2006)

In the baseline evaluation the MHQ scale was constructed with binary response items, obtaining information about structured mealtimes without taking snacks into consideration. Participants were asked: ‘When you eat, generally, what do you do?’, indicating their mealtime habits with either ‘yes’ or ‘no’. Then, an exploratory factor analysis was performed to uncover underlying factors and factor loadings of eight binary items. The factor solution of mealtime habit items was composed of only one factor, with an explained variance of 26·6 %; advisable mealtime habits were correlated positively and unadvisable mealtime habits were correlated negatively. The MHQ scale was constructed by summing the contribution of each item weighted by its factor loading. Since the indicator had negative values, the number 5 was added to the total summarized score to obtain a positive scale. Each participant received an individual score representing the quality of his/her mealtime habits, with higher scores corresponding to better quality. The MHQ scale had a median of 6·2 points, with a range of 1·3 to 8·7 (sd 1·3); internal consistency with Cronbach’s α test was 0·84( Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 ) (Table 1).

Factor analysis at follow-up assessment (2010–2012)

In the follow-up assessment, the MHQ scale was constructed with multiple-choice responses. Thus an exploratory factor analysis for polytomous responses was performed on ten items using the principal component method. The factor solution was composed of only one factor with an explained variance of 26·7 %. Eight items had a factor loading of greater than 0·30, and advisable mealtime habits were correlated positively and unadvisable mealtime habits were correlated negatively. Just as in the baseline assessment of the MHQ, the scale had negative values, so the number 5 was added to the total summarized score in order to obtain a positive scale. The mealtime habits score in this assessment had a median of 3·7 points, ranging from −0·54 to 6·71 (sd 1·19); internal consistency with Cronbach’s α test was 0·60 (Table 1).

Data analyses

Differences in sociodemographic, lifestyle and anthropometric characteristics were examined at baseline and follow-up, testing differences between two means using the t test for paired samples and assessing differences between two proportions using the McNemar test (Table 2).

Table 2 Demographic, anthropometric and lifestyle characteristics of the Mexican adult participants (n 837; men 24·9 % and women 75·1 %) at baseline and follow-up evaluations; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

*P value calculated using the McNemar test for differences in two proportions, and the t test for paired samples for differences in two means, between baseline and follow-up assessments. Proportions and means were adjusted for age and sex. P≤0·05 was considered to be significant.

Normal weight, BMI=18·5–24·9 kg/m2; overweight, BMI=25·0–29·9 kg/m2; obesity, BMI≥30·0 kg /m2.

Abdominal obesity was defined as waist circumference ≥88 cm for women or ≥102 cm for men.

§ Depression was defined by the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), twenty-item version: depressed participants defined by CES-D score ≥16. This criterion has been previously used to define depressive symptoms in the adult Mexican population( Reference Gallegos-Carrillo, Flores and Denova-Gutiérrez 22 ).

The MHQ scale was constructed by summing the factor loadings of each mealtime habits item and participants were assigned an individual MHQ score at baseline and another at the follow-up assessment. Next, panel data were created for obtaining a personal-level data set, in order to control for unobserved heterogeneity( Reference Baltagi 44 ), and individuals were classified into tertiles (the highest tertile indicates a more advisable MHQ), with one MHQ status for baseline and another for follow-up, so we could relate MHQ status with each participant’s corresponding weight gain and obesity status at baseline or follow-up. Then, in order to demonstrate if there were any differences in sociodemographic, anthropometric, lifestyle characteristics and dietary patterns across the MHQ tertiles, Cochran’s Q tests were performed to assess the differences between three proportions for nominal variables across MHQ tertiles and the Fisher test was performed to determine any difference between the means of the continuous variables across the MHQ tertiles (Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3 Demographic, anthropometric and lifestyle characteristics of the Mexican adult participantsFootnote * across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score at baseline (2004–2006) and follow-up (2010–2012) evaluations; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

* For the longitudinal analysis, repeated measures were included in the panel data. Thus, the number of participants was 837, while the number of observations was 1674.

MHQ score was created by summing the factor loadings of each mealtime habits item. Each participant received an individual MHQ score at baseline and another at the follow-up assessment. The MHQ categories were defined as: lower MHQ category (score=0 to 4·27 points); middle MHQ category (score=4·28 to 5·70 points); and higher MHQ category (score=5·71 to 8·08 points).

P value calculated using Cochran’s Q test to assess differences in three proportions, and Fisher’s test from linear regression to assess differences in three means, among low-, middle- and high-quality mealtime habits. Proportions and means were adjusted for age and sex. P≤0·05 was considered as significant.

§ Normal weight, BMI=18·5–24·9 kg/m2; overweight, BMI=25·0–29·9 kg/m2; obesity, BMI≥30·0 kg/m2.

|| Abdominal obesity was defined as waist circumference ≥88 cm for women or ≥102 cm for men.

To determine which participants had depressive symptoms at baseline and follow-up, a twenty-item version of a self-administered questionnaire created by the Center for Epidemiologic Studies (CES-D) was used, which has been validated in Spanish-speaking groups and in this study population( Reference Gallegos-Carrillo, Flores and Denova-Gutiérrez 22 ).

Table 4 Dietary patterns adherence of the Mexican adult participantsFootnote * across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score at baseline (2004–2006) and follow-up (2010–2012) evaluations; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

* For the longitudinal analysis, repeated measures were included in the panel data. Thus, the number of participants was 770, while the number of observations was 1540.

MHQ score was created by summing the factor loadings of each mealtime habits item. Each participant received an individual MHQ score at baseline and another at the follow-up assessment. The MHQ categories were defined as: lower MHQ category (score=0 to 4·27 points); middle MHQ category (score=4·28 to 5·70 points); and higher MHQ category (score=5·71 to 8·08 points).

P value calculated using Cochran’s Q test to assess differences in three proportions, and Fisher’s test from linear regression to assess differences in three means, among low-, middle- and high-quality mealtime habits. Proportions and means were adjusted for age and sex. P ≤0·05 was considered as significant.

Dietary patterns: prudent dietary pattern is typified by greater intakes of processed vegetable juices, potatoes, fresh fruits, fresh vegetables and legumes, and a lower intake of pastries; Western dietary pattern is typified by higher intakes of pastries, refined cereals, corn tortillas and soft drinks, and lower intakes of whole cereals, seafood and full-fat dairy products; and high protein/fat dietary pattern is typified by greater intakes of red meat, processed meat, margarine and eggs, and lower intakes of fruits and wholegrain cereals. Tertile 3 (T3) of each pattern represents greater adherence to the dietary pattern.

Then we analysed the three events of interest; some participants were eliminated from the analysis: (i) to estimate the risk of gaining ≥5 % of body weight, 130 participants who lost ≥5 % of their body weight were excluded, leaving 707 participants; (ii) to estimate the risk of becoming overweight/obese, only those participants with a normal BMI at baseline were included (n 358); and (iii) to evaluate the risk for becoming abdominally obese, only those with normal waist circumference (<88 cm for women and <102 cm for men) at baseline were considered (n 470). To estimate the longitudinal effect of the MHQ on anthropometric outcomes (gaining ≥5 % of body weight, becoming overweight/obese and developing abdominal obesity), Cochran’s Q tests were performed to assess changes in proportions of anthropometric indicators according to MHQ categories at baseline and after 7 years of follow-up (Table 5).

Table 5 Change in anthropometric indicators of the Mexican adult participantsFootnote * across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score after 7 years of follow-up; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

* For the longitudinal analysis, repeated measures were included in the panel data. Thus, the number of participants was 707, while the number of observations was 1414.

MHQ score was created by summing the factor loadings of each mealtime habits item. Each participant received an individual MHQ score at baseline and another at the follow-up assessment. The MHQ categories were defined as: lower MHQ category (score=0 to 4·27 points); middle MHQ category (score=4·28 to 5·70 points); and higher MHQ category (score=5·71 to 8·08 points).

P values were calculated using Cochran’s Q test to assess the differences in three proportions adjusted for sex and age.

§ Gained body weight was defined as an increase of ≥5 % in body weight after follow-up period. For this analysis, 130 participants who lost ≥5 % of their body weight were excluded.

|| Became overweight/obese was defined as having a normal BMI (18·5–24·9 kg/m2) at baseline, but having an overweight/obese BMI (≥25·0 kg/m2) after the follow-up period. Only participants with normal BMI at baseline were included in this analysis.

Developed abdominal obesity was defined as having a normal waist circumference at baseline, but having a waist circumference of ≥88 cm for women or ≥102 cm for men after the follow-up period. Only participants with normal waist circumferences at baseline were included in this analysis.

For both stages, generalized linear models with a log-link and binomial distribution were performed in order to obtain adjusted relative risks of weight gain, becoming overweight/obese and developing abdominal obesity according to the MHQ categories (Table 6)( Reference Twisk 45 ). We used generalized linear models because it is a more accurate method when the outcome event is common (incidence of 10 % or more)( Reference Greenland 46 , Reference McNutt, Wu and Xue 47 ). Analyses were performed using the Stata statistical software package version 12.0.

Table 6 Relative risks (RR) and 95 % confidence intervalsFootnote * of change in anthropometric indicators in the Mexican adult participantsFootnote across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score after 7 years of follow-up; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

* RR and their 95 % CI were calculated by means of generalized linear models, which were adjusted for sex, dietary patterns, depression, recreational physical activity and level education as categorical variables, and age and energy intake as continuous variables.

For the longitudinal analysis, repeated measures were included in the panel data. Thus the number of participants was 534, while the number of observations of 1068.

MHQ score was created by summing the factor loadings of each mealtime habits item. Each participant received an individual MHQ-score at baseline and another at the follow-up assessment. The MHQ categories were defined as: lower MHQ category (score=0 to 4·27 points); middle MHQ category (score=4·28 to 5·70 points); and higher MHQ category (score=5·71 to 8·08 points).

§ Gaining weight was defined as an increase of ≥5 % in body weight after the follow-up period. For this analysis 130 participants who lost ≥5 % of body weight were excluded.

|| Becoming overweight/obese was defined as having a normal BMI (18·5–24·9 kg/m2) at baseline, but having an overweight/obese BMI (≥25·0 kg/m2) after the follow-up period. Only participants with normal BMI at baseline were included in this analysis.

Developing abdominal obesity was defined as having normal waist circumference at baseline, but having a waist circumference of ≥88 cm in women and of ≥102 cm in men after the follow-up period. Only participants with normal waist circumferences at baseline were included in this analysis.

Results

The study population was composed of mainly middle-aged participants (44·5 years old at baseline and 52·6 years at follow-up); most participants were women (75 %), married or living in free union (64·8 % at baseline and 60·6 % in the follow-up) and had a bachelor’s degree or higher (48·2 % at baseline and 48·7 % in follow-up assessment). Some the participants’ other lifestyle and anthropometric characteristics changed after the study period: MHQ scores decreased from 5·3 to 4·8 points; the percentage of overweight and obesity increased from 57·2 to 64·6 %; abdominal obesity increased from 21·3 to 31·5 % in men and from 50·0 to 63·5 % in women; the percentage of recreational physically active participants decreased from 37·1 to 31·6 %; and the percentage of current smokers decreased from 15·8 to 11·3 %. The prevalence of participants who were categorized as depressed was similar at both times of assessment (21·0 and 20·0 %, respectively; Table 2).

Based on the baseline and follow-up MHQ scores a panel data set was obtained, then individuals were classified into tertiles in order to relate their MHQ status to their corresponding obesity status and weight gain from baseline to follow-up. Table 3 reports the relationship between sociodemographic, anthropometric and lifestyle characteristics and the MHQ categories, considering both assessments in the longitudinal panel data. Participants in the higher MHQ category were older than those in the lower MHQ group (mean age: 47·5 v. 46·0 years, respectively; P=0·000) and a greater proportion of participants with a high school level education were in the lower MHQ category (26·2 %) than in the higher MHQ group (19·6 %; P=0·040). Participants in the lower MHQ category had a greater body weight (66·3 v. 61·1 kg, respectively; P=0·000), a higher proportion of overweight (44·9 v. 37·4 %, respectively; P=0·010) and obesity (24·5 v. 12·8 %, respectively; P=0·000) and a greater percentage of abdominal obesity (54·7 v. 40·6 %, respectively; P=0·000) than those in the higher MHQ group. Regarding lifestyle characteristics, participants in the higher MHQ category were more likely to be physically active (37·7 v. 26·7 %, respectively; P=0·000) and had a lower prevalence of depression (16·8 v. 26·3 %, respectively; P=0·000) than participants classified in the lower MHQ group.

Table 4 presents the participants’ adherence to each of the three dietary patterns and their relationship with the MHQ categories, considering both assessments in the longitudinal panel data. A higher percentage of participants with greater adherence to the prudent dietary pattern were classified into the higher MHQ category than in the lower MHQ (39·2 v. 27·1 %, respectively; P=0·002). A greater proportion of participants who followed the Western dietary pattern were grouped into the lower MHQ than the higher MHQ category (38·9 v. 26·7 %, respectively; P=0·000). Finally, more participants with lower adherence to the high protein/fat dietary pattern were classified into the higher MHQ than the lower MHQ category (41·1 v. 27·0 %, respectively; P=0·000).

Table 5 shows that individuals classified in the lower MHQ category developed significantly worse anthropometric indicators over time, compared with those in the higher MHQ category. A significantly higher percentage of participants in the lower MHQ group gained ≥5 % of weight (34·2 v. 5·6 %, respectively), became overweight/obese (24·6 v. 2·8 %, respectively) or developed abdominal obesity (25·0 v. 3·9 %, respectively) than in the higher MHQ group (P=0·000 for all). These trends regarding gaining weight and becoming overweight/obese were similar for men and women; however, the percentage of men who developed abdominal obesity over time was not significantly different across MHQ categories.

Table 6 indicates the likelihood of gaining body weight, becoming overweight/obese or developing abdominal obesity across the three MHQ categories, after 7 years of follow-up. Participants in the middle and lower MHQ groups (compared with the higher MHQ category) showed a 4·1 and 6·2 times greater risk of gaining ≥5 % of body weight, respectively (both P=0·000). Participants in the middle and lower MHQ groups also had an increased risk of being overweight/obese of 6·6- and 8·6-fold, respectively (both P=0·000), as well as a 3·8- and 5·3-fold higher risk of developing abdominal obesity, respectively (both P=0·000). Among women, the association between MHQ and the anthropometric indicators we evaluated was similar to what we observed with the total study population (P=0·000 in all cases). Men who were categorized in the middle and lower MHQ had a 7·5- and 12·6-fold greater risk of gaining weight than those in the higher MHQ category (P=0·007 and P=0·000, respectively) and men in the lower MHQ presented a 5·0-fold greater risk of developing abdominal obesity than those in the higher MHQ category (P=0·027).

Discussion

The present prospective study provides evidence about the influence of a set of habitual mealtime behaviours on anthropometric indicators. Our findings support the hypothesis that participants who engage in unadvisable mealtime habits have an increased risk of developing an unhealthy body weight and anthropometric profile. Considering both assessments, individuals in the middle and low MHQ categories had a significantly higher likelihood of gaining weight, becoming overweight/obese and developing abdominal obesity after 7 years of follow-up than those in the high MHQ category.

The meal is the focus of nutrition research because most food is consumed as part of a meal, making it an appropriate context for addressing concerns about food intake( Reference Meiselman 48 ). Our findings also support the hypothesis that usual mealtime habits can influence food intake and anthropometric measures, which is key to understanding how some mealtime circumstances may lead to high energy intake and consequently the development of unhealthy anthropometric indicators. In the present study, MHQ categories were associated with three dietary patterns: the healthiest MHQ category was linked to a prudent diet, while the unhealthiest MHQ category was related to a Western diet. A previous study of 7472 Mexican adults also showed that participants who were in the healthiest MHQ category reported a greater adherence to a prudent dietary pattern than to a Western one( Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 ).

In our study we analysed prospectively four mealtime items that are related to the availability of time to eat: taking time to eat, rushing meals, skipping meals and eating in large mouthfuls. Previous studies have shown that scarcity of time when eating might lead to rushing meals, eating out instead of at home and skipping meals, which are all linked to an increased consumption of fast foods and fewer home cooked meals( Reference Larson, Nelson and Neumark-Sztainer 13 , Reference Larson, Perry and Story 49 ). Skipping meals and rushing meals can lead to decreased feelings of satiety at meal completion and are both linked to being overweight( Reference Andrade, Green and Melanson 50 Reference Kimura, Nanri and Matsushita 52 ). Rushing meals has been associated with higher intakes of soft drinks, fast foods and fat, a lower intake of healthy foods( Reference Larson, Nelson and Neumark-Sztainer 13 ) and a greater energy intake( Reference Maruyama, Sato and Ohira 51 ). Other studies report that eating quickly is positively associated with a higher BMI, independent of total energy intake( Reference Maruyama, Sato and Ohira 51 , Reference Kamath and D’Souza 53 ). Their findings suggest that energy intake is lower when a meal is eaten slowly, and satiety is higher at meal completion( Reference Andrade, Green and Melanson 50 ).

We also examined activities that distract from eating, with the following items: distracted watching TV or reading, and distracted taking advantage of mealtime to accomplish work activities. Watching TV impairs one’s ability to keep track of food consumption, which may lead to higher energy and fat intakes, thus increasing the likelihood of becoming overweight( Reference Moray, Fu and Brill 9 , Reference Hetherington, Anderson and Norton 10 ). Watching TV is also associated with lower fruit and vegetable consumption( Reference Cleland, Schmidt and Dwyer 8 , Reference Moray, Fu and Brill 9 ). There is evidence that TV watching can impact subsequent meals by reducing memories of the last meal consumed, increasing food intake in the next meal and increasing the likelihood of consuming snacks( Reference Robinson, Aveyard and Daley 4 ). Habitual TV viewing can also influence body weight simply by increasing inactivity, decreasing metabolic rate and exposing the viewer to appetizing commercials. Sitting in front of the TV leads to transient and exaggerated elevations in blood glucose, fatty acids and TAG( Reference Thorp, McNaughton and Owen 5 , Reference Cleland, Schmidt and Dwyer 8 ). In fact, TV viewing and eating snacks have been jointly associated with the metabolic syndrome and its components by means of ‘mindless eating’, causing people to ignore feelings of fullness( Reference Thorp, McNaughton and Owen 5 ). Many other distractors from eating have also been related to increased energy intake( Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Slama 2 ), including listening to a detective story( Reference Bellisle and Dalix 54 ), listening to music( Reference Stroebele and De Castro 55 ), playing video games( Reference Ogden, Coop and Cousins 3 ) and engaging in other tasks( Reference Boon, Stroebe and Schut 56 ).

Environmental factors at mealtime and their influence on energy intake and anthropometric status were also investigated in the present research, through the following items: eating with friends, family or colleagues, and number of times per week that participants ate their main meal at home. Previous research shows that environmental factors such as atmospherics, social interactions and distractions are associated with food intake( Reference Wansink 1 , Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Slama 2 , Reference Hetherington, Anderson and Norton 10 , Reference Boitelle, Birnbaum and Lytle 57 , Reference Wansink and Van Kleef 58 ). The potential stimulatory influence of different environmental factors can make people more vulnerable to hunger( Reference Wansink 1 ). Eating with others has been shown to increase energy intake in some contexts. For example, the sensory stimulation from food and the number of persons present at the meal can critically influence meal size, regardless of hunger( Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Slama 2 ); however, eating with family members has some potential benefits, because families frequently adopt healthy eating habits( Reference Wansink and Van Kleef 58 , Reference Videon and Manning 59 ). Other studies have shown that meals eaten with other people tend to be larger and longer in duration compared with meals eaten alone, regardless of the relationship with the eating companion( Reference Hetherington, Anderson and Norton 10 ). These results were consistently independent of the time of day, with similar effects observed in morning, noontime and evening meals( Reference De Castro 20 ). An experimental study was conducted with two groups of forty healthy adult women (divided by level of dietary restraint), who participated in five different meal situations: eating alone, in a group, listening to a detective story, watching TV and watching advertisements on TV. Women with high levels of dietary restraint were more sensitive to external distractions present at mealtime and, contrary to expected, they ate less in groups than alone( Reference Bellisle, Dalix and Airinei 19 ).

Eating is an important part of culture, tradition and personal history; it is also a powerful expression of social identity( Reference Anderson 60 ). Familial and cultural eating habits were also explored in the current study through the following items: eating with family, friends or colleagues, eating at home, and eating everything on the plate. This last item is an unwritten social rule in many countries that generally shows appreciation for the food and sends a non-verbal message that the food was delicious. However, this custom has become an unhealthy habit because of the enormous serving sizes at restaurants( Reference Anderson 60 , Reference Ouwehand and De Ridder 61 ). In the familial context, large portions may contribute to overeating since relatives sometimes encourage children to finish their meal even if they are full, because it is interpreted as a sign of well-adjusted and healthy eating. Family rituals also dictate with whom, where and how long one eats, which seems to be the greatest driving factor of both adult and child BMI( Reference Anderson 60 ). A study that examined the relationship between BMI and family dinner found that families who frequently ate dinner in the kitchen or in the dining room and remained at the table until everyone was finished eating had lower BMI for both parents and children, as compared with families who ate elsewhere( Reference Videon and Manning 59 ).

As part of our study, we also assessed food choices and the joy of eating with the following three items: choosing the type of food one eats thinking of one’s health, choosing the amount of food one eats and enjoyment of eating. Food selection is an important intermediary factor to understand why people become overweight. What people choose to eat is influenced by individual, cultural and social values that can further empower consumers to make more conscious and informed choices( Reference Smith and Hawks 26 ). People generally claim that taste is the most important factor in choosing what to eat, and foods high in sugar and fat are highly enjoyable and usually the most appealing to taste preferences, which triggers a desire to eat( Reference Ouwehand and De Ridder 61 ). Contrarily, intuitive eating is highly related with balanced food intake and involves an ability to identify hunger sensations and satiety, allowing individuals to choose foods that are personally satisfying without any sense of deprivation. Intuitive eating has been associated with fewer food anxieties and less dieting behaviours, with greater pleasure associated with food, lower BMI, no negative aspect of diet composition, diet diversification and with eating breakfast( Reference Smith and Hawks 26 ).

The present study has some weaknesses that should be addressed. We realize that the MHQ scale may be unable to capture all possible variations of mealtime behaviours, such as which meal of the day it is, the day of the week, specific eating contexts or the people present at particular meals. The items used in the study tended to generalize people’s daily life experiences regarding meals, since the meal is the most important source of food intake( Reference Meiselman 48 ), and we sought to reflect a generally accurate picture of mealtime habits by identifying what people usually did at mealtimes. In addition, the MHQ scale was composed of binary response items at the baseline assessment that were changed to a multiple-choice response format in the follow-up assessment, which allowed us to explore a wider range of mealtime behaviours. An important strength of the study is that the MHQ scale is a comprehensive, reliable and valid measure due to its structure and ability to relate mealtime behaviours with dietary patterns and body weight status. Another major strength of the study is its longitudinal design, which can reveal changes in the MHQ score and in anthropometric measures more accurately than a cross-sectional design. This subject is relevant, due to anthropometric markers of obesity having been shown to significantly predispose people to metabolic diseases( 35 ), arterial stiffness, subclinical atherosclerosis( Reference Recio-Rodriguez, Gomez-Marcos and Patino-Alonso 62 ) and all-cause general mortality( Reference Boggs, Rosenberg and Cozier 63 , Reference Berrington de Gonzalez, Hartge and Cerhan 64 ). Finally, our findings are consistent with other studies in the Mexican population: a cross-sectional study with 353 Mexican adolescents found that regular or better mealtime habits were associated with a 69 and 72 % reduced risk of being overweight or obese, respectively( Reference Pierlot 65 ). Another cross-sectional study of Mexican health workers reported that poor mealtime habits were related to unhealthy dietary patterns and a higher BMI( Reference Dosamantes-Carrasco, Méndez-Hernández and Denova-Gutiérrez 21 ).

The MHQ scale is a measure that brings together a set of different mealtime behaviours that people engage in simultaneously and attempts to represent a balance of unhealthy and healthy mealtime behaviours. Thus, the MHQ scale could be a useful population tool to predict long-term weight change because the meal is one of the most important sources of food intake and consequently weight status. Moreover, the associations found through the MHQ scale should be able to be replicated in other populations (such as children, adolescents and the elderly) to confirm the measure’s ability to predict risk of developing certain metabolic disorders. This scale may also be useful for assessing the impact of health promotion interventions focused on mealtime habits, demonstrating how health education interventions that target mealtime behaviours can improve diet quality and reduce risk of weight gain.

Acknowledgements

Financial support: This study was funded by the Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CONACyT; National Science and Technology Council) (grant numbers 7876 (year 2002) and 87783 (year 2008)) and supported by the HWCS. Y.N.F was supported by the National Cancer Institute (NCI)/National Institutes of Health (NIH) (grant number K07CA197179) for her work on this study. CONACyT, HWCS and NCI/NIH had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: The authors declare that they have no competing interests. Authorship: L.D.D.-C. and P.M.-H. were involved in the design of the study, statistical analysis and drafting the manuscript. C.S., B.R.-P. and K.G.-C. were involved in drafting the manuscript. E.D.-G., P.R. and E.S.-M. assisted with the statistical analysis and were involved in drafting the manuscript. Y.N.F. and J.S. contributed to the study design, coordination and to the original conception and design of the HWCS, and have given final approval of the manuscript to be published. All authors made critical comments during the preparation of the manuscript and fully accept responsibility for the work. Ethics of human subject participation: This research was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social (Mexican Social Security Institute) from the state of Morelos, Mexico. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980016002184

References

1. Wansink, B (2004) Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu Rev Nutr 24, 455579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Bellisle, F, Dalix, AM & Slama, G (2004) Non food-related environmental stimuli induce increased meal intake in healthy women: comparison of television viewing versus listening to a recorded story in laboratory settings. Appetite 43, 175180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Ogden, J, Coop, N, Cousins, C et al. (2013) Distraction, the desire to eat and food intake. Towards an expanded model of mindless eating. Appetite 62, 119126.Google Scholar
4. Robinson, E, Aveyard, P, Daley, A et al. (2013) Eating attentively: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of food intake memory and awareness on eating. Am J Clin Nutr 97, 728742.Google Scholar
5. Thorp, AA, McNaughton, SA, Owen, N et al. (2013) Independent and joint associations of TV viewing time and snack food consumption with the metabolic syndrome and its components; a cross-sectional study in Australian adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 10, 96.Google Scholar
6. Long, S, Meyer, C, Leung, N et al. (2011) Effects of distraction and focused attention on actual and perceived food intake in females with non-clinical eating psychopathology. Appetite 56, 350356.Google Scholar
7. Higgs, S & Woodward, M (2009) Television watching during lunch increases afternoon snack intake of young women. Appetite 52, 3943.Google Scholar
8. Cleland, VJ, Schmidt, MD, Dwyer, T et al. (2008) Television viewing and abdominal obesity in young adults: is the association mediated by food and beverage consumption during viewing time or reduced leisure-time physical activity? Am J Clin Nutr 87, 11481155.Google Scholar
9. Moray, J, Fu, A, Brill, K et al. (2007) Viewing television while eating impairs the ability to accurately estimate total amount of food consumed. Bariatr Surg Pract Patient Care 2, 7176.Google Scholar
10. Hetherington, MM, Anderson, AS, Norton, GN et al. (2006) Situational effects on meal intake: a comparison of eating alone and eating with others. Physiol Behav 88, 498505.Google Scholar
11. Martínez-Díaz, DJ (2009) Valores y creencias en el consumo de comidas en los hogares barraquilleros. Rev Cient Pensamiento Gestión issue 27; available at http://rcientificas.uninorte.edu.co/index.php/pensamiento/article/view/835/4954 Google Scholar
12. Raulio, S, Roos, E, Mukala, K et al. (2008) Can working conditions explain differences in eating patterns during working hours? Public Health Nutr 11, 258270.Google Scholar
13. Larson, NI, Nelson, MC, Neumark-Sztainer, D et al. (2009) Making time for meals: meal structure and associations with dietary intake in young adults. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 7279.Google Scholar
14. Smith, KJ, Gall, SL, McNaughton, SA et al. (2010) Skipping breakfast: longitudinal associations with cardiometabolic risk factors in the Childhood Determinants of Adult Health Study. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 13161325.Google Scholar
15. Timlin, MT & Pereira, MA (2007) Breakfast frequency and quality in the etiology of adult obesity and chronic diseases. Nutr Rev 65, 268281.Google Scholar
16. Lachat, C, Nago, E, Verstraeten, R et al. (2012) Eating out of home and its association with dietary intake: a systematic review of the evidence. Obes Rev 13, 329346.Google Scholar
17. Crawford, D, Ball, K, Mishra, G et al. (2007) Which food-related behaviors are associated with healthier intakes of fruits and vegetables among women? Public Health Nutr 10, 256265.Google Scholar
18. Kruger, J, Blanck, HM & Gillespie, C (2008) Dietary practices, dining out behavior and physical activity correlates of weight loss maintenance. Prev Chronic Dis 5, A11.Google Scholar
19. Bellisle, F, Dalix, AM, Airinei, G et al. (2009) Influence of dietary restraint and environmental factors on meal size in normal-weight women. A laboratory study. Appetite 53, 309313.Google Scholar
20. De Castro, JM (1994) Family and friends produce greater social facilitation of food intake than other companions. Physiol Behav 56, 445445.Google Scholar
21. Dosamantes-Carrasco, D, Méndez-Hernández, P, Denova-Gutiérrez, E et al. (2011) Scale for assessing the quality of Mexican adults’ mealtime habits. Salud Publica Mex 53, 152159.Google Scholar
22. Gallegos-Carrillo, K, Flores, YN, Denova-Gutiérrez, E et al. (2013) Physical activity and reduced risk of depression: results of a longitudinal study of Mexican adults. Health Psychol 32, 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences, World Health Organization (2002) International Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects. Geneva: CIOMS.Google Scholar
24. Spector, P (1992) Summated Rating Scale Construction: An Introduction. Sage University Paper Series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, no. 07-082. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
25. Goon, S & Islam, S (2014) Breakfast skipping and obesity risk among urban adults in Bangladesh. Int J Public Health Sci 3, 1522.Google Scholar
26. Smith, T & Hawks, SR (2006) Intuitive eating, diet composition, and the meaning of food in healthy weight promotion. Am J Health Educ 37, 130136.Google Scholar
27. DeVellis, RF (2003) On building Likert scales. In Scale Development: Theory and Applications, pp. 37. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
28. Mendez-Hernandez, P, Dosamantes-Carrasco, D, Siani, C et al. (2012) A workplace physical activity program at a public university in Mexico can reduce medical costs associated with type 2 diabetes and hypertension. Salud Publica Mex 54, 2027.Google Scholar
29. Méndez-Hernández, P, Siani, C, Lamure, M et al. (2008) Préférences déclarées sur trois programs d’activité physique pour empêcher les maladies chroniques au Mexique. Proceedings of 9ème Conférence internationale sur la science des systèmes de santé. Nouvelles technologies de l’information et gouvernance des systèmes de santé, Lyon, France, 3–5 September, p. 188.Google Scholar
30. Hernández-Avila, M, Romieu, I, Parra, S et al. (1998) Validity and reproducibility of a food frequency questionnaire to assess dietary intake of women living in Mexico City. Salud Publica Mex 40, 133140.Google Scholar
31. Denova-Gutiérrez, E, Castañón, S, Talavera, JO et al. (2010) Dietary patterns are associated with metabolic syndrome in an urban Mexican population. J Nutr 140, 18551863.Google Scholar
32. National Cholesterol Education Program, National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, National Institutes of Health (2001) Third Report of the National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation and Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol in Adults (Adult Treatment Panel III); Executive Summary. NIH Publication no. 01-3670. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
33. World Health Organization (1995) Physical Status: The Use and Interpretation of Anthropometry. Report of a WHO Expert Committee. WHO Technical Report Series no. 854. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
34. Colditz, GA, Willett, WC, Rotnitzky, A et al. (1995) Weight gain as a risk factor for clinical diabetes mellitus in women. Ann Intern Med 122, 481486.Google Scholar
35. World Health Organization (2003) Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Report of a Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation. WHO Technical Report Series no. 916. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
36. Katzmarzyk, PT, Mire, E, Bray, GA et al. (2013) Anthropometric markers of obesity and mortality in white and African American adults: the Pennington center longitudinal study. Obesity (Silver Spring) 21, 10701075.Google Scholar
37. Maru, S, Van Der Schouw, YT, Gimbrère, CHF et al. (2004) Body mass index and short-term weight change in relation to mortality in Dutch women after age 50 y. Am J Clin Nutr 80, 231236.Google Scholar
38. Radloff, LS (1977) The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39. Wolf, AM, Hunter, DJ, Colditz, GA et al. (1994) Reproducibility and validity of a self-administered physical activity questionnaire. Int J Epidemiol 23, 991999.Google Scholar
40. Martínez-González, MA, López-Fontana, C, Varo, JJ et al. (2005) Validation of the Spanish version of the physical activity questionnaire used in the Nurses’ Health Study and the Health Professionals’ Follow-up Study. Public Health Nutr 8, 920927.Google Scholar
41. Méndez-Hernández, P, Flores, Y, Siani, C et al. (2009) Physical activity and risk of metabolic syndrome in an urban Mexican cohort. BMC Public Health 9, 276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
42. Fernández-García, V & Hernández-Tezoquipa, I (2007) Promoción de la actividad Física. Cuernavaca, México: Instituto Nacional de Salud Pública, Secretaría de Salud.Google Scholar
43. World Health Organization (1997) Tobacco or Health: A Global Status Report. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
44. Baltagi, BH (2004) Panel Data Theory and Applications. Series: Studies in Empirical Economics. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag GmbH & Co. KG.Google Scholar
45. Twisk, JW (2003) Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis. A Practical Guide. Cambrige, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
46. Greenland, S (2004) Model-based estimation of relative risks and other epidemiologic measures in studies of common outcomes and in case–control studies. Am J Epidemiol 160, 301305.Google Scholar
47. McNutt, LA, Wu, C & Xue, X (2003) Estimating the relative risk in cohort studies and clinical trials of common outcomes. Am J Epidemiol 157, 940943.Google Scholar
48. Meiselman, HL (2009) Meals in Science and Practice: Interdisciplinary Research and Business Applications. Woodhead Publishing Series in Food Science, Technology and Nutrition, no. 171. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press LLC.Google Scholar
49. Larson, NI, Perry, CL, Story, M et al. (2006) Food preparation by young adults is associated with better diet quality. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 20012007.Google Scholar
50. Andrade, AM, Green, GW & Melanson, KJ (2008) Eating slowly led to decreases in energy intake within meals in healthy women. J Am Diet Assoc 108, 11861191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Maruyama, K, Sato, S, Ohira, T et al. (2008) The joint impact on being overweight of self reported behaviours of eating quickly and eating until full: cross sectional survey. BMJ 337, a2002.Google Scholar
52. Kimura, Y, Nanri, A, Matsushita, Y et al. (2011) Eating behavior in relation to prevalence of overweight among Japanese men. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 20, 2934.Google ScholarPubMed
53. Kamath, S & D’Souza, J (2013) Prevalence of obesity among the medical students: a cross sectional study in a south Indian medical college. Al Ameen J Med Sci 6, 9395.Google Scholar
54. Bellisle, F & Dalix, A (2001) Cognitive restraint can be offset by distraction, leading to increased meal intake in women. Am J Clin Nutr 74, 197200.Google Scholar
55. Stroebele, N & De Castro, JM (2006) Listening to music while eating is related to increases in people’s food intake and meal duration. Appetite 47, 285289.Google Scholar
56. Boon, B, Stroebe, W, Schut, H et al. (2002) Ironic processes in the eating behaviour of restrained eaters. Br J Health Psychol 7, 110.Google Scholar
57. Boitelle, KN, Birnbaum, AS, Lytle, LA et al. (2003) Associations between perceived family meal environment and parent intake of fruit, vegetables, and fat. J Nutr Educ Behav 35, 2429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
58. Wansink, B & Van Kleef, E (2013) Dinner rituals that correlate with child and adult BMI. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, E91E95.Google Scholar
59. Videon, TM & Manning, CK (2003) Influences on adolescent eating patterns: the importance of family meals. J Adolesc Health 32, 365373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60. Anderson, EN (2014) Me, myself, and the others: food as a social marker Everyone Eats: Understanding Food and Culture, 2nd ed., pp. 171187. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
61. Ouwehand, C & De Ridder, DT (2008) Effects of temptation and weight on hedonics and motivation to eat in women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 16, 17881793.Google Scholar
62. Recio-Rodriguez, JI, Gomez-Marcos, MA, Patino-Alonso, MC et al. (2012) Abdominal obesity vs general obesity for identifying arterial stiffness, subclinical atherosclerosis and wave reflection in healthy, diabetics and hypertensive. BMC Cardiovasc Disord 12, 3.Google Scholar
63. Boggs, DA, Rosenberg, L, Cozier, YC et al. (2011) General and abdominal obesity and risk of death among black women. N Engl J Med 365, 901908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64. Berrington de Gonzalez, A, Hartge, P, Cerhan, JR et al. (2010) Body-mass index and mortality among 1.46 million white adults. N Engl J Med 363, 22112219.Google Scholar
65. Pierlot, R (2016) Estudio sobre la promoción de un estilo de vida saludable en jóvenes de bachillerato de Ixtenco, Tlaxcala, México (Research on the promotion of a healthy lifestyle in high schoolers from Ixtenco, Tlaxcala, México). PhD Thesis, Universidad Autónoma de Tlaxcala.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Factor loadings of the mealtime habit items, comparing binary responses at baseline and multiple-choice responses at follow-up assessment; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

Figure 1

Table 2 Demographic, anthropometric and lifestyle characteristics of the Mexican adult participants (n 837; men 24·9 % and women 75·1 %) at baseline and follow-up evaluations; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

Figure 2

Table 3 Demographic, anthropometric and lifestyle characteristics of the Mexican adult participants* across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score at baseline (2004–2006) and follow-up (2010–2012) evaluations; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

Figure 3

Table 4 Dietary patterns adherence of the Mexican adult participants* across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score at baseline (2004–2006) and follow-up (2010–2012) evaluations; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

Figure 4

Table 5 Change in anthropometric indicators of the Mexican adult participants* across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score after 7 years of follow-up; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

Figure 5

Table 6 Relative risks (RR) and 95 % confidence intervals* of change in anthropometric indicators in the Mexican adult participants† across categories of the Mealtime Habits Quality (MHQ) score after 7 years of follow-up; Health Workers Cohort Study, Morelos, Mexico

Supplementary material: File

Dosamantes-Carrasco supplementary material

Table S1

Download Dosamantes-Carrasco supplementary material(File)
File 17.3 KB