Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T11:42:36.452Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language and executive function relationships in the real world: insights from deafness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 March 2024

Mario Figueroa*
Affiliation:
Department of Basic, Developmental and Educational Psychology, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Nicola Botting
Affiliation:
Department of Language and Communication Science, City, University of London, London, UK
Gary Morgan
Affiliation:
Psychology and Education Department, University Oberta de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Mario Figueroa; Email: mario.figueroa@uab.cat
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Executive functions (EFs) in both regulatory and meta-cognitive contexts are important for a wide variety of children’s daily activities, including play and learning. Despite the growing literature supporting the relationship between EF and language, few studies have focused on these links during everyday behaviours. Data were collected on 208 children from 6 to 12 years old of whom 89 were deaf children (55% female; M = 8;8; SD = 1;9) and 119 were typically hearing children (56% female, M = 8;9; SD = 1;5). Parents completed two inventories: to assess EFs and language proficiency. Parents of deaf children reported greater difficulties with EFs in daily activities than those of hearing children. Correlation analysis between EFs and language showed significant levels only in the deaf group, especially in relation to meta-cognitive EFs. The results are discussed in terms of the role of early parent–child interaction and the relevance of EFs for everyday conversational situations.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

The relationship between language and wider cognitive abilities is of great interest in developmental psychology (D’souza et al., Reference D’souza, D’souza and Karmiloff-Smith2017; Nip et al., Reference Nip, Green and Marx2011). The origins of this link are in place by the first months of life when listening to language can assist object categorization (Perszyk & Waxman, Reference Perszyk and Waxman2018). From 12 months, infants begin to establish relations between language and abstract representations (Carey, Reference Carey2009; Perszyk & Waxman, Reference Perszyk and Waxman2018) and in the next few years language begins to organize into three interrelated components: content (semantics and vocabulary); form (phonology, morphology syntax) and language use (pragmatics, Bloom & Lahey, Reference Bloom and Lahey1978). In addition to these three components, language reference and cohesion are essential when communication requires symbols to refer to information beyond the immediate context or relations between utterances, respectively (Bebko & McKinnon, Reference Bebko, McKinnon, Marschark and Clark1998). The current study focuses on the link between language and the executive function (EF) system. This link not only illuminates how children use their growing cognitive abilities to support their language learning, but also illustrates how the language system itself can be co-opted for cognitive reasoning tasks that require EFs. One scenario, which can shed light on this relationship, is when children experience delays in language development, for example, in the context of deaf children with hearing versus deaf parents. It is possible EF might help compensate for difficulties, or conversely could be affected by poor language.

In the rest of the paper, we briefly describe the EF system and then explain why we are interested in exploring the relationship between EF and language in both typically developing and deaf children who have variable language development. To do this, we split EF into two main types of abilities: meta-cognitive and behavioural regulation – and then empirically ask what the relationship is between these EFs and language.

2. Executive functions

The EF system comprises a set of top-down processes involved in coordinating and manipulating information, as well as controlling thoughts, behaviours and emotions (Zelazo & Carlson, Reference Zelazo and Carlson2012). EFs are required to solve a novel task, or plan a sequence of actions, for example, a child thinking what to put in a bag for a school trip the next day (Diamond, Reference Diamond2012). Different models exist for how the EF components work together (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Giesbrecht, Müller, McInerney and Kerns2012; Usai et al., Reference Usai, Viterbori, Traverso and De Franchis2014; Wiebe et al., Reference Wiebe, Sheffield, Nelson, Clark, Chevalier and Espy2011). Most models (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Miyake & Friedman, Reference Miyake and Friedman2012) include three areas: the resistance to interference (inhibition); the ability to flexibly shift from one area of focus to another (cognitive flexibility) and the ability to hold and manipulate information (working memory). It has been suggested that these three EFs underlie other executive abilities such as planning and cognitive fluency. In the model used in the current research, Gioia et al. (Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) defined eight clinical scales for children, which were inhibition, shifting, emotional control, initiation, working memory, planning, organization of materials and monitoring. Three of these scales form the Behaviour Regulation Index (inhibition, shifting and emotional control), while the Meta-cognition Index includes five subdomains (initiation, working memory, planning organization of materials and monitoring). All these subdomains are interrelated during everyday behaviour such as when a child gets their school bag ready for the next day. EFs in the Gioia et al. (Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) and Zelazo and Müller (Reference Zelazo, Müller and Goswami2002) model adopts a multifactorial approach that integrates cognitive and emotional processes.

Based on these models, Brock et al. (Reference Brock, Rimm-Kaufman, Nathanson and Grimm2009) proposed two sets of interrelated but distinguishable processes: behavioural regulation (hot) components of EF (e.g., inhibition or regulation of emotion) and meta-cognitive (cool) components (e.g., working memory or planning). Experimental tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test or Stroop are considered meta-cognition tasks since they lack an emotional component. In contrast, the assessment of behavioural regulation components involves more motivational contexts (e.g., delay of gratification tasks; Zelazo & Carlson, Reference Zelazo and Carlson2012). Using these concepts with a large group of kindergarteners, Brock et al. (Reference Brock, Rimm-Kaufman, Nathanson and Grimm2009) found cool EF predicted maths achievement and learning-related classroom behaviours.

The development of the EF system takes place throughout childhood, with emerging abilities in regulation in the first year and continuing refinements of meta-cognitive control in adolescence and young adulthood (Munakata et al., Reference Munakata, Snyder and Chatham2012). Deficits in EF can negatively affect children’s participation in many areas of social and academic activities (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg2014). Developmental studies show that while behavioural regulation and meta-cognition components are not clearly dissociable during infancy (Peterson & Welsh, Reference Peterson, Welsh, Goldstein and Naglieri2014), they develop rapidly during the preschool years when prefrontal regions undergo considerable growth (Carlson & Wang, Reference Carlson and Wang2007; Hongwanishkul et al., Reference Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee and Zelazo2005; Kerr & Zelazo, Reference Kerr and Zelazo2004). Inhibition is one of the most extensively studied EF skills in relation to development. Basic inhibitory control emerges in the first year of life and continues to develop rapidly throughout infancy and preschool years (Diamond, Reference Diamond, Stuss and Knight2002; Gandolfi et al., Reference Gandolfi, Viterbori, Traverso and Carmen Usai2014). This early development enables toddlers to regulate their behaviour in the face of external demands and challenges of conflict, delay and compliance (Kochanska & Aksan, Reference Kochanska and Aksan2006; Kopp, Reference Kopp2002). However, inhibitory skills are characterised by great interpersonal variation and early inhibitory development is very diverse (Wolfe & Bell, Reference Wolfe and Bell2007). More complex meta-cognitive skills, such as planning, develop beyond infancy throughout childhood and adolescence (Best et al., Reference Best, Miller and Jones2009; Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010). From middle childhood and adolescence onwards, a distinction between behavioural regulation and meta-cognition components seems more compelling, with studies suggesting different developmental trajectories (Fernández García et al., Reference Fernández García, Merchán, Phillips-Silver and Daza González2021; Peterson & Welsh, Reference Peterson, Welsh, Goldstein and Naglieri2014). It may be the case that the EFs used for regulation, are more strongly associated with early social skills and interaction behaviours (Fernández García et al., Reference Fernández García, Merchán, Phillips-Silver and Daza González2021; Tsermentseli & Poland, Reference Tsermentseli and Poland2016). From 6 to 12 years old, children use EFs to acquire and consolidate conceptual or social skills (Brocki & Bohlin, Reference Brocki and Bohlin2010; Eccles, Reference Eccles1999; Fernández García et al., Reference Fernández García, Merchán, Phillips-Silver and Daza González2021). Meta-cognitive EFs are more significantly related to later complex language comprehension and academic achievement (Hooper et al., Reference Hooper, Luciana, Conklin and Yarger2004; Lensing & Elsner, Reference Lensing and Elsner2018).

There is also a growing literature looking at how early experiences influence EF development (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Koyasu, Oh, Ogawa, Short and Huang2009; Pascual et al., Reference Pascual, Moyano and Robres2019; Robson et al., Reference Robson, Allen and Howard2020). For example, early parental interaction with infants at 2 years of age can facilitate the regulation of emotional regulation (Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2009). Individual differences in this type of maternal scaffolding predicted children’s growing abilities to hold things in mind or inhibit impulse-driven responses 2 years later. Hughes et al. (Reference Hughes, White, Ensor and Lagattuta2014) point out that these early EF skills affect children’s ability to engage in social interactions such as early pretend play and enable infants to benefit from conversational environments. Children’s early family context and interaction, which supports self-regulation, have been identified as important factors in explaining variability in EF development (Carlson, Reference Carlson2003; Shonkoff & Phillips, Reference Shonkoff and Phillips2000). This is particularly important in instances where early interaction is disrupted, for example, in neonatal deafness (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Curtin and Botting2021; Rieffe, Reference Rieffe2012; Thompson & Steinbeis, Reference Thompson and Steinbeis2020).

2.1. How do EF and language relate during development?

Despite a rich body of research that has examined the relationship between EF skills and language, the nature of this developmental relationship is still unclear (Gandolfi & Viterbori, Reference Gandolfi and Viterbori2020; Slot & von Suchodoletz, Reference Slot and von Suchodoletz2018; Tonér & Gerholm, Reference Tonér and Gerholm2021). Some studies propose a model whereby EF is a driver of language development in typical (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2003; Gandolfi & Viterbori, Reference Gandolfi and Viterbori2020; Traverso et al., Reference Traverso, Viterbori, Gandolfi, Zanobini and Usai2022; Verhagen & Leseman, Reference Verhagen and Leseman2016) and atypical groups (Blom & Boerma, Reference Blom and Boerma2020; Pellicano, Reference Pellicano2010). Gandolfi and Viterbori (Reference Gandolfi and Viterbori2020) showed that early inhibition control in typically developing 2–3-year olds predicted future language outcomes in receptive grammar. EF skills may also contribute more than general intelligence in the language use scores of preschool children by helping them control their assertiveness and respond in socially appropriate ways (Blain-Brière et al., Reference Blain-Brière, Bouchard and Bigras2014). EF skills can facilitate language development by enabling children to focus attention, handle multiple sources of information simultaneously, consolidate meaning, monitor mistakes and make decisions in light of information received (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Weiland et al., Reference Weiland, Barata and Yoshikawa2014).

Other research argues for a model whereby language is the primary influence of EF development (Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Willoughby, Vernon-Feagans, Blair, Cox, Burchinal, Burton, Crnic, Crouter, Garrett-Peters, Greenberg, Lanza, Mills-Koonce, Werner and Willoughby2016; Miller & Marcovitch, Reference Miller and Marcovitch2015). The role of language as a key mechanism of self-regulation was recognised by Vygotsky (Reference Vygotsky, Vygotsky, Hanfmann and Vakar1962, Reference Vygotsky, Riewer and Carton1987) and Luria (Reference Luria1959, Reference Luria1961), who argued that from around the age of 2–3 years children develop the capacity to use private or self-directed speech to self-regulate. More recently, Zelazo (Reference Zelazo2015) has extended this relationship to broader EFs in their iterative reprocessing model, whereby language has a core role both in reflection (the elaborative reprocessing of information) and in the formulation and maintenance of goal-specific rules in working memory. In other words, language, via private speech, in older children acts as a meta-cognitive tool during EF tasks (Müller et al., Reference Müller, Zelazo, Hood, Leone and Rohrer2004). In the few existing longitudinal studies looking at this question, early language appears to predict later self-regulation skills and EF in typically developing children (Blom & Boerma, Reference Blom and Boerma2020; Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Willoughby, Vernon-Feagans, Blair, Cox, Burchinal, Burton, Crnic, Crouter, Garrett-Peters, Greenberg, Lanza, Mills-Koonce, Werner and Willoughby2016; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Bates and Staples2014) more than the other way around.

Finally, other studies suggest a bidirectional relation (Bohlmann et al., Reference Bohlmann, Maier and Palacios2015; Romeo et al., Reference Romeo, Flournoy, McLaughlin and Lengua2022; Slot & von Suchodoletz, Reference Slot and von Suchodoletz2018). Although language and EF are likely to be at least partially bi-directionally related during development, a model that identifies which is the stronger influence would be useful both theoretically and clinically. Notably, none of the studies cited above have considered the distinction between behavioural regulation and meta-cognition EF and language development. Finally, some of the mixed findings in the literature may be due to different researchers using different measures of EF and language ability, making clear conclusions difficult to interpret.

2.2. Measuring EF

Two primary approaches exist to measure EF competence in childhood: experimental tasks and questionnaires or inventories. Historically, the study of EF has been addressed through neuropsychological tests in adults under relatively decontextualized, non-emotional and analytical testing conditions (Peterson & Welsh, Reference Peterson, Welsh, Goldstein and Naglieri2014). Turning to assessments of children, many previous studies (Elliott, Reference Elliott2003; Funahashi, Reference Funahashi2001) use experimental tasks of language and EF rather than assessments of how these abilities work in real life scenarios. Experimental tasks (e.g., Stroop) are less related to real-world scenarios (Guare, Reference Guare2014) and normally take place in laboratory-based settings or in quiet environments for minimizing distractions. Also, the examiner tests the participant individually in non-dynamic and non-emotional charged contexts. This means children are usually evaluated in a context that is removed from the real world such as the home or classroom settings.

In contrast, inventories such as the Behavioural Rating Inventory of EF (BRIEF: Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) measure EF capabilities in real scenarios (e.g., does your child have difficulties waiting in line at school?) and as such may have more ecological validity. The BRIEF also has cut-offs for clinically significant EF difficulties which can identify children at risk and provide information for which behaviours can be targeted to therapists and educators (McCoy, Reference McCoy2019). Despite the BRIEF being designed to complement experimental tasks, they often do not correlate well with each other. This suggests that the two approaches (experimental vs. inventories) may be assessing different aspects of the same construct (Isquith et al., Reference Isquith, Roth and Gioia2013; Toplak et al., Reference Toplak, West and Stanovich2013). One premise relevant to the present study is that language and EF are both multidimensional constructs. However, to date, research on the language and EF developmental relationship has focused narrowly on children’s knowledge of language forms (i.e., words or grammar) rather than how language is used in different communicative situations (Gandolfi & Viterbori, Reference Gandolfi and Viterbori2020; Newbury et al., Reference Newbury, Klee, Stokes and Moran2016; Usai et al., Reference Usai, Viterbori, Gandolfi and Zanobini2020; Verhagen & Leseman, Reference Verhagen and Leseman2016; Yuile & Sabbagh, Reference Yuile and Sabbagh2021).

A weakness in EF coincides in many populations with language and communication difficulties (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Nation and Patterson2014). A range of factors in early childhood, such as neurobiological disorder or environmental restrictions to language access, can disrupt the development of language and EF skills. This is particularly relevant in the case of congenital deafness.

2.3. EFs and deafness

Deafness provides a unique lens for understanding the relationship between EF and language because in this case, language difficulties are usually caused by sensory rather than cognitive impairments. Research involving deaf children with hearing parents has documented EF difficulties both in the case of experimental tasks (Botting et al., Reference Botting, Jones, Marshall, Denmark, Atkinson and Morgan2017; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Atkinson, Marshall, Botting, St Clair and Morgan2020; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Colson, Henning and Pisoni2014b) and inventories (Beer et al., Reference Beer, Kronenberger, Castellanos, Colson, Henning and Pisoni2014; Hintermair, Reference Hintermair2013; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Beer, Castellanos, Pisoni and Miyamoto2014a). There are some inconsistencies reported across these studies potentially because of different methods of assessing EF and language or because studies conflated both behavioural regulation and meta-cognition EF tasks. For example, Figueroa et al. (Reference Figueroa, Silvestre and Darbra2022) found no significant differences between a deaf group with cochlear implant and a hearing control group on experimental EF tasks, while other authors found difficulties with meta-cognition and behavioural regulation skills (Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Xu and Pisoni2020). A small number of studies comparing deaf children with deaf parents (native signers) finds comparable performance on EF measures with hearing peers, strengthening the proposed link between early communication and language experience and typical development of EFs (Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Carrigan, Walker and Coppola2022; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2017, Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2018; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Jones, Denmark, Mason, Atkinson, Botting and Morgan2015).

Those studies using the BRIEF (Beer et al., Reference Beer, Kronenberger and Pisoni2011, Reference Beer, Kronenberger, Castellanos, Colson, Henning and Pisoni2014; Hintermair, Reference Hintermair2013; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Beer, Castellanos, Pisoni and Miyamoto2014a; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Xu and Pisoni2020) suggest EF problems at different developmental stages and with different aspects of EF in the questionnaires. Beer et al. (Reference Beer, Kronenberger, Castellanos, Colson, Henning and Pisoni2014) and more recently Blank and Holt (Reference Blank and Holt2022) document that hearing parents of deaf toddlers reported greater difficulty in meta-cognitive skills such as working memory than parents of a matched hearing group. Parents of deaf children did not report greater difficulty in behavioural regulation skills at this age. At school age, Hintermair (Reference Hintermair2013) assessed 214 deaf children from general schools and schools for the Deaf and found that both meta-cognition and behavioural regulation skills were both significantly lower in deaf children, especially if they were enrolled in schools for the Deaf. This is consistent with Hall et al. (Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2018), which showed that even with cochlear implant, deaf children have greater EF difficulties than hearing children. Beer et al. (Reference Beer, Kronenberger and Pisoni2011) conducted a study on 45 deaf children with cochlear implants. Results revealed poor behavioural regulation when compared with age norms. Thus, some studies report better behaviour regulation early on while consistent difficulties with meta-cognitive aspects, which would be consistent with a developmental explanation, while other studies report extended difficulties in both domains.

Furthermore, within the subscales of EF there are inconsistent findings with the BRIEF. Recently, McCreery and Walker (Reference McCreery and Walker2022) examined 177 deaf and 86 hearing children on working memory, shifting and inhibition. Deaf children only exhibited difficulties in working memory. Highlighting the heterogeneity of the deaf child population across these studies, deaf parents of deaf signers reported similar EF levels to parents of hearing children (Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Carrigan, Walker and Coppola2022; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2017, Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2018). This finding emphasises the importance of early successful language development and interaction (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Jones, Botting and Morgan2020). See Table S1 in the Supplementary Material for an overview of studies on behavioural regulation and meta-cognition studies in deaf populations.

In the literature concerning the developmental relationship between language and EF in deaf samples, the results have also been mixed. Several studies show EF growth is linked to deaf children’s knowledge of vocabulary and grammar that is language influences EF rather than in the other direction (Botting et al., Reference Botting, Jones, Marshall, Denmark, Atkinson and Morgan2017; Figueras et al., Reference Figueras, Edwards and Langdon2008; Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Carrigan, Walker and Coppola2022; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2018; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Atkinson, Marshall, Botting, St Clair and Morgan2020; Remine et al., Reference Remine, Care and Brown2008). Most of these studies used experimental methods to test EF. It is not clear if the influence of language over EF would be the same if measures of real EF scenarios and language were used. By comparing performance on sub-domains of the measure, we may be able to evaluate in a more nuanced way how language and EF relate to each other. The wider literature on hearing children points to a developmental progression between early and later EF abilities. Early to appear is basic behaviour regulation involving inhibition in the first year of life, which improves throughout infancy and the preschool years (Gandolfi et al., Reference Gandolfi, Viterbori, Traverso and Carmen Usai2014). More complex meta-cognitive skills, such as planning, develop in later childhood and adolescence (Best et al., Reference Best, Miller and Jones2009; Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010). In the current research on deaf children, we extend the question of how language and EF relate by examining the particular types of EF that have different developmental trajectories and language abilities with more real-world validity. The Language Proficiency Profile-2 (LPP-2: Bebko et al., Reference Bebko, Calderon and Treder2003) measures real-world uses of language as well as forms (i.e., knowledge of vocabulary and grammar). Furthermore, the LPP-2 was originally developed and has been used with studies of deaf children’s language (e.g., Sidera et al., Reference Sidera, Morgan and Serrat2020). In the current research, it is suggested that some of the ambiguity in the previous literature in how language and EF relate in deafness comes from the failure to decompose EF into behaviour regulation and metacognition. It is possible that these have different relationships with language, and this question is addressed in the current study.

In summary, it is unclear how EF and language work together in development. It is also necessary in regard to this question, to look at different components of EF, such as behaviour regulation and meta-cognition in order to ask if these are differentially affected by delayed language skills. EF development has been compared in deaf and hearing samples with neuro-psychological tasks, but less is known about how different meta-cognition and behavioural regulation components function in real-world scenarios (Ching et al., Reference Ching, Cupples, Leigh, Hou and Wong2021; Guardino & Antia, Reference Guardino and Antia2012). The questions that guided the present study were therefore:

  1. 1. Do deaf and hearing children differ on EF behaviours as reported by the BRIEF? Based on the wider literature reviewed previously, our hypothesis is that deaf children will perform significantly poorer than hearing children.

  2. 2. Are there differences between deaf and hearing children within sub-parts of the BRIEF related to meta-cognition versus behavioural regulation? We predict meta-cognition will be more delayed than behaviour regulation EFs. This prediction is based on the wider literature, which finds that behavioural regulation emerges earlier than meta-cognition in development.

  3. 3. Does language correlate with the BRIEF? We predict, based on consideration of the literature outlined previously, that a correlation exists, but we do not predict which direction of influence will be primary. In order to explore the contribution of EF and language to development, we ask further questions connected to the specific roles of meta-cognition versus behavioural regulation and different aspects of language.

3. Methodology

3.1. Participants

A total of 208 children from the United Kingdom and Ireland took part in this study of which 89 were deaf children and 119 were typical hearing children. All children were recruited through schools. In this study, the term ‘deaf’ means mild to profound hearing loss (range 26–91 dB). All included children fell into these categories.

Children with professionally diagnosed additional disabilities were not included in the current study. As the two groups differed significantly in non-verbal intelligence, deaf children with a score below the 10th percentile on non-verbal intelligence were also excluded. The deaf sample averaged 8 years and 9 months of age (SD = 1;9; range = 6;9–11;10; expressed as “years;months”) when parents completed the forms (see Table 1 and Table 2 for descriptive statistics). Analysed by ethnicity, 78% of deaf children were White British followed by 10% Asian, 5% Black and 7% from other backgrounds or where ethnicity was not reported. This is representative of the UK population. Concerning the socioeconomic background of the families, parents of deaf children worked mainly in skilled (36%) or semi-skilled jobs (36%), while a smaller percentage of them were unskilled workers (28%).

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sample

Table 2. Audiological and linguistic characteristics of deaf group

Hearing children averaged 8 years and 9 months of age (SD = 1;5; range = 6;0–11;11). In the hearing sample, children were White British (88%), 3% Asian, 3% Black and 5% from other backgrounds or backgrounds that were not reported. This is representative of the U.K. population. Parents of hearing children worked in skilled jobs (43%) followed by semi-skilled (29%) and unskilled jobs (28%). To control for socioeconomic background, hearing children were enrolled at the same schools as deaf participants. Participants attended several different schools in urban (58%) and rural (42%) settings. In order to obtain a representative sample, deaf children studied in mainstream educational settings with specialist classrooms/units (37%) or without such resources (37%) and 25% were enrolled in deaf only specialist schools.

The deaf group was divided in two groups according to their language characteristics: deaf children who used British Sign Language or Sign Supported English (BSL/SSE; n = 49, M age = 8;10, SD = 1;9) and deaf children who used spoken English (n = 40, M age = 8;6, SD = 1;8). The BSL/SSE group consisted of 49 children of which 19% were native signers. Most of the BSL/SSE children had profound hearing loss (69%) and only a small proportion had severe (17%) or mild/moderate hearing loss (14%). Regarding deaf children who used spoken English, 42% of them had severe hearing loss followed by 38% with profound hearing loss and 20% with mild/moderate hearing loss (see Table S2 in the Supplementary Material).

We then compared the specific group of deaf children with deaf parents. The results showed a significant effect of early language exposure in language and EF scores. We thus report this comparison in Section 4.

3.2. Measures

3.2.1. Executive functions

The BRIEF questionnaire (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000) was filled out by parents. The BRIEF is designed for school-aged children, and has been widely used with children with language difficulties such as deaf children (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2018; Hintermair, Reference Hintermair2013; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Beer, Castellanos, Pisoni and Miyamoto2014a). Through this checklist, parents indicate how often (never, sometimes or often) a child displays various behaviours over the past 6 months. The BRIEF contains 86 items divided into eight clinical scales. Five scales (Initiate, Plan/Organize, Working memory, Organization of materials and Monitor) form the Meta-cognition Index. Three scales (Inhibit, Shift and Emotional control) comprise the Behavioural Regulation Index. Both the Behaviour Regulation Index (raw scores range 28–84) and the Meta-cognition Index (raw scores range 44–132) form the Global Executive Composite (raw scores range 72–216). Internal consistency of the BRIEF is excellent for the Meta-cognition Index (Cronbach’s alpha of. 96 for clinical populations and. 94 for typical populations), the Behavioural Regulation Index (Cronbach’s alpha of. 96 for clinical populations and typical populations) and the Global Executive Composite (Cronbach’s alpha of. 98 for clinical populations and. 97 for typical populations). Forms were scored according to the instructions in the manual and raw scores were converted into T-scores which higher values indicate poor child functioning. A T-score of 50 represents the mean of the T-score distribution, while a T-score of 65 represents the point 1.5 standard deviation above the mean. T-scores at or above 65 are considered as having potential clinical significance (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000). T-scores range between 30 and 101.

3.2.2. Language

The LPP-2 was filled out by parents. It is a rating scale developed to assess language and communication skills across all communication modes (such as sign language, gestures and/or oral language). Thus, the LPP-2 was designed specifically for use with deaf children with consideration of the complexity and heterogeneity of their expressive communication styles (Bebko et al., Reference Bebko, Calderon and Treder2003). Despite this, the LPP-2 can also be used in hearing children demonstrating good construct and acceptable concurrent validity with other language measures. The LPP-2 is based on Bloom and Lahey’s (Reference Bloom and Lahey1978) model of language development and thus assesses five domains of children’s expressive language and communication skills: content, form, use, cohesion and reference. The LPP-2 contains 56 items, and each item was scored as follows: two points are given if the child is either past that skill level or currently has the skill, one point when the skill is currently emerging and zero point if either the child does not acquire the skill or the parent does not know the level of mastery the child has achieved. Up to 18 points can be obtained for form, 24 for content, 22 for reference, 22 for cohesion and 26 for use (total maximum score is 112 points). Both the BRIEF and LPP questionnaires were completed 1 week after parental consent was given to join the study, at the same time as the experimenter collected the non-verbal IQ data.

3.2.3. Non-verbal intelligence

The matrix reasoning subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) was administered as a control measure for nonverbal cognitive ability. The child is presented with a pattern with a missing section and must select the correct response from five choices. After 4/5 successive incorrect answers, the test is terminated. This subtest yields a T-score with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 (T-scores range 20–80). The matrix reasoning has been shown to demonstrate good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha reliability of. 87).

3.3. Procedure

The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee. Informed written consent was obtained from all parents. Children gave verbal consent at the time of testing and were informed that they could opt out at any time. Researchers recommended that parents complete LPP-2 and BRIEF forms in a quiet setting within a week of receiving them. The completion of the forms took 15 min each. Once forms were completed and returned by one of the parents, children were evaluated with the WASI in a quiet room at school or at the child’s home.

3.4. Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS 26.0. First, descriptive statistics were computed and then group differences were analysed using Welch’s t tests as the deaf and the hearing group had unequal variances (data are available at https://osf.io/srauf/?view_only=f41142d74a844c0ba35b1920cbc5c8a0). The relative risks of clinically significant scores of deaf participants relative to hearing controls and the expected rates according to a normal distribution are reported. For the latter, a hypothetical sample of the same size as the deaf groups and subgroups was created. The clinically significant scores expected according to the normal distribution were calculated by multiplying the sample size of each group by 6.7% and the resulting values were rounded to the nearest integer.

Pearson’s correlations were employed, and the effect sizes were set at. 10 (small effect),. 30 (medium effect) and. 50 (large effect). The statistical significance was set at p < .05. In addition, we applied the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure with a false discovery rate of 0.05 to reduce the familywise error rate. Multicollinearity was not present as tolerance values ranged between. 50 and. 98, and all variance inflation factors ranged between 1.00 and 2.00.

3.4.1. Subdivision of the deaf group by language use

There was a possibility that the group of DHH children were too heterogeneous in respect of language and EF skills to be treated as one group. Therefore, an analysis was carried out by comparing subgroups of deaf children. The group of deaf children was divided according to their linguistic characteristics: deaf children who used BSL/SSE and deaf children who used spoken English. Means, standard deviations and statistical differences are shown in Table S2 in the Supplementary Material.

The analysis of subgroups of deaf children showed that there were no significant differences in the global BRIEF score. Regarding the rates of elevated scores, both subgroups of deaf children were at significantly greater risk relative to the hearing group and the normal distribution (BSL/SSE children: RR vs. hearing group = 4.34; 95% CI = 1.95, 9.67 and RR vs. normal distribution = 4.67; 95% CI = 1.43, 15.20; spoken English children: RR vs. hearing group = 5.58; 95% CI = 2.56, 12.17 and RR vs. normal distribution = 3.94; 95% CI = 1.18, 13.11). Furthermore, the analysis of sub-parts of BRIEF revealed no differences in behavioural regulation or meta-cognition scores. Relative risk ratios of behavioural regulation index indicated again that both subgroups of deaf children were at significantly greater risk of having elevated scores (BSL/SSE children: RR vs. hearing group = 2.67; 95% CI = 1.36, 5.25 and RR vs. normal distribution = 4.67; 95% CI = 1.43, 15.20; spoken English children: RR vs. hearing group = 2.98; 95% CI = 1.51, 5.87 and RR vs. normal distribution = 4.66; 95% CI = 1.43, 15.15). All subgroups of deaf children were also at significantly greater risk of meta-cognition difficulties in comparison to the hearing group and the normal distribution (BSL/SSE children: RR vs. hearing group = 4.65; 95% CI = 2.11, 10.24 and RR versus normal distribution = 5.00; 95% CI = 1.55, 16.16; spoken English children: RR vs. hearing group = 4.09; 95% CI = 1.77, 9.45 and RR vs. normal distribution = 3.94; 95% CI = 1.18, 13.11). We subdivided the deaf group into a small number of deaf children with deaf parents and compared their language and EF scores with the hearing peers and deaf children with hearing parents. The risk ratio of deaf signers indicate that they are as likely as hearing participants to have T-scores at or above 65 in meta-cognition (RR vs. hearing group = 1.06; 95% CI = 0.14, 7.88 and RR vs. normal distribution = 1.00; 95% CI = 0.07, 14.45). Risk ratios in behavioural regulation and global BRIEF scores cannot be calculated because no children scored within the clinically significant range on these scales. However, those participants with late language exposure were at significantly greater risk of global EF difficulties in comparison with the hearing group and the normal distribution (RR vs. hearing group = 5.03; 95% CI = 2.39, 10.55 and RR vs. normal distribution = 8.04; 95% CI = 3.22, 20.08), as well as, behavioural regulation (RR vs. hearing group = 3.22; 95% CI = 1.77, 5.86 and RR vs. normal distribution = 1.00; 95% CI = 0.07, 14.45) and meta-cognition difficulties (RR vs. hearing group = 5.23; 95% CI = 2.50, 10.93 and RR vs. normal distribution = 8.36; 95% CI = 3.36, 20.82).

3.4.2. Subdivision the deaf group by age band

Another possibility was that age played a role in results and the sample could be divided into age groups. Additional analysis was conducted in order to study the effect of age on language and EF skills. Deaf and hearing groups were divided into three subgroups: from 6 to 7 years (young deaf group: n = 33, M age = 6;10, SD = 0;7; young hearing group: n = 38, M age = 7;2, SD = 0;2), from 8 to 9 years (middle-aged deaf children: n = 33, M age = 8;11, SD = 0;9; middle-aged hearing children: n = 59, M age = 9;0, SD = 0;8) and from 10 to 11 years (deaf pre-adolescents: n = 23, M age = 10;11, SD = 0;7; hearing pre-adolescents: n = 25; M age = 10;10, SD = 0;7). The analysis revealed a non-significant effect of age on language summaries, for deaf and hearing subgroups (see Table S3 in the Supplementary Material). Thus, in the following sets of analyses we do not sub-divide by age groups.

3.4.3. Missing data

The original sample included 208 children; however, some missing data were detected. Low levels of missing data occurred primarily on the language variables. The missing rates for the language dimensions ranged from 2.8 to 4.3% in the overall sample, (5.6–7.8% in the case of deaf sample and 0.8–2.5% in the case of hearing sample), while the missing data for the BRIEF variables were only 0.5% (1.1% in the deaf sample and no missing data in the hearing sample).

4. Results

4.1. Preliminary analysis

Descriptive statistics and group differences in language are depicted in Table 3 and show that hearing children outperformed deaf children on LPP scores. Group differences were found in each language domain, and they favoured the hearing children.

Table 3. Means and standard deviations of BRIEF and LPP-2 by group and Welch’s t-tests between both groups

Note: BRIEF and LPP-2 variables are expressed in T-scores and raw scores, respectively. Higher T-scores on BRIEF reflect increased incidence of problematic behaviour, while higher scores on LPP-2 reflect better language skills.

Abbreviations: BRI, Behavioural Regulation Index; GEC, Global Executive Composite; LPP, Language Proficiency Profile; MI, Meta-cognition Index.

Research question 1a: Do deaf and hearing children differ on EF behaviours as reported by the BRIEF?

According to BRIEF global scores, deaf children showed significantly lower EF capabilities. Following previous studies, we analysed clinically significant scores in the deaf group compared to the hearing group, that is, scores at or above 65. Table 4 reports the percentage of children in each group that fell within the clinically significant range on each BRIEF index and the relative risk ratios for the deaf group. When the confidence intervals are above one, risk ratios are considered significant. Deaf children were 4.77 times more likely than those with typical hearing to be at clinical risk for EF capabilities. When the relative risk was calculated according to the normal distribution, the relative risk for EF was also greater in the deaf group.

Table 4. Relative risk for each scale and subscale of the BRIEF

Note: BRI, Behavioural Regulation Index; GEC, Global Executive Composite; LPP, Language Proficient Profile; MI, Meta-cognition Index.

We also analysed the effect of early language experience by comparing deaf native signers and the hearing group with those deaf participants whose language (signed or spoken) was not acquired early (see Table S4 in the Supplementary Material). The results showed a significant effect of early language exposure in language and EF scores. Post hoc comparisons revealed that both the hearing group and the native signers group obtained similar language scores, while the deaf participants with late language exposure exhibited more language difficulties than both hearing (p < .001) and deaf signers group (p = .005).

Research question 1b: Are there differences between deaf and hearing children within sub-parts of the BRIEF related to meta-cognition versus behavioural regulation?

Deaf children displayed more difficulties than hearing children in both behavioural regulation and meta-cognition sub-parts. However, in one area of meta-cognition: the organization of materials, there was no difference between groups (see Table 4).

Clinical risk on EF items was very low in the hearing sample. The relative risk for EF was greater in the deaf group (relative risk of 3.05 for behavioural regulation EF and 5.15 for meta-cognition EF). The deaf group obtained the highest risk ratios in the meta-cognition subscales of ‘monitor’ and ‘initiate’ when compared with the hearing group. However, when the scores of the deaf group are compared with the normal distribution, the highest risk ratios were found in the behavioural regulation subscales.

When deaf native signers and the other deaf children were compared (see Table S4 in the Supplementary Material), the deaf participants with late language exposure obtained poorer scores in behavioural regulation and global BRIEF than hearing (behavioural regulation: p < .001; global BRIEF: p < .001) and deaf signers group (behavioural regulation: p = .001; global BRIEF: p = .006). In the case of meta-cognition, the hearing group and the deaf native signers group obtained a similar performance. Deaf children with late language exposure had higher scores for EF problems than the hearing group (p < .001).

Research question 2a: Does global language score on the LPP correlate with global EF scores on the BRIEF?

Pearson’s correlations were performed in each group (see Table S5 in the Supplementary Material for r values). Given the exploratory nature of the correlation analyses, Table S5 in the Supplementary Material indicates both uncorrected statistically significant correlations and ones that were robust to the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure. The deaf group showed significant correlations between global BRIEF scores and total LPP-2 scores, while no significant correlations were found in the hearing group. When the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure was applied, no significant correlations were found between global BRIEF and total LPP-2 scores.

Research question 2b: Are there associations between language and sub-parts of the BRIEF?

The correlations between total LPP-2 scores and behavioural regulation and meta-cognition showed a different pattern between groups. Meta-cognition was linked to language in deaf children, while no correlations were found between sub-parts of the BRIEF and language for hearing children (see Table S5 in the Supplementary Material). This correlation in the deaf group remained significant after the Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment.

Research question 2c: Are there associations between language domains and the BRIEF?

In the deaf group, the results showed that the LPP-2 variable ‘language use’ was closely linked to meta-cognition, even after Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment (see Table S5 in the Supplementary Material). Turning to the hearing group, we only observed significant correlations between the global BRIEF scores and LPP-2 for language reference (r = −.22; p = .018). Language reference was also significantly related with the behavioural regulation EF index (r = −.25; p = .007). These significant correlations in the hearing group also remained significant after Benjamini–Hochberg adjustment.

5. Discussion

This study investigated the relationship between EF and language skills in a large sample of deaf and hearing children from 6 to 12 years old. The results revealed that when examining EF and language with parent-report tools, we find different relationships between the various EF subdomains for deaf than we find for hearing children of the same age. In the deaf native signers, we find protected EF and language development. Hearing parents of deaf children reported greater difficulties with EF in daily activities than parents of hearing children. Moreover, correlation analyses showed significant associations between EF and language only for the deaf group. In the case of the hearing group, this only occurred between EF and one specific LPP subscale: the control of reference. We had a small group of deaf children with deaf parents who typically would have better language and EF abilities (Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Carrigan, Walker and Coppola2022; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2017, Reference Hall, Eigsti, Bortfeld and Lillo-Martin2018; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Jones, Denmark, Mason, Atkinson, Botting and Morgan2015) and this was borne out in our results. We discuss these findings in more detail in particular how this is relevant to the ongoing debates about the relationship between EF and language in deaf children in the following sections.

The first set of research questions concerned a comparison of EF abilities as measured by the BRIEF. As a group (all deaf children together), the deaf children show more than four times as many clinical concerns for EF development than hearing children of the same age. In a novel step, we separated out the BRIEF items into behavioural regulation and meta-cognitive abilities (Brock et al., Reference Brock, Rimm-Kaufman, Nathanson and Grimm2009). There is some suggestion that typically developing children develop inhibition or regulation of emotion before the meta-cognitive components of working memory or planning (Best & Miller, Reference Best and Miller2010; Kochanska & Aksan, Reference Kochanska and Aksan2006). There is mixed information on this topic in previous studies of deaf children, as we reviewed in Table S1 in the Supplementary Material. When we separated out different domains of EF from the BRIEF data, we observed heightened concerns for the deaf group with hearing parents on meta-cognition. Meta-cognition EF items in the BRIEF involve behaviours that demand concentration to plan a sequence of actions during problem-solving (e.g., a child planning what to put in a bag for a school trip tomorrow; Diamond, Reference Diamond2012). These meta-cognition skills typically develop later in childhood and are required for the academic development of children (Fernández García et al., Reference Fernández García, Merchán, Phillips-Silver and Daza González2021) for example to solve abstract problems with minimal affective or motivational content. There are many implications of this heightened difficulty with meta-cognition for the continued academic gap (especially in literacy and numeracy) seen between deaf and hearing children; Marschark & Knoors, Reference Marschark and Knoors2020).

Turning to the relationship between EF and language. The LPP-2 language scores were correlated with global EF level but only in the deaf group. Both behaviour-regulation and meta-cognitive sub-components of EF were correlated with language in the deaf children. While some research observes a link between language and EF in young (pre-school) hearing children (Blain-Brière et al., Reference Blain-Brière, Bouchard and Bigras2014; Gandolfi & Viterbori, Reference Gandolfi and Viterbori2020), our study suggests the link between language and EF continues to exist in much older deaf children. Studies of the development of meta-cognition propose an extended period of growth (Luria, Reference Luria1959, Reference Luria1961; Vygotsky, Reference Vygotsky, Vygotsky, Hanfmann and Vakar1962, Reference Vygotsky, Riewer and Carton1987). Recently, Zelazo (Reference Zelazo2015) has proposed that language, via private speech, in older children acts as a meta-cognitive tool during EF tasks (Müller et al., Reference Müller, Zelazo, Hood, Leone and Rohrer2004). In a few studies of this question in hearing children with language delays, meta-cognition was also affected by difficulties with implementing private speech (Lidstone et al., Reference Lidstone, Meins and Fernyhough2011; Vissers et al., Reference Vissers, Tomas and Law2020).

The proposal here is that deaf children with delayed language are also more prone to low scores on measures of both behaviour regulation and more so meta-cognition. This is because of how these EFs are related to early social-communication in development (Gandolfi et al., Reference Gandolfi, Viterbori, Traverso and Carmen Usai2014; Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Willoughby, Vernon-Feagans, Blair, Cox, Burchinal, Burton, Crnic, Crouter, Garrett-Peters, Greenberg, Lanza, Mills-Koonce, Werner and Willoughby2016; Miller & Marcovitch, Reference Miller and Marcovitch2015; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Jones, Botting and Morgan2020). At 2 years of age, children begin to use private speech to self-regulate through intensive social and emotional interactions with parents, so language becomes a key mechanism of self-regulation (Luria, Reference Luria1959, Reference Luria1961; Vygotsky, Reference Vygotsky, Vygotsky, Hanfmann and Vakar1962, Reference Vygotsky, Riewer and Carton1987). We now return to the discussion of family environment and how this influences the growth of EF (Hughes & Ensor, Reference Hughes and Ensor2009; Peterson & Welsh, Reference Peterson, Welsh, Goldstein and Naglieri2014; Wolfe & Bell, Reference Wolfe and Bell2007). In the early years parent–child, as well as, peer interactions provide opportunities to observe and extract models of appropriate behaviours and participate in conversational exchanges (Jung & Short, Reference Jung and Short2002). Furthermore, parents modulate children’s behaviour and help them to regulate their emotions (Alamos et al., Reference Alamos, Williford, Downer and Turnbull2022). The behavioural regulation aspects of EF are thus functioning before typically developing hearing children reach 6 years old and in deaf children with deaf parents. The deaf children with deaf parents had early and accessible communicative experiences and thus develop both behaviour regulation and meta-cognitive abilities appropriately. In contrast, the vast majority of deaf infants grow up in hearing families who typically use spoken language that the child finds difficult to follow. Thus, they miss out on many of these daily conversations involving emotional and behaviour regulation (Rieffe, Reference Rieffe2012). In terms of the establishment of behavioural regulation, early social interactions may act as a type of ‘experience-expectant’ input for later EFs and language development (Thompson & Steinbeis, Reference Thompson and Steinbeis2020).

Turning to the meta-cognitive aspects of the BRIEF, language, via private speech in older hearing children acts as a meta-cognitive tool during EF tasks (Müller et al., Reference Müller, Zelazo, Hood, Leone and Rohrer2004). If deaf children are still in the process of establishing self-regulation this may delay the connection of private speech during meta-cognitive tasks (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2015). On our LPP-2 measure, this is seen most clearly in the use items.

We were interested in specific language abilities and whether these were related to EF in different ways. We found that language use and language form were associated with meta-cognition EF skills in deaf children and the control of reference (e.g., the planning of narrative language) in hearing children, respectively. In terms of language use, those deaf children with better meta-cognition EF were reported on the LPP-2 measure to engage and maintain a conversation for longer and in more diverse contexts. Language use is linked to EF through the need to hold in mind and update linguistic and contextual information and to think ahead to what will be communicated next (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Biney and Abbot-Smith2018). Therefore, using more advanced language relies on strong EFs especially enabling children to focus attention, handle multiple sources of information simultaneously and analyse meaning in complex language (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Weiland et al., Reference Weiland, Barata and Yoshikawa2014).

The second part of the explanation for an EF influence over language in deaf children may relate to how we measured EF and language in the current study compared to previous experimental methods. Using measures of the real-life implementation of EF and language use, we have uncovered a different characterisation of the EF-language relationship than previously reported in the literature (Botting et al., Reference Botting, Jones, Marshall, Denmark, Atkinson and Morgan2017; Figueras et al., Reference Figueras, Edwards and Langdon2008; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Atkinson, Marshall, Botting, St Clair and Morgan2020). In addition, the LPP is a broader assessment of language and reflects communication and social interaction related to daily conversations and pragmatics. Thus, EF ability is more salient in real-world scenarios involving deaf children having to follow and use language appropriately. There have been similar reports of this direction of association in deaf children based on questionnaire measures (Hintermair, Reference Hintermair2013).

5.1. Limitations

An obvious limitation of this study is that all the analyses reported were based on cross-sectional data. The current study assessed EF and language skills with parent-reports. Despite the ecological validity stemming from parent-reports, these measurements are more susceptible to bias than experimental EF tasks (Robson et al., Reference Robson, Allen and Howard2020). For example, parents who know their children have language difficulties might be more prone to rate their child as having poorer regulation or vice versa. Furthermore, the LPP-2 has particular strengths and weaknesses which should be considered. It is ecologically valid, feasibly completed by non-specialists (e.g., hearing parents), and can be uniformly applied across all languages and modalities. However, its psychometric properties are not well-established and it also lacks age-based norms. Our study did not find an effect of age on language scores. However, this does not imply age is not important for language development. Taking a broader view than just vocabulary, the relationship between language and age is complex. The LPP has a high pragmatic load, which may contribute to a lower-than-expected scores in the deaf sample. In addition, at a certain age, the LPP may not be sensitive to change and thus reaching the ceiling effect with the hearing sample. Also, while, the LPP-2 has good construct validity and marginally acceptable concurrent validity with other language tests, no information about reliability is available. Nonetheless, the EF and LPP-2 have been shown to be an appropriately sensitive tool for clinical and research studies in children (Bebko et al., Reference Bebko, Calderon and Treder2003; Hintermair, Reference Hintermair2013). Lastly, while we argue early social-interaction explains the EF-language relationship, future research needs to directly measure early communicative experiences in deaf children and follow longitudinally the impacts of early EF variability on future language components.

5.2. Conclusions and implications

The current study is important since, given the heterogeneity and representativeness of the sample, it reinforces the previous literature and highlights the difficulties deaf children have in areas of regulation and meta-cognition in real-life situations. The finding of EF delays in deaf children is in line with previous studies based on parent-report questionnaires (e.g., Hintermair, Reference Hintermair2013; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Beer, Castellanos, Pisoni and Miyamoto2014a) and experimental tasks (Botting et al., Reference Botting, Jones, Marshall, Denmark, Atkinson and Morgan2017; Kronenberger et al., Reference Kronenberger, Colson, Henning and Pisoni2014b). Deaf children are more than four times more likely to have a clinical concern for EF development for both behavioural regulation and the majority of meta-cognitive aspects of EF. Meta-cognition EF components and language are more closely related for deaf children. Our results have implications for clinical practice. First, in order to see strengths and weaknesses better, it is necessary to evaluate EF and language in multidimensional ways (Fernández García et al., Reference Fernández García, Merchán, Phillips-Silver and Daza González2021; Shokrkon & Nicoladis, Reference Shokrkon and Nicoladis2022). All children depend on good meta-cognition EF and language to enter into essential learning activities in a school environment and to facilitate coordinated play (Yogman et al., Reference Yogman, Garner, Hutchinson, Hirsh-Pasek and Golinkoff2018). Thus, difficulties in EF have been related to a wide range of effects in both the home and school environment (Ching et al., Reference Ching, Cupples, Leigh, Hou and Wong2021; Snyder et al., Reference Snyder, Prichard, Schrepferman, Patrick and Stoolmiller2004). The real-world assessment of EF and language carried out in the present study may assist clinical and educational specialists when guiding parents in intervention programs in which the context is considered. These interventions could foster better promotion of language and EF in learning situations. Given the strong relationships between language and EF in both deaf and typically developing children, early difficulties in EF may serve as a warning sign for later difficulties in the language domain. Early assessment of these neuropsychological aspects is essential to detect and prevent difficulties in both skills, as well as to define linguistic profiles in deaf children.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2024.10.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available through the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/srauf/?view_only=f41142d74a844c0ba35b1920cbc5c8a0.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all the children, families and schools who took part in this study.

Funding statement

The research was funded by the ESRC (Grant No. 620–28-600 Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre). The work of the first author was supported by the European Union-Next Generation EU (Margarita Salas modality).

Competing interest

The authors declare none.

References

Alamos, P., Williford, A. P., Downer, J. T., & Turnbull, K. L. P. (2022). How does inhibitory control predict emotion regulation in preschool? The role of individual children’s interactions with teachers and peers. Developmental Psychology, 11, 20492063. https://doi.org/10.1037/DEV0001415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bebko, J. M., Calderon, R., & Treder, R. (2003). The Language Proficiency Profile-2: Assessment of the global communication skills of deaf children across languages and modalities of expression. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8(4), 438451. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/eng034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bebko, J. M., & McKinnon, E. E. (1998). Assessing pragmatic language skills in deaf children: The Language Proficiency Profile. In Marschark, M. & Clark, M. D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness (1st ed., Vol. 2, pp. 243264). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Beer, J., Kronenberger, W. G., Castellanos, I., Colson, B. G., Henning, S. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (2014). Executive functioning skills in preschool-age children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(4), 15211534. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-H-13-0054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beer, J., Kronenberger, W. G., & Pisoni, D. B. (2011). Executive function in everyday life: Implications for young cochlear implant users. Cochlear Implants International, 12(Suppl 1), S89S91. https://doi.org/10.1179/146701011X13001035752570CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 16411660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180200. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.DR.2009.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., Nation, K., & Patterson, K. (2014). When words fail us: Insights into language processing from developmental and acquired disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20120403Placeholder Text. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blain-Brière, B., Bouchard, C., & Bigras, N. (2014). The role of executive functions in the pragmatic skills of children age 4-5. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 240. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00240Google ScholarPubMed
Blank, A., & Holt, R. F. (2022). Associations between paternal play and executive function in deaf and hard of hearing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 65(8), 30563078. https://doi.org/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00661CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E., & Boerma, T. (2020). Do children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) have difficulties with interference control, visuospatial working memory, and selective attention? Developmental patterns and the role of severity and persistence of DLD. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(9), 30363050. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders (2nd ed.). Wiley.Google Scholar
Bohlmann, N. L., Maier, M. F., & Palacios, N. (2015). Bidirectionality in self-regulation and expressive vocabulary: Comparisons between monolingual and dual language learners in preschool. Child Development, 86(4), 10941111. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., Jones, A., Marshall, C., Denmark, T., Atkinson, J., & Morgan, G. (2017). Nonverbal executive function is mediated by language: A study of deaf and hearing children. Child Development, 88(5), 16891700. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12659CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grimm, K. J. (2009). The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24(3), 337349. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ECRESQ.2009.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2010). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26(2), 571593. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN2602_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (2009). Language and core cognition. The origin of concepts, pp. 247285. https://doi.org/10.1093/ACPROF:OSO/9780195367638.003.0007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M. (2003). Executive function in context: Development, measurement, theory, and experience. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 138151. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1540-5834.2003.06803012.XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 489510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ching, T. Y. C., Cupples, L., Leigh, G., Hou, S., & Wong, A. (2021). Predicting quality of life and behavior and emotion from functional auditory and pragmatic language abilities in 9-year-old deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(22), 5357. https://doi.org/10.3390/JCM10225357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’souza, D., D’souza, H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2017). Precursors to language development in typically & atypically developing infants & toddlers: The importance of embracing complexity. Journal of Child Language, 44(3), 591627. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500091700006XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In Stuss, D. & Knight, R. (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466503). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2012). The early development of executive functions. In Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169539.003.0006Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S. (1999). The development of children ages 6 to 14. Future of Children, 9(2), 3044. https://doi.org/10.2307/1602703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 65, 4959. https://doi.org/10.1093/BMB/65.1.49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández García, L., Merchán, A., Phillips-Silver, J., & Daza González, M. T. (2021). Neuropsychological development of cool and hot executive functions between 6 and 12 years of age: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 687337. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.687337CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueras, B., Edwards, L., & Langdon, D. (2008). Executive function and language in deaf children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(3), 362377. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enm067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueroa, M., Silvestre, N., & Darbra, S. (2022). Specific EF-related tasks and reading in adolescents with typical hearing or a cochlear implant. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 43(3), 152162. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740120976109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funahashi, S. (2001). Neuronal mechanisms of executive control by the prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience Research, 39(2), 147165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00224-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gandolfi, E., & Viterbori, P. (2020). Inhibitory control skills and language acquisition in toddlers and preschool children. Language Learning, 70(3), 604642. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gandolfi, E., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., & Carmen Usai, M. (2014). Inhibitory processes in toddlers: A latent-variable approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 381. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2014.00381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). BRIEF: Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C., Carrigan, E., Walker, K., & Coppola, M. (2022). Language not auditory experience is related to parent-reported executive functioning in preschool-aged deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Child Development, 93(1), 209224. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guardino, C., & Antia, S. D. (2012). Modifying the classroom environment to increase engagement and decrease disruption with students who are deaf or hard of hearing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 17(4), 518533. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ens026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guare, R. (2014). Context in the development of executive functions in children. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3(3), 226232. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.870015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, M. L., Eigsti, I.-M., Bortfeld, H., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2017). Auditory deprivation does not impair executive function, but language deprivation might: Evidence from a parent-report measure in deaf native signing children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 22(1), 921. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enw054CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, M. L., Eigsti, I.-M., Bortfeld, H., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2018). Executive function in deaf children: Auditory access and language access. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(8), 19701988. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintermair, M. (2013). Executive functions and behavioral problems in deaf and hard-of-hearing students at general and special schools. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18(3), 344359. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/ent003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 617644. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, C. J., Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., & Yarger, R. S. (2004). Adolescents’ performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: Implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 11481158. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. A. (2009). How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function? New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2009(123), 3550. https://doi.org/10.1002/CD.234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., White, N., & Ensor, R. (2014). How does talk about thoughts, desires, and feelings foster children’s socio-cognitive development? Mediators, moderators and implications for intervention. In Lagattuta, K. H. (Ed.), Children and emotion. New insights into developmental affective sciences. Contributions to human development (Vol. 26, pp. 95105. KargerPlaceholder Text. https://doi.org/10.1159/000354362Google Scholar
Isquith, P. K., Roth, R. M., & Gioia, G. (2013). Contribution of rating scales to the assessment of executive functions. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 2(2), 125132. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.748389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A., Atkinson, J., Marshall, C., Botting, N., St Clair, M. C., & Morgan, G. (2020). Expressive vocabulary predicts nonverbal executive function: A 2-year longitudinal study of deaf and hearing children. Child Development, 91(2), 400414. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13226CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, V., & Short, R. H. (2002). Integrated perspective of evolving intrapsychic and person-environment functions: Implications for deaf and hard of hearing individuals. American Annals of the Deaf, 147(3), 2634. https://doi.org/10.1353/AAD.2012.0201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 148157. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00275-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2006). Children’s conscience and self-regulation. Journal of Personality, 67(2), 490507. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00421.xGoogle Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (2002). Commentary: The codevelopments of attention and emotion regulation. Infancy, 3(2), 199208. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0302_5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronenberger, W. G., Beer, J., Castellanos, I., Pisoni, D. B., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2014a). Neurocognitive risk in children with cochlear implants. JAMA Otolaryngology–Head & Neck Surgery, 140(7), 608615. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaoto.2014.757CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronenberger, W. G., Colson, B. G., Henning, S. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (2014b). Executive functioning and speech-language skills following long-term use of cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19(4), 456470. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronenberger, W. G., Xu, H., & Pisoni, D. B. (2020). Longitudinal development of executive functioning and spoken language skills in preschool-aged children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(4), 1128. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., Blair, C., Cox, M., Burchinal, P., Burton, L., Crnic, K., Crouter, A., Garrett-Peters, P., Greenberg, M., Lanza, S., Mills-Koonce, R., Werner, E., & Willoughby, M. (2016). The contribution of children’s time-specific and longitudinal expressive language skills on developmental trajectories of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 2034. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JECP.2016.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lensing, N., & Elsner, B. (2018). Development of hot and cool executive functions in middle childhood: Three-year growth curves of decision making and working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 187204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.04.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, C., Koyasu, M., Oh, S., Ogawa, A., Short, B., & Huang, Z. (2009). Culture, executive function, and social understanding. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2009(123), 6985. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.236CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lidstone, J., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2011). Individual differences in children’s private speech: Consistency across tasks, timepoints, and contexts. Cognitive Development, 26(3), 203213. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COGDEV.2011.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1959). The directive function of speech in development and dissolution. Word, 15(2), 341352. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1959.11659703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. Liveright.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., & Knoors, H. (2020). The oxford handbook of deaf studies in learning and cognition. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, C., Jones, A., Denmark, T., Mason, K., Atkinson, J., Botting, N., & Morgan, G. (2015). Deaf children’s non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 527. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2015.00527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2018). Individual differences in children’s pragmatic ability: A review of associations with formal language, social cognition, and executive functions. Language, Learning and Development, 14(3), 186223. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCoy, D. C. (2019). Measuring young children’s executive function and self-regulation in classrooms and other real-world settings. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 22(1), 6374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-019-00285-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCreery, R. W., & Walker, E. A. (2022). Variation in auditory experience affects language and executive function skills in children who are hard of hearing. Ear and Hearing, 43(2), 347360. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0000000000001098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, M. R., Giesbrecht, G. F., Müller, U., McInerney, R. J., & Kerns, K. A. (2012). A latent variable approach to determining the structure of executive function in preschool children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13(3), 395423. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.585478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, S. E., & Marcovitch, S. (2015). Examining executive function in the second year of life: Coherence, stability, and relations to joint attention and language. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 101114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 814. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, G., Curtin, M., & Botting, N. (2021). The interplay between early social interaction, language and executive function development in deaf and hearing infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 64, 101591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, G., Jones, A., & Botting, N. (2020). Links between language and cognitive development of deaf children. In Morgan, G. (Ed.), Understanding deafness, language and cognitive development: Essays in honour of Bencie Woll (pp. 115131). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/TILAR.25.07MORCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., Hood, S., Leone, T., & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75(5), 15941609. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-8624.2004.00759.XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 7177. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412436807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newbury, J., Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., & Moran, C. (2016). Interrelationships between working memory, processing speed, and language development in the age range 2–4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(5), 11461158. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0322CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nip, I. S. B., Green, J. R., & Marx, D. B. (2011). The co-emergence of cognition, language, and speech motor control in early development: A longitudinal correlation study. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(2), 149160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascual, A. C., Moyano, N., & Robres, A. Q. (2019). The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: Review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1582. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01582CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 530544. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0018287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perszyk, D. R., & Waxman, S. R. (2018). Linking language and cognition in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 231250. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011701CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, I. T., Bates, J. E., & Staples, A. D. (2014). The role of language ability and self-regulation in the development of inattentive-hyperactive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 96, 221237. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000698Google Scholar
Peterson, E., & Welsh, M. C. (2014). The development of hot and cool executive functions in childhood and adolescence: Are we getting warmer? In Goldstein, S. & Naglieri, J. (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 4565). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Remine, M. D., Care, E., & Brown, P. M. (2008). Language ability and verbal and nonverbal executive functioning in deaf students communicating in spoken English. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13(4), 531545. https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enn010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieffe, C. (2012). Awareness and regulation of emotions in deaf children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 477492. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02057.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robson, D. A., Allen, M. S., & Howard, S. J. (2020). Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 146(4), 324354. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romeo, R. R., Flournoy, J. C., McLaughlin, K. A., & Lengua, L. J. (2022). Language development as a mechanism linking socioeconomic status to executive functioning development in preschool. Developmental Science, 25, e13227. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, L. (2014). The associations between executive functions’ capacities, performance process skills, and dimensions of participation in activities of daily life among children of elementary school age. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 4(3), 148156. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.821652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shokrkon, A., & Nicoladis, E. (2022). The directionality of the relationship between executive functions and language skills: A literature review. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 848696. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2022.848696CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Sidera, F., Morgan, G., & Serrat, E. (2020). Understanding pretend emotions in children who are deaf and hard of hearing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 25(2), 141152. https://doi.org/10.1093/DEAFED/ENZ040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slot, P. L., & von Suchodoletz, A. (2018). Bidirectionality in preschool children’s executive functions and language skills: Is one developing skill the better predictor of the other? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42, 205214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, J., Prichard, J., Schrepferman, L., Patrick, M. R., & Stoolmiller, M. (2004). Child impulsiveness-inattention, early peer experiences, and the development of early onset conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(6), 579594. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JACP.0000047208.23845.64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, A., & Steinbeis, N. (2020). Sensitive periods in executive function development. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 98105. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COBEHA.2020.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonér, S., & Gerholm, T. N. (2021). Links between language and executive functions in Swedish preschool children: A pilot study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 42(1), 207241. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716420000703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(2), 131143. https://doi.org/10.1111/JCPP.12001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traverso, L., Viterbori, P., Gandolfi, E., Zanobini, M., & Usai, M. C. (2022). The contribution of inhibitory control to early literacy skills in 4‐ to 5‐year‐old children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 59, 265286. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ECRESQ.2021.11.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsermentseli, S., & Poland, S. (2016). Cool versus hot executive function: A new approach to executive function. Encephalos, 53, 1114.Google Scholar
Usai, M. C., Viterbori, P., Gandolfi, E., & Zanobini, M. (2020). The relationship between temperamental dimensions and inhibitory control in early childhood: Implications for language acquisition. Infant Behavior and Development, 61, 101495. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.INFBEH.2020.101495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Usai, M. C., Viterbori, P., Traverso, L., & De Franchis, V. (2014). Latent structure of executive function in five- and six-year-old children: A longitudinal study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(4), 447462. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2013.840578CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 6582. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JECP.2015.06.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vissers, C. T. W. M., Tomas, E., & Law, J. (2020). The emergence of inner speech and its measurement in atypically developing children. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 496530. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2020.00279/BIBTEXCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). The development of scientific concepts in childhood. In Vygotsky, L. S., Hanfmann, E., & Vakar, G. (Eds.), Thought and language (pp. 82118). MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Language and speech. In Riewer, R. W. & Carton, A. S. (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky. Volumen 1. Problems of general psychology (pp. 39285). Plenum.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weiland, C., Barata, M. C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2014). The co-occurring development of executive function skills and receptive vocabulary in preschool-aged children: A look at the direction of the developmental pathways. Infant and Child Development, 23(1), 421. https://doi.org/10.1002/ICD.1829CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 436452. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JECP.2010.08.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2007). The integration of cognition and emotion during infancy and early childhood: Regulatory processes associated with the development of working memory. Brain and Cognition, 65(1), 313. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BANDC.2006.01.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yogman, M., Garner, A., Hutchinson, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2018). The power of play: A pediatric role in enhancing development in young children. Pediatrics, 142(3), e20182058. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-2058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuile, A. R., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2021). Inhibitory control and preschoolers’ use of irregular past tense verbs. Journal of Child Language, 48(3), 480498. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 5568. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.DR.2015.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 354360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In Goswami, U. (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445469). Blackwell Publishers Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470996652.ch20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Audiological and linguistic characteristics of deaf group

Figure 2

Table 3. Means and standard deviations of BRIEF and LPP-2 by group and Welch’s t-tests between both groups

Figure 3

Table 4. Relative risk for each scale and subscale of the BRIEF

Supplementary material: File

Figueroa et al. supplementary material

Figueroa et al. supplementary material
Download Figueroa et al. supplementary material(File)
File 693.5 KB