Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T03:56:14.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Breakfast frequency among adolescents: associations with measures of family functioning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2016

Trine Pagh Pedersen*
Affiliation:
National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark
Bjørn E Holstein
Affiliation:
National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark
Mogens Trab Damsgaard
Affiliation:
National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark
Mette Rasmussen
Affiliation:
National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark, Øster Farimagsgade 5, 1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark
*
*Corresponding author: Email tpp@bornungesorg.dk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To investigate (i) associations between adolescents’ frequency of breakfast and family functioning (close relations to parents, quality of family communication and family support) and (ii) if any observed associations between breakfast frequency and family functioning vary by sociodemographic factors.

Design

School-based cross-sectional study. Students completed a web-based questionnaire. Associations were estimated by multilevel multivariate logistic regression.

Setting

Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014.

Subjects

Adolescents aged 13 and 15 years (n 3054) from a random sample of forty-one schools.

Results

Nearly one-quarter of the adolescents had low breakfast frequency. Low breakfast frequency was associated with low family functioning measured by three dimensions. The OR (95 % CI) of low breakfast frequency was 1·81 (1·40, 2·33) for adolescents who reported no close relations to parents, 2·28 (1·61, 3·22) for adolescents who reported low level of quality of family communication and 2·09 (1·39, 3·15) for adolescents who reported low level of family support. Joint effect analyses suggested that the odds of low breakfast frequency among adolescents with low family functioning compared with high family functioning were highest among adolescents being girls, immigrants and living in other than a traditional family structure.

Conclusions

Low breakfast frequency was associated with low family functioning measured by close relations to parents, quality of family communication and family support. Further, analyses suggested that the associations were more pronounced among girls, immigrants and adolescents from other family structure than traditional. The study highlights the importance of the family setting in promoting regular breakfast frequency among adolescents.

Type
Research Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2016 

A growing literature indicates that breakfast consumption is associated with several health outcomes among adolescents. Adolescents who eat breakfast often have a more favourable nutrient intake than adolescents with low breakfast frequency( Reference Nicklas, Baranowski and Cullen 1 , Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 2 ). Also, despite some inconsistency in findings( Reference Mesas, Munoz-Pareja and Lopez-Garcia 3 ), an association between low breakfast frequency and overweight among children and adolescents has been observed( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 2 , Reference Fabritius and Rasmussen 4 , Reference Szajewska and Ruszczynski 5 ). Low breakfast frequency in childhood and adulthood is associated with metabolic risk factors in adulthood such as higher BMI, higher mean fasting insulin and higher LDL cholesterol concentrations( Reference Smith, Gall and McNaughton 6 , Reference Wennberg, Gustafsson and Wennberg 7 ). Additionally, there are indications in the literature that breakfast consumption is positively associated with children’s ability to concentrate in school( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 2 , Reference Hoyland, Dye and Lawton 8 , Reference Cooper, Bandelow and Nevill 9 ). Further, low breakfast frequency in adolescence predicts low breakfast frequency in late adolescence and young adulthood( Reference Pedersen, Holstein and Flachs 10 , Reference Merten, Williams and Shriver 11 ). In the existing literature the definition of breakfast consumption varies( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 2 , Reference Merten, Williams and Shriver 11 Reference Sjöberg, Hallberg and Höglund 15 ). Whereas others have used terms such as ‘breakfast pattern’, ‘skipping breakfast’ and ‘regularity of breakfast’, we apply the term ‘breakfast frequency’. This terminology directly reflects the applied breakfast measure in the present study.

The family is a significant setting for influencing the development of adolescents’ health behaviours( Reference Tinsley 16 ). In the family adolescents are influenced by their parents’ attitudes, beliefs and behaviours( Reference Tinsley 16 ). Breakfast is often consumed in the home and the family setting is therefore important when studying breakfast habits among adolescents. Others have highlighted the importance of investigating the influence of the family setting for adolescent breakfast frequency( Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely 13 , Reference Sleddens, Kroeze and Kohl 17 ). Breakfast frequency has been associated with the family setting characterized by physical factors such as food availability and food poverty in the home and sociodemographic factors such as socio-economic position( Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely 13 ). Further, sociocultural factors have been associated with breakfast frequency and especially parental breakfast consumption and family type have been investigated in several studies( Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely 13 ). A sociocultural factor that has had limited study is family functioning. Family functioning includes interpersonal interactions between parent and child such as problem solving, communication, roles, adaptability, warmth/closeness and behaviour control( Reference Kitzman-Ulrich, Wilson and George 18 , Reference Broderick 19 ). Low family functioning is associated with health outcomes and health behaviours such as overweight( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 , Reference Halliday, Palma and Mellor 21 ), sedentary behaviour( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ) and low intake of fruit and vegetables( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 , Reference Mellin, Neumark-Sztainer and Story 22 , Reference Neumark-Sztainer, Story and Resnick 23 ).

Franco et al. suggested that adolescents who experience a high level of family communication and support are more susceptible to parents’ advice about breakfast consumption( Reference Franko, Thompson and Bauserman 24 ). A recent study by Berge et al. found that high family functioning (communication, closeness, problem solving, behavioural control) among US adolescents (mean age of 14·4 years) was associated with daily breakfast consumption( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ). Young and Fors found among US 9th–12th graders that the ability to communicate with parents about serious issues was positively associated with daily healthy breakfast (healthy not defined)( Reference Young and Fors 25 ). Also for family cohesion, involving measures of emotional bonding and supportiveness, positive associations have been found with frequent breakfast consumption among 13–16-year-old New Zealanders( Reference Moore and Harre 26 ) and 9–19-year-old US adolescents( Reference Franko, Thompson and Bauserman 24 ). The existing studies have used different measures of family functioning and are based solely on populations of adolescents from the USA and New Zealand. When planning interventions it is important to get detailed insights into which aspects of family functioning are associated with breakfast frequency and whether the observed associations exist across settings.

The family setting is also characterized by sociodemographic factors and for several of these, associations with adolescent breakfast frequency have been observed. Low breakfast frequency is most common among girls and the prevalence increases with increasing age( Reference Sjöberg, Hallberg and Höglund 15 , Reference Pedersen, Meilstrup and Holstein 27 , Reference Currie, Zanotti and Morgan 28 ). Low socio-economic position( Reference Sjöberg, Hallberg and Höglund 15 , Reference Höglund, Samuelson and Mark 29 Reference Vereecken, Dupuy and Rasmussen 32 ), not living with two parents( Reference Vereecken, Dupuy and Rasmussen 32 Reference Levin, Kirby and Currie 35 ) and being an immigrant( Reference Delva, O’Malley and Johnston 36 Reference Jensen and Holstein 38 ) are associated with low breakfast frequency among adolescents.

A model that can be used to gain insight into causal mechanisms between for example the family setting and energy-related behaviour is Kremers’ Environmental Research framework for weight Gain prevention (EnRG) model( Reference Kremers, de Bruijn and Visscher 39 ). We consider breakfast frequency to be an example of weight-gaining behaviour. According to Kremers’ model, the causal background for energy balance-related behaviours is the physical and sociocultural characteristics of the settings in which people act. The model suggests that personal (e.g. sociodemographic factors) and behavioural characteristics act as modifying factors between the setting and behaviour( Reference Kremers, de Bruijn and Visscher 39 , Reference Kremers 40 ). Brug et al. highlighted that most studies have not examined the differences in environmental correlates for distinct subgroups as proposed by the Kremers’ model with regard to effect modifiers such as sociodemographic variables( Reference Brug, Kremers and Lenthe 41 ). Information on such modifying influences is important for developing efficient public health interventions. Based on this perspective, it can be hypothesized that sociodemographic factors such as high family social class and family structure buffer the influence of low family functioning on breakfast frequency. Information on such modifying or joint influences is important for designing well-targeted public health interventions. Berge et al. studied the modifying effect of ethnicity on the association between family functioning and breakfast consumption, finding no modifying effect( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ). Franko et al. studied the modifying effect of age on the association between family cohesion and frequency of breakfast consumption and found no modifying effect of age( Reference Franko, Thompson and Bauserman 24 ). Additional studies examining the modifying effect of other sociodemographic factors are still lacking.

Therefore, the aim of the current study was to gain more insight into which aspects of family functioning are associated with adolescent breakfast frequency by investigating the following research questions: (i) is there an association between family functioning (measured by close relations to parents, quality of family communication and family support) and breakfast frequency among adolescents? (ii) Are any observed associations modified by sociodemographic factors?

Methods

Study design and study population

We used Danish data from the international, cross-sectional, Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study( Reference Currie, Nic Gabhainn and Godeau 42 ). Data collection is conducted every fourth year in each participating country among students aged 11, 13 and 15 years (in Denmark, equivalent to 5th, 7th and 9th grade, respectively) in a random sample of schools (i.e. cluster sampling). Students completed the self-administered, internationally standardized and anonymous HBSC questionnaire at school( Reference Roberts, Freeman and Samdal 43 ). In 2014 we selected schools at random from a complete list of schools in Denmark. We used a regional-specific sampling to assure equal proportional representation of six geographical regions. We substituted every school that declined participation with another school chosen at random within the same region. In total we approached 168 schools of which forty-eight agreed to participate. In the majority of cases, the reason given for non-participation was that, at the time of recruitment, Danish schools were in the process of implementing a new and comprehensive school reform. Further, some schools had participated in similar surveys and did not have resources available for participation in yet another study. The school acceptance rate was higher among private than public schools (χ 2 test, P=0·0328) but did not relate to school location or school size. We do not suspect that this non-participation pattern resulted in important student-related selection bias. The participating schools comprised 5292 students in 248 classes at grade 5, 7 and 9. Of the students present on the day of data collection, 4534 students submitted a satisfactorily completed questionnaire (based on subjective identification of questionnaires not filled in seriously). The response rate was 85·7 % (4534/5292). We have no information on reasons for non-participation among students. In the Danish data collection items on family communication were mainly included in the questionnaire applied in grade 7 and 9. The sample for the present study therefore comprises 3054 students in 7th and 9th grade from 167 classes in forty-one schools. The mean age of the participants in grade 7 and 9 was 13·8 (sd 0·4) years and 15·8 (sd 0·4) years, respectively.

The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. In Denmark there is no formal ethics agency that grants ethical approval of school-based surveys. We received study approval from the school headmaster, the parents’ school board and the students’ council in each of the participating schools. Since the school headmaster and the parents’ school board approved the study, we did not ask the parents for approval of the study. The students received oral and written information that participation was voluntary and anonymous. The study has been approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency (reference number 2015-621-0030).

Measurements

Dependent variable: breakfast frequency

Breakfast frequency was measured by a frequency question for weekdays. We dichotomized the variable following conceptual considerations and defined low breakfast frequency as consuming breakfast on fewer than four out of five weekdays (Table 1). The definition of low breakfast frequency varies( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 2 , Reference Merten, Williams and Shriver 11 Reference Sjöberg, Hallberg and Höglund 15 ). According to the definition applied in the present study, breakfast can still be defined as frequent despite occasionally being skipped. Sensitivity analyses involving cut-off points defined by consuming breakfast on fewer than five, three, two and one weekday(s) were conducted. These showed no changes in the directions of associations. The breakfast frequency measure has been validated in a Danish study against 7 d, 24 h recall measures among 11–15-year-olds, demonstrating 87 % agreement and κ=0·65 for the dichotomized variable( Reference Pedersen, Holstein and Laursen 44 ). Also, the measure of breakfast frequency was included in a larger qualitative validation study of meal habits where face and content validity of the breakfast frequency item were tested among 11-, 13- and 15-year-old students. In total the first author conducted twenty gender-homogeneous focus group discussions at five schools with two to five students in each group. The objective was to learn about students’ perceptions and experiences of the measure immediately after they had answered the questionnaire. Further, we wished to understand how they perceived the concept of breakfast. We found high face validity as the students found it easy to answer the item. To clarify the content validity the students were asked about how they defined breakfast. The term ‘breakfast’ was a generally used term and the students generally defined it as food eaten in the morning before school (CA Johnson and TP Pedersen, unpublished results; available upon request).

Table 1 Item wording, response keys and categorization used in analyses

Independent variables: family functioning

Three dimensions of family functioning were measured( Reference Kitzman-Ulrich, Wilson and George 18 , Reference Broderick 19 ): (i) closeness, measured by the extent of close relations to parents; (ii) communication, measured by the quality of family communication; and (iii) support, measured by family support.

Close relations to parents

Close relations to parents was measured by asking the students how easy they find it to talk to each of their parents/step-parents about issues that really bother them (Table 1). Following conceptual considerations responses were dichotomized for each parent: close relations to parents (=‘very easy’ or ‘easy’) v. no close relations to parents (=‘difficult’, ‘very difficult’ or ‘don’t have or see this person’). Afterwards the variables were combined into close relations with at least two parents, close relations with one parent, and no close relations with parents. The item has proved useful and has been reported as such by different focus groups and teams in the international HBSC study( Reference Brooks, Tabak and Zaborskis 45 , Reference Damsgaard, Holstein and Koushede 46 ).

Quality of family communication

Quality of family communication was measured by an index constructed based on a short version of the clear communication scale from the Family Dynamics Measure II( Reference White, Grzankowski and Paavilainen 47 ). The students responded to four statements about the communication in their family. Based on conceptual considerations responses were dichotomized for each statement: ‘strongly agree’ or ‘agree’ (=1) v. ‘neither agree nor disagree’, ‘disagree’ or ‘strongly disagree’ (=0). Next the four dichotomized variables were coded into a sum score with five levels of quality of family communication: 0 (low level of quality of family communication) to 4 (high level of quality of family communication; Table 1). The short four-item version has been tested among 11−17-year-olds in the international HBSC study and showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α=0·8)( Reference Brooks, Tabak and Zaborskis 45 ). To test the reliability of the index in the present study sample we determined the index’s internal consistency by Cronbach’s α (=0·9), which indicated an excellent internal consistency in the index( Reference Fayers and Machin 48 ). Further, we tested the index for differential item function (DIF) to investigate whether or not the items in the index perform differently in subgroups( Reference Fayers and Machin 49 ). We tested for DIF in relation to the included sociodemographic variables, namely gender, age group, family social class, family structure and migration status. According to Scott et al., meaningful DIF should be considered if the significant odds ratio estimates are larger than 2 or lower than 0·5( Reference Scott, Fayers and Aaronson 50 ). We found DIF (OR=0·42; 95 % CI 0·25, 0·70) in the group of immigrants compared with natives for the statement: ‘When there is a misunderstanding we talk it over until it’s clear’. To explore the DIF with regard to migration status we compared the final analyses with analyses conducted on a sub-sample consisting of only natives. These showed no changes in the directions of associations.

Family support

Family support was measured by an index inspired by the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS)( Reference Zimet, Dahlem and Zimet 51 ). The scale covers social support from both family and friends, but in the present study we included only the family component. The students responded to four statements about family support. Following conceptual considerations responses were dichotomized for each statement: ‘strongly agree’ or ‘agree’ (=1) v. ‘neither agree nor disagree’, ‘disagree’ or ‘strongly disagree’ (=0). Afterwards the four dichotomized variables were coded into a sum score with five levels of family support: 0 (low level of family support) to 4 (high level of family support; Table 1). The MSPSS has shown a well-established scale construction with good validity and reliability( Reference Brooks, Tabak and Zaborskis 45 , Reference Zimet, Dahlem and Zimet 51 , Reference Ng, Amer Siddiq and Aida 52 ). The Danish version of the index is not fully comparable with the original MSPSS as we applied five response categories (‘strongly agree’, ‘agree’, ‘neither agree or disagree’, ‘disagree’, ‘strongly disagree’). The original MSPSS has seven categories from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. We tested the family support index for internal consistency by Cronbach’s α, which was high (=0·9). Further, we tested for DIF in relation to the sociodemographic subgroups and found no significant DIF.

Sociodemographic variables

We included gender, age group, socio-economic position, migration status and family structure in the analyses. Grade (7th and 9th) was used as a proxy for age group as the age variation within grades in Denmark is small. Socio-economic position was measured by family occupational social class. Students’ responses to items about their parents’ occupation were coded into family social class by the research staff and categorized into seven groups (social class I, II, III IV, V, economically inactive and unclassifiable). We followed the definitions of social class applied by the Danish National Institute of Social Research, which is almost identical to the UK Registrar General’s classification( Reference Galobardes, Shaw and Lawlor 53 , Reference Hansen 54 ). In the analyses family social class was dichotomized into high (I, II, III) and low social class (IV, V, economically inactive and unclassifiable). Sensitivity analyses with group III included in the low family social class category were performed and the estimates did not change notably. Further, before including unclassifiable responses (n 244) in the low family social class group, the analyses were performed with unclassifiable categorized in a separate group. This revealed an association between the category unclassifiable and breakfast frequency similar to the association between low family social class and breakfast frequency. Students’ migration status was classified as either native Danes or immigrants/descendants of immigrants, based on their responses to items about their own and their parents’ country of birth. Family structure was defined based on students’ reports of who they live with and categorized as traditional family (living with two biological parents) and other (single parent, reconstructed family, other family types; Table 1).

Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted using the statistical software package SAS version 9·3. In the descriptive analysis of distributions χ 2 tests of significance were used to examine differences in breakfast frequency by sociodemographic factors (gender, age group, family social class, family structure and migration status).

We used Cronbach’s α to test the internal consistency of the quality of family communication index and the family support index. To test for DIF we used logistic regression with each item in the index as dependent variable and the index and the sociodemographic variable as independent variables( Reference Fayers and Machin 49 , Reference Scott, Fayers and Aaronson 50 ).

Logistic regression models were generated to estimate the association between breakfast frequency and (i) close relations to parents, (ii) quality of family communication and (iii) family support. Initial analyses stratified by gender revealed same-direction associations for boys and girls, and analyses are therefore presented for the total sample. To account for the risk of data dependency due to the applied cluster design, we specified three-level hierarchical models (students nested within classes nested within schools) using SAS 9·3 PROC GLIMMIX. In the first step we analysed the associations unadjusted and in the second step we adjusted the analyses for the included sociodemographic variables. In initial analyses we included the sociodemographic variables one at a time and we conducted the analyses with family social class and family structure with and without dichotomizing the variables, but the associations between family functioning and low breakfast frequency did not alter.

In the third step we tested the associations for modification by sociodemographic factors. First, we examined the overall effect modification by including interaction terms between family functioning and sociodemographic factors in the model one at a time. Second, we examined the joint effect; that is, the combined effect of two variables for the three dimensions of family functioning and the sociodemographic variables. By including the combined effect it is possible to compare associations for the different combinations of the two variables with a common reference category whereby it is possible to identify protective or harmful combinations( Reference Vandenbroucke, von Elm and Altman 55 , Reference de Mutsert, Jager and Zoccali 56 ). Associations for all combinations were compared and those combinations that were identified as harmful were further investigated by testing for multiplicative effect modification ( $${\rm ratio\; of\; odds\; ratios}\,{\equals}\,{\rm OR}_{{{\plus}\!{\plus}}} /{\rm OR}_{{{\plus}{\minus}}}{\times}{\rm OR}_{{{\minus}{\plus}}}{\plus}1$$ ) and additive effect modification (relative excess risk due to interaction, $${\rm RERI}\,{\equals}\,{\rm OR}_{{{\plus}\!{\plus}}} {\minus}{\rm OR}_{{{\plus}{\minus}}} {\minus}{\rm OR}_{{{\minus}{\plus}}} {\plus}1$$ )( Reference Knol and VanderWeele 57 , Reference Vanderweele and Knol 58 ). The confidence intervals for the ratio of odds ratios and RERI were calculated as proposed by Hosmer and Lemeshow( Reference Hosmer and Lemeshow 59 ). Only the relevant combinations are presented in the figures.

Only twenty-two adolescents (0·7 %) did not answer the breakfast frequency item. Analyses of missing data on the family functioning measures were conducted by comparing sociodemographic differences between respondents and non-respondents and testing differences by χ 2 tests of significance. We found that adolescents with missing information on the family functioning measures were characterized by more often reporting low breakfast frequency, being a boy, being 13 years old (not significant for the measure of close relations to parents), being from low social class, being an immigrant and living in another family structure than traditional (only significant for the measure of close relations to parents; P<0·05).

Results

Descriptive results

Nearly one-quarter of the adolescents consumed breakfast on fewer than four out of five weekdays (low breakfast frequency) and significantly more girls than boys reported low breakfast frequency (P<0·0001; Table 2). There was a significantly larger proportion of participants with low breakfast frequency among 15- than 13-year-olds (P=0·0023), adolescents from low family social class (P<0·0001), immigrants (P<0·0001) and other family structure than traditional families (P<0·0001).

Table 2 Gender-specific distribution of family functioning variables, sociodemographic variables and the proportion of low breakfast frequency among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014

* Low breakfast frequency defined as eating breakfast on less than four days during weekdays.

Table 2 shows that a significantly larger proportion reported low breakfast frequency among adolescents with no close relations to parents (32·8 %) compared with adolescents with close relations to two or more parents (18·8 %; P<0·0001). A significantly larger proportion of adolescents reported low breakfast frequency when they experienced low quality of family communication (40·2 %) compared with adolescents who reported high quality of family communication (18·8 %; P<0·0001). A significantly larger proportion of adolescents reporting low family support had low breakfast frequency (38·9 %) compared with adolescents who reported high family support (19·5 %; P<0·0001).

Logistic regression analyses

Table 3 shows that inclusion of the sociodemographic variables in the statistical models attenuated the associations between the three measures of family functioning and breakfast frequency. Still, statistical significance remained. The adjusted analyses in Table 3 show that low breakfast frequency was associated with low family functioning when measured by three dimensions. The OR for low breakfast frequency was 1·81 (95 % CI 1·40, 2·33) among adolescents who reported no close relations to parents compared with adolescents who reported close relations to two or more parents. Among adolescents who had low level of quality of family communication the OR for low breakfast frequency was 2·28 (95 % CI 1·61, 3·22) compared with adolescents with high level of quality of family communication (lower level of score compared with uppermost level of score). The OR for low breakfast frequency was 2·09 (95 % CI 1·39, 3·15) among adolescents who reported low level of family support compared with adolescents who reported high level of family support (lower level of score compared with uppermost level of score).

Table 3 Odds ratios (95 % CI) for low breakfast frequency by family functioning, unadjusted and adjusted for sociodemographic variables, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014

Significant associations are shown in bold font.

The adjusted analyses of all three dimensions of family functioning showed associations between the included sociodemographic variables (gender, age group, family social class, migration status and family structure) and low breakfast frequency. Low breakfast frequency was more common among girls, 15-year-olds, adolescents from low family social class, immigrant adolescents and adolescents living in other family structures than traditional.

Effect modification analyses

The joint effect analysis suggested for all three dimensions of family functioning that the odds of having low breakfast frequency among adolescents with low compared with high family functioning were considerably higher among girls than among boys (Figs 13). This finding was supported by tests for effect modification. We found positive additive effect modification of no close relations to parents and being a girl, RERI=1·30 (95 % CI 0·19, 2·42). Further, we found positive multiplicative and additive effect modification of low level of quality of family communication and being a girl. The multiplicative ratio of odds ratios =2·32 (95 % CI 1·09, 4·96), RERI=2·89 (95 % CI 0·84, 4·95). We found positive effect modification of low level of family support and being a girl but the finding was not significant.

Fig. 1 Odds ratios (with 95 % CI represented by vertical bars) for low breakfast frequency by combinations of close relations to parents and gender, adjusted for sociodemographic variables*, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014. Effect modification of the combination of no close relations to parents and being a girl: ratio of odds ratios=3·36/(1·46×1·60)=1·44 (95 % CI 0·86, 2·42); relative excess risk due to interaction=3·36–1·46–1·60+1=1·30 (95 % CI 0·19, 2·42). *Only the combinations that had an effect are illustrated (ref., reference category)

Fig. 2 Odds ratios (with 95 % CI represented by vertical bars) for low breakfast frequency by combinations of quality of family communication and gender, adjusted for sociodemographic variables*, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014. Effect modification of the combination of low level of family communication and being a girl: ratio of odds ratios=4·78/(1·30×1·59)=2·32 (95 % CI 1·09, 4·96); relative excess risk due to interaction=4·78–1·30–1·59+1=2·89 (95 % CI 0·84, 4·95). *Only the combinations that had an effect are illustrated (ref., reference category)

Fig. 3 Odds ratios (with 95 % CI represented by vertical bars) for low breakfast frequency by combinations of family support and gender, adjusted for sociodemographic variables*, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014. Effect modification of the combination of low level of family support and being a girl: ratio of odds ratios =3·98/(1·64×1·61)=1·51 (95 % CI 0·65, 3·52); relative excess risk due to interaction=3·98–1·64–1·61+1=1·73 (95 % CI –0·54, 4·00). *Only the combinations that had an effect are illustrated (ref., reference category)

Further, the joint effect analyses suggested that the odds of having low breakfast frequency for adolescents with no close relations to parents compared with adolescents with close relations to two or more parents were considerably higher among immigrants than among native Danes, OR=5·96 (95 % CI 3·20, 11·09; figure not shown). Test for effect modification showed positive effect modification but the finding was not significant. Further, the odds of having low breakfast frequency for adolescents with low level of family support compared with adolescents with high level of family support were considerably higher among immigrants than among native Danes, OR=5·56 (95 % CI 1·85, 16·73; figure not shown). Further, test for effect modification revealed positive effect modification but the finding was not significant. Also the odds of having low breakfast frequency for adolescents with low compared with high family support were considerably higher among adolescents living in a family structure other than traditional as compared with adolescents from a traditional family structure, OR=4·35 (95 % CI 2·48, 7·62; figure not shown). Test for effect modification showed positive effect modification but the finding was not significant. The joint effect analyses with age group and family social class did not reveal any modifying effect.

Discussion

The present study showed that nearly one-quarter of Danish adolescents had low breakfast frequency and that low breakfast frequency was most common among 15-year-olds, adolescents from low family social class, immigrant adolescents and adolescents from other family structures than traditional families. Low breakfast frequency was associated with low family functioning measured by three dimensions: close relations to parents, quality of family communication and family support. Joint effect analyses suggested that the relationship between low family functioning and low breakfast frequency was stronger among girls than among boys. These findings were supported by effect modification tests for the two family functioning dimensions of close relations to parents and quality of family communication.

The finding that low family functioning is associated with low breakfast frequency corresponds to previous studies of family functioning and breakfast consumption among adolescents, although the measures for family functioning vary by study( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 , Reference Young and Fors 25 , Reference Moore and Harre 26 , Reference Franko, Thompson and Affenito 60 ). Berge et al. studied daily breakfast consumption and family functioning by measuring family communication, closeness, problem solving and behavioural control( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ). Franko et al. studied breakfast frequency and family cohesion measured by emotional bonding, supportiveness, family boundaries and spending time together( Reference Franko, Thompson and Bauserman 24 ). Moore and Harre studied breakfast frequency and family cohesion measured by emotional bonding, space, friends and decision making( Reference Moore and Harre 26 ) and Young and Fors studied daily consumption of healthy breakfast and communication with parents about serious issues( Reference Young and Fors 25 ). The latter is similar to our measure of close relations to parents. Young and Fors found results similar to ours although studying healthy breakfast (not defined). The findings for the three dimensions of family functioning included in the present study add to the existing findings and highlight the importance of family functioning in relation to breakfast consumption.

With the exception of quality of family communication, the joint effect analyses of the present study suggested that the combination of being an immigrant and having low level of family functioning increased the risk of low breakfast frequency. One previous study investigated the modifying effect of ethnicity, but did not identify such( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ). The present findings therefore add to the literature documenting less frequent breakfast consumption among immigrants compared with natives( Reference Delva, O’Malley and Johnston 36 Reference Jensen and Holstein 38 ). Our findings also suggested that the combined effect of being a girl and having low family functioning increased the odds for low breakfast frequency. It is well documented that girls skip breakfast more often than boys( Reference Sjöberg, Hallberg and Höglund 15 , Reference Pedersen, Meilstrup and Holstein 27 , Reference Currie, Zanotti and Morgan 28 ). The present findings add to this knowledge and highlight the relevance of gender when aiming at understanding the relationship between family functioning and breakfast habits. Further, analyses also suggested that living in another family structure than traditional and having low family support increased the odds of low breakfast frequency. Interestingly, this finding is present for only one of the dimensions of family functioning (family support). Still, it adds to the existing knowledge of family structure and adolescents’ breakfast consumption( Reference Vereecken, Dupuy and Rasmussen 32 Reference Levin, Kirby and Currie 35 ).

Kremers’ model highlights the importance of the family setting for adolescent energy-related behaviours( Reference Kremers, de Bruijn and Visscher 39 , Reference Kremers 40 ). Breakfast consumption has earlier been shown to be associated with the family setting( Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely 13 ) and the measures of family functioning included in the present study support the importance of the family setting when studying breakfast consumption among adolescents. It could be hypothesized that adolescents who experience a high level of family communication and support are more susceptible to parents’ advice about breakfast consumption and healthy living( Reference Franko, Thompson and Bauserman 24 ). High family functioning could also be a general characteristic of families that support healthy habits and provide the availability of breakfast or even share the breakfast meal( Reference Franko, Thompson and Bauserman 24 ).

Limitations and strengths

The presented results should be assessed in relation to considering the limitations and strengths of the study. The response rate in the participating schools was high but the risk of selection bias cannot be neglected. If non-respondents are more likely to come from low family functioning families and also more likely to skip breakfast, the presented associations between family functioning and breakfast frequency may be underestimated.

Further, analyses of adolescents not responding to the family functioning items revealed that the group of non-respondents more often had low breakfast frequency, were a boy, were 13 years old, came from low social class, were an immigrant or from other family structure than traditional. The proportion of missing was small (close relations to parents, 2·9 %; quality of family communication, 3·5 %; family support, 5·8 %).

The current study was conducted based on a cross-sectional study design and it is therefore not possible to establish a causal link. In the study family functioning is considered to be a determinant for breakfast frequency. However, it could be hypothesized that a shared meal may lead to better family functioning. Others have found an association between family functioning and shared family meals( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ) and the family meal has been used as a measure of family functioning( Reference Berge 61 ).

The applied family functioning measures are widely used and the internal reliability has been tested in the present study sample. The measure of close relations to parents is limited to only including parents and step-parents. Family constellations differ and adolescents could have close relations to other than parents.

In the present study parents’ working hours could constitute unmeasured confounding. Parents who leave for work early or sleep late due to late working hours do not have the possibility for providing breakfast for their children( Reference Bauer, Hearst and Escoto 62 ). Also, it could be hypothesized that the absence of parents due to working hours may affect adolescents’ perception of their family’s functioning. Unfortunately, the study does not include data about parents’ working hours.

The present study is strengthened by inclusion of a large, nationally representative sample of adolescents. The response rates were high and initial pilot studies suggested that the measurement of breakfast frequency was valid. Further, for the included sociodemographic variables we tested effect modification by both multiplicative and additive effect modification. The multiplicative effect modification is often reported in epidemiological studies but the importance of presenting effect modification on both the multiplicative and additive scales has been emphasized, and the additive effect modification has been stated as more relevant in public health research( Reference Vandenbroucke, von Elm and Altman 55 , Reference de Mutsert, Jager and Zoccali 56 ). In the current study some of the joint effect findings are not supported by the effect modification tests, which may be due to a limited sample size. Investigating effect modification is most appropriate in large data samples as such estimations are very power-sensitive( Reference Vanderweele and Knol 58 ). Rothman and Greenland recommend that assessments are not based on statistical significance alone( Reference Rothman and Greeenland 63 ).

Implications

Breakfast consumption has been highlighted as important for the health of adolescents and the present study emphasizes the importance of including the family setting, particularly family functioning, in future studies of adolescent breakfast consumption. To understand the mechanisms underlying the link between family functioning and breakfast consumption, additional qualitative and quantitative studies should be conducted. Qualitative studies would contribute to a deeper and more detailed understanding of the underlying mechanism. Quantitative studies should include combinations of family functioning measures and other measures of family functioning such as measures of family meal culture, and also explore the importance of parents’ working hours. Others have found positive associations between parental monitoring( Reference Young and Fors 25 ), parental style( Reference Pearson, Atkin and Biddle 64 ) and breakfast frequency and this supports the importance of the family setting. Further, also the social context of the breakfast meal, such as the shared family breakfast meal, has been associated with better nutrition( Reference Larson, MacLehose and Fulkerson 65 ) and others have found an association between family functioning and shared family meals( Reference Berge, Wall and Larson 20 ). To understand these underlying mechanisms additional research is needed. Further, future studies should also refine the breakfast measure to include measures of quality of the breakfast meal and measures of where the breakfast meal is consumed.

The practical implications of the present study relate to the emphasis that should be directed towards the family setting when intervening at breakfast consumption. However, intervening directly at family functioning is difficult. Instead adolescents who skip breakfast may be reached through providing breakfast at school. This is in line with the ecological theory, which suggests that the effect of one setting (e.g. school) on health behaviour may modify the effect of another setting (e.g. family)( Reference Bronfenbrenner 66 ).

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors thank the HBSC study for use of data. The international coordinator is Professor Candace Currie from the University of St Andrews and the international databank manager is Professor Oddrun Samdal from the University of Bergen. The Danish principal investigator is Associated Professor Mette Rasmussen from the National Institute of Public Health. Financial support: This work was supported by the Nordea Foundation (grant number 02-2011-0122) and the Tryg Foundation. The Nordea Foundation and the Tryg Foundation had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: T.P.P., B.E.H. and M.R. contributed to the planning of research questions and the analytical strategy. Data analyses were conducted by T.P.P. T.P.P. drafted the manuscript with critical input and interpretations from B.E.H., M.T.D. and M.R. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the principles of informed consent, providing oral and written information to the participants that participation was voluntary and completely anonymous. It was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki. In Denmark, there is no ethics agency for approval of non-invasive studies such as questionnaire-based studies in the general population. Because there is no such agency, we asked for approval from the school headmasters and school boards.

References

1. Nicklas, TA, Baranowski, T, Cullen, KW et al. (2001) Eating patterns, dietary quality and obesity. J Am Coll Nutr 20, 599608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Rampersaud, GC, Pereira, MA, Girard, BL et al. (2005) Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 743760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Mesas, AE, Munoz-Pareja, M, Lopez-Garcia, E et al. (2011) Selected eating behaviours and excess body weight: a systematic review. Obes Rev 13, 106135.Google ScholarPubMed
4. Fabritius, K & Rasmussen, M (2008) Breakfast habits and overweight in Danish schoolchildren. The role of socioeconomic positions. Ugeskr Laeger 170, 25592563.Google ScholarPubMed
5. Szajewska, H & Ruszczynski, M (2010) Systematic review demonstrating that breakfast consumption influences body weight outcomes in children and adolescents in Europe. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 50, 113119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Smith, KJ, Gall, SL, McNaughton, SA et al. (2010) Skipping breakfast: longitudinal associations with cardiometabolic risk factors in the Childhood Determinants of Adult Health Study. Am J Clin Nutr 92, 13161325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Wennberg, M, Gustafsson, PE, Wennberg, P et al. (2015) Poor breakfast habits in adolescence predict the metabolic syndrome in adulthood. Public Health Nutr 18, 122129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Hoyland, A, Dye, L & Lawton, CL (2009) A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutr Res Rev 22, 220243.Google ScholarPubMed
9. Cooper, SB, Bandelow, S & Nevill, ME (2011) Breakfast consumption and cognitive function in adolescent schoolchildren. Physiol Behav 103, 431439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Pedersen, TP, Holstein, BE, Flachs, EM et al. (2013) Meal frequencies in early adolescence predict meal frequencies in late adolescence and early adulthood. BMC Public Health 13, 445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Merten, MJ, Williams, AL & Shriver, LH (2009) Breakfast consumption in adolescence and young adulthood: parental presence, community context, and obesity. J Am Diet Assoc 109, 13841391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12. Levin, KA & Kirby, J (2012) Irregular breakfast consumption in adolescence and the family environment: underlying causes by family structure. Appetite 59, 6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Pearson, N, Biddle, SJ & Gorely, T (2009) Family correlates of breakfast consumption among children and adolescents. A systematic review. Appetite 52, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Pereira, MA, Erickson, E, McKee, P et al. (2011) Breakfast frequency and quality may affect glycemia and appetite in adults and children. J Nutr 141, 163168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Sjöberg, A, Hallberg, L, Höglund, D et al. (2003) Meal pattern, food choice, nutrient intake and lifestyle factors in the Goteborg Adolescence Study. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 15691578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Tinsley, BJ (2003) How Children Learn to be Healthy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
17. Sleddens, EF, Kroeze, W, Kohl, LF et al. (2015) Determinants of dietary behavior among youth: an umbrella review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12, 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Kitzman-Ulrich, H, Wilson, DK St, George, SM et al. (2010) The integration of a family systems approach for understanding youth obesity, physical activity, and dietary programs. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 13, 231253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Broderick, C (1993) Understanding Family Process: Basics of Family Systems Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications Inc.Google Scholar
20. Berge, JM, Wall, M, Larson, N et al. (2013) Family functioning: associations with weight status, eating behaviors, and physical activity in adolescents. J Adolesc Health 52, 351357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. Halliday, JA, Palma, CL, Mellor, D et al. (2014) The relationship between family functioning and child and adolescent overweight and obesity: a systematic review. Int J Obes (Lond) 38, 480493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Mellin, AE, Neumark-Sztainer, D, Story, M et al. (2002) Unhealthy behaviors and psychosocial difficulties among overweight adolescents: the potential impact of familial factors. J Adolesc Health 31, 145153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Neumark-Sztainer, D, Story, M, Resnick, MD et al. (1996) Correlates of inadequate fruit and vegetable consumption among adolescents. Prev Med 25, 497505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24. Franko, DL, Thompson, D, Bauserman, R et al. (2008) What’s love got to do with it? Family cohesion and healthy eating behaviors in adolescent girls. Int J Eat Disord 41, 360367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25. Young, EM & Fors, SW (2001) Factors related to the eating habits of students in grades 9–12. J Sch Health 71, 483488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26. Moore, J & Harre, N (2007) Eating and activity: the importance of family and environment. Health Promot J Aust 18, 143148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Pedersen, TP, Meilstrup, C, Holstein, BE et al. (2012) Fruit and vegetable intake is associated with frequency of breakfast, lunch and evening meal: cross-sectional study of 11-, 13-, and 15-year-olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 9, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Currie, C, Zanotti, C, Morgan, A et al. (2012) Social Determinants of Health and Well-Being Among Young People. Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) Study: International Report from the 2009/2010 Survey. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
29. Höglund, D, Samuelson, G & Mark, A (1998) Food habits in Swedish adolescents in relation to socioeconomic conditions. Eur J Clin Nutr 52, 784789.Google ScholarPubMed
30. Utter, J, Denny, S, Crengle, S et al. (2011) Socio-economic differences in eating-related attitudes, behaviours and environments of adolescents. Public Health Nutr 14, 629634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Moore, GF, Tapper, K, Murphy, S et al. (2007) Associations between deprivation, attitudes towards eating breakfast and breakfast eating behaviours in 9–11-year-olds. Public Health Nutr 10, 582589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Vereecken, C, Dupuy, M, Rasmussen, M et al. HBSC Eating & Dieting Focus Group (2009) Breakfast consumption and its socio-demographic and lifestyle correlates in schoolchildren in 41 countries participating in the HBSC study. Int J Public Health 54, Suppl. 2, 180190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Johansen, A, Rasmussen, S & Madsen, M (2006) Health behaviour among adolescents in Denmark: influence of school class and individual risk factors. Scand J Public Health 34, 3240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Jørgensen, A, Pedersen, TP, Meilstrup, CR et al. (2011) The influence of family structure on breakfast habits among adolescents. Dan Med Bull 58, A4262.Google ScholarPubMed
35. Levin, KA, Kirby, J & Currie, C (2012) Family structure and breakfast consumption of 11–15 year old boys and girls in Scotland, 1994–2010: a repeated cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 12, 228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Delva, J, O’Malley, PM & Johnston, LD (2006) Racial/ethnic and socioeconomic status differences in overweight and health-related behaviors among American students: national trends 1986–2003. J Adolesc Health 39, 536545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Utter, J, Scragg, R, Mhurchu, CN et al. (2007) At-home breakfast consumption among New Zealand children: associations with body mass index and related nutrition behaviors. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 570576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. Jensen, HN & Holstein, BE (2010) Health and Wellbeing Among Immigrants, Descendants and Ethnic Danes in 5th, 7th and 9th Grade. Copenhagen: National Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark.Google Scholar
39. Kremers, SP, de Bruijn, GJ, Visscher, TL et al. (2006) Environmental influences on energy balance-related behaviors: a dual-process view. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Kremers, SP (2010) Theory and practice in the study of influences on energy balance-related behaviors. Patient Educ Couns 79, 291298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41. Brug, J, Kremers, SP, Lenthe, F et al. (2008) Environmental determinants of healthy eating: in need of theory and evidence. Proc Nutr Soc 67, 307316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42. Currie, C, Nic Gabhainn, S & Godeau, E; International HBSC Network Coordinating Committee (2009) The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children: WHO Collaborative Cross-National (HBSC) study: origins, concept, history and development 1982–2008. Int J Public Health 54, Suppl. 2, 131139.Google ScholarPubMed
43. Roberts, C, Freeman, J, Samdal, O et al.; International HBSC Study Group (2009) The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study: methodological developments and current tensions. Int J Public Health 54, Suppl. 2, 140150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. Pedersen, TP, Holstein, BE, Laursen, B et al. (2015) Main meal frequency measures in the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study: agreement with 7-day 24-h recalls. Int J Public Health 60, 945952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45. Brooks, F, Tabak, I, Zaborskis, A et al. (2014) Family culture. In Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) Study Protocol: Background, Methodology and Mandatory Items for the 2013/14 Survey, pp. 98115 [C Currie, J Inchley, M Molcho et al., editors]. St Andrews: Child and Adolescent Health Research Unit.Google Scholar
46. Damsgaard, MT, Holstein, BE, Koushede, V et al. (2014) Close relations to parents and emotional symptoms among adolescents: beyond socio-economic impact? Int J Public Health 59, 721726.Google ScholarPubMed
47. White, MA, Grzankowski, J, Paavilainen, E et al. (2003) Family dynamics and child abuse and neglect in three Finnish communities. Issues Ment Health Nurs 24, 707722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48. Fayers, PM & Machin, D (2007) 5. Multi-item scales. In Quality of Life: The Assessment, Analysis and Interpretation of Patient-Reported Outcomes, 2nd ed., pp. 109130. Chichester: John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
49. Fayers, PM & Machin, D (2007) 7. Item response theory and differential item functioning. In Quality of Life: The Assessment, Analysis and Interpretation of Patient-Reported Outcomes, 2nd ed., pp. 161188. Chichester: John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
50. Scott, NW, Fayers, PM, Aaronson, NK et al. (2010) Differential item functioning (DIF) analyses of health-related quality of life instruments using logistic regression. Health Qual Life Outcomes 8, 81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Zimet, GD, Dahlem, NW, Zimet, SG et al. (1988) The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J Pers Assess 52, 3041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
52. Ng, CG, Amer Siddiq, AN, Aida, SA et al. (2010) Validation of the Malay version of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS-M) among a group of medical students in Faculty of Medicine, University Malaya. Asian J Psychiatr 3, 36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53. Galobardes, B, Shaw, M, Lawlor, DA et al. (2006) Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 2). J Epidemiol Community Health 60, 95101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54. Hansen, EJ (1986) The Living Circumstances of the Danish People – 1986 Compared to 1976: An Interview Study of 4500 Danish People. Copenhagen: Hans Reitzels.Google Scholar
55. Vandenbroucke, JP, von Elm, E, Altman, DG et al. (2007) Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE): explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med 4, e297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56. de Mutsert, R, Jager, KJ, Zoccali, C et al. (2009) The effect of joint exposures: examining the presence of interaction. Kidney Int 75, 677681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57. Knol, MJ & VanderWeele, TJ (2012) Recommendations for presenting analyses of effect modification and interaction. Int J Epidemiol 41, 514520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58. Vanderweele, TJ & Knol, MJ (2014) A tutorial on interaction. Epidemiol Methods 3, 3372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
59. Hosmer, DW & Lemeshow, S (1992) Confidence interval estimation of interaction. Epidemiology 3, 452456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60. Franko, DL, Thompson, D, Affenito, SG et al. (2008) What mediates the relationship between family meals and adolescent health issues. Health Psychol 27, 2 Suppl., S109S117.Google ScholarPubMed
61. Berge, JM (2009) A review of familial correlates of child and adolescent obesity: what has the 21st century taught us so far? Int J Adolesc Med Health 21, 457483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62. Bauer, KW, Hearst, MO, Escoto, K et al. (2012) Parental employment and work–family stress: associations with family food environments. Soc Sci Med 75, 496504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63. Rothman, KJ & Greeenland, S (editors) (1998) The emergence of modern epidemiology. In Modern Epidemiology, 2nd ed., pp. 36. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
64. Pearson, N, Atkin, AJ, Biddle, SJ et al. (2010) Parenting styles, family structure and adolescent dietary behaviour. Public Health Nutr 13, 12451253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65. Larson, N, MacLehose, R, Fulkerson, JA et al. (2013) Eating breakfast and dinner together as a family: associations with sociodemographic characteristics and implications for diet quality and weight status. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 16011609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66. Bronfenbrenner, U (1979) The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA/London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Item wording, response keys and categorization used in analyses

Figure 1

Table 2 Gender-specific distribution of family functioning variables, sociodemographic variables and the proportion of low breakfast frequency among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014

Figure 2

Table 3 Odds ratios (95 % CI) for low breakfast frequency by family functioning, unadjusted and adjusted for sociodemographic variables, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Odds ratios (with 95 % CI represented by vertical bars) for low breakfast frequency by combinations of close relations to parents and gender, adjusted for sociodemographic variables*, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014. Effect modification of the combination of no close relations to parents and being a girl: ratio of odds ratios=3·36/(1·46×1·60)=1·44 (95 % CI 0·86, 2·42); relative excess risk due to interaction=3·36–1·46–1·60+1=1·30 (95 % CI 0·19, 2·42). *Only the combinations that had an effect are illustrated (ref., reference category)

Figure 4

Fig. 2 Odds ratios (with 95 % CI represented by vertical bars) for low breakfast frequency by combinations of quality of family communication and gender, adjusted for sociodemographic variables*, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014. Effect modification of the combination of low level of family communication and being a girl: ratio of odds ratios=4·78/(1·30×1·59)=2·32 (95 % CI 1·09, 4·96); relative excess risk due to interaction=4·78–1·30–1·59+1=2·89 (95 % CI 0·84, 4·95). *Only the combinations that had an effect are illustrated (ref., reference category)

Figure 5

Fig. 3 Odds ratios (with 95 % CI represented by vertical bars) for low breakfast frequency by combinations of family support and gender, adjusted for sociodemographic variables*, among adolescents (n 3054) aged 13 and 15 years, Danish arm of the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study, 2014. Effect modification of the combination of low level of family support and being a girl: ratio of odds ratios =3·98/(1·64×1·61)=1·51 (95 % CI 0·65, 3·52); relative excess risk due to interaction=3·98–1·64–1·61+1=1·73 (95 % CI –0·54, 4·00). *Only the combinations that had an effect are illustrated (ref., reference category)