Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T18:52:43.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does fruit and vegetable consumption impact mental health? Systematic review and meta-analyses of published controlled intervention studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 June 2023

Katherine M. Appleton*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
Lucy R. Boxall
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
Olabimpe Adenuga-Ajayi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
Dilara F. Seyar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Katherine Appleton, email k.appleton@bournemouth.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Associations between fruit and vegetable (FV) consumption and mental health are suggested, largely from observational studies. This systematic review aimed to identify and summarise all published controlled intervention studies investigating the effects of FV consumption on mental health in adults. Four academic databases (Medline, PsycINFO, PubMed, Web of Science) were searched on 16 September 2022, over all years, for studies that used an intervention design; included FV consumption; included an appropriate non-FV-consumption control; used a validated measure of mental health and were conducted in healthy adults or adults with solely a depressive or anxiety-related condition. Study details were tabulated and combined using meta-analyses. Risk of bias was assessed using the domains of the Cochrane Collaboration. Six studies, enrolling 691 healthy adults and reporting on one or more mental health outcomes, were found. Meta-analyses found small and imprecise effects of FV consumption for: psychological well-being (4 studies, 289 participants) standardised mean difference (SMD) = 0·07 (95 % CI −0·17, 0·30), P = 0·58, I2 = 0 %; depressive symptomology (3 studies, 271 participants) SMD = –0·15 (95 % CI −0·40, 0·10), P = 0·23, I2 = 47 % and anxiety-related symptomology (4 studies, 298 participants) SMD = –0·15 (95 % CI −0·39, 0·08), P = 0·20, I2 = 71 %. Some benefit for psychological well-being was found in change-from-baseline data: SMD = 0·28 (95 % CI 0·05, 0·52), P = 0·02, I2 = 0 %. Risk of bias was high in many studies. Limitations include the consideration only of published studies and stem from the studies found. Given the few, limited studies available and the small size of effects, stronger evidence is needed before recommending FV consumption for mental health.

Type
Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Fruit and/or vegetable (FV) consumption is robustly associated with reduced risk from a number of global physical health concerns, including cardiovascular disease, type II diabetes and obesity(Reference Aune, Giovannucci and Boffetta14). Recent reviews also suggest some benefits for mental or psychological health(Reference Aucoin, LaChance and Naidoo5Reference Tuck, Farrow and Thomas10), and while physical health conditions continue to result in the greatest number of years of life lost globally(11), depressive and anxiety disorders contribute increasingly to number of years lived with disability(11). Recent estimates suggest increases of 14·3 % and 12·8 % years lived with disability for depressive and anxiety-related disorders, respectively, since 2007, with depressive disorders ranked the third leading cause of years lived with disability in 2017(11).

Potential mechanisms for effects of FV consumption on mental health are available. First, some of the biological compounds present in FV are thought to have a direct impact on brain health and function, via roles in neurotransmitter synthesis, activities and reuptake(Reference Conner, Brookie and Richardson12Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14). Vitamin C and many of the B vitamins present in FV play a role in the production and activities of serotonin, dopamine and other monoamine neurotransmitters, known to be important in experiences of mood(Reference Conner, Brookie and Richardson12Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14). Second, some of the biological compounds in FV which impact physical health may also impact mental health(Reference Conner, Brookie and Richardson12Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14). Antioxidants, for example, such as vitamins E, C and serum carotenoids, are known to reduce oxidative stress and inflammation; activities associated with reduced depression and increased psychological well-being(Reference Conner, Brookie and Richardson12,Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14) . Other micronutrients, such as phytochemicals, polyphenols and flavonoids, and some minerals, for example, magnesium, have also been linked to mental state(Reference Mueller, Ganesh and Bonnes13,Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14) . Third, FV consumption may displace the consumption of other foods, resulting in the absence of compounds within the diet, such as saturated fats, that may be deleterious for physical and mental health(Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14Reference Ocean, Howley and Ensor16). Fourth, a more psychological route for the impacts of FV consumption on mental health can also be proposed, where FV consumption results in perceptions of self-care, healthiness or expectations of improved mental functioning(Reference Rooney, McKinley and Woodside14,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith, Stevenson and Dudley18) , which may then improve mental health. Finally, any combination of these suggested mechanisms may apply.

While benefits for mental health have been suggested however, reviews, to date, have focused almost solely on observational cross-sectional and prospective studies(Reference Aucoin, LaChance and Naidoo5Reference Tuck, Farrow and Thomas10). Using these study designs, direction of effect can only be suggested(Reference Conner, Brookie and Richardson12,Reference Mujcic and Oswald19,Reference White, Horwath and Conner20) , reverse causation remains a concern(Reference Boehm, Soo and Zevon21) and confounding or secondary variables, such as other dietary, health and lifestyle behaviours and various demographic characteristics, may explain effects(Reference Ocean, Howley and Ensor16,Reference Blanchflower, Oswald and Stewart-Brown22Reference Vermeulen-Smit, Ten Have and Van Laar26) . Reviews of FV consumption using observational studies, thus, may more accurately be described as reviews of the effects of a healthy diet or lifestyle which includes FV, rather than reviews of the effects of FV consumption per se. Isolation of the effects of FV consumption is important if FV are to be recommended for benefits for mental health.

This systematic review aimed to identify and summarise all published controlled intervention studies that investigated an effect of FV consumption, as a distinct intervention, on mental health, in adults.

Methodology

The review was conducted following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines(Reference Page, McKenzie and Bossyt27), with advice from the Cochrane Collaboration(Reference Higgins, Thomas and Chandler28). A protocol for the work was published following an initial run of all searches on the Open Science Framework (ID: https://osf.io/txcsm/; date of registration: 30.07.22). We adhered to our published protocol in all respects but extended our search terms from those detailed in the protocol, to also allow the capture of studies measuring ‘life satisfaction’ and ‘quality of life’ following recognition that these terms are often used to capture mental health in medical fields. Some additional refinements were also made throughout the review process as detailed below.

Searches

Electronic databases: Medline, PsycINFO, PubMed and Web of Science were searched over all years of records. Searches used the search terms (fruit* OR vegetable*) combined with (AND) (anxiety* OR depress* OR ‘mental health’ OR ‘mental disorders’ OR ‘well being’ OR wellbeing OR well-being OR ‘psychological health’ OR ‘life satisfaction’ OR ‘quality of life’), searched for in ‘title’ and ‘abstract’ fields. Phrases were identified as such as necessary in each database. Reference lists of identified papers and review articles were also searched for any papers that may have been missed by our database searches. Our intention was to find as many articles of relevance to our research aim as possible.

Study inclusion

Studies were included in the review if they were conducted in either healthy adults (aged 18–65 years) or in adults with solely a depressive or anxiety-related condition; included FV consumption as an exposure; included a measure of mental health, using a validated measure; used an intervention design with an appropriate non-FV consumption control and were published in English at the time of our searches.

Only studies in adults (aged 18–65 years) were included; studies in children and adolescents were excluded. Populations were required to be healthy, that is, were experiencing no major physical or mental health condition, with the exception that studies on individuals with a sole diagnosis of a depressive or anxiety-related condition were considered for inclusion, given the nature of our review. There were no exclusions based on participant sex, age within 18–65 years or years of condition, if appropriate. Studies were included regardless of the consumption of fruit, vegetables or fruit and vegetables together, to include also the consumption of individual fruits/vegetables and fruit and/or vegetable juices. We did not include studies where the intervention was a fruit/vegetable oil, extract, powder or component, for example, a vitamin, polyphenol or vegetable protein, or where the intervention was based on a fruit/vegetable with added items. To distinguish between fruit juices, powders and extracts, we included interventions that were commercially available or that we considered could be made from whole fruit in a home kitchen, for example, via cooking, while we excluded interventions that were custom-made or required technical processes, such as freeze-drying, or where the process was unclear. We also did not consider studies using herbs, other medicinal plants or supplements. Studies were excluded if the FV exposure was contaminated by the simultaneous consumption of other foods that may also impact physical and/or mental health, that were not also consumed by the control group; only studies that could ascertain an effect solely due to FV consumption were included. Similarly, we did not include studies using interventions based on dietary patterns high in FV, for example, the Mediterranean diet, nor did we include studies using interventions where FV consumption was combined with other lifestyle behaviours, such as physical activity. There were no exclusions based on specific FV consumed, type or length of intervention.

We defined mental health as a long-term state of positive well-being or freedom from negative symptomology(29). Studies were included in the review if validated measures were used to measure psychological well-being, depressive symptomology, anxiety-related symptomology, mental health or quality of life, as experiences that last over the long term. We did not consider measures of ‘mood’ or ‘emotion’, as these experiences are typically temporary, short-term and transitory in nature and may be, but are not necessarily, related to longer-term experiences. We also did not consider outcomes that, while psychological in part, may also be physical, such as ‘fatigue’ or ‘vitality’, nor did we consider outcomes that are psychological and may be linked to mental health but are not necessarily markers of good mental health, such as ‘self-efficacy’ or ‘creativity’. ‘Life satisfaction’ and ‘quality of life’ were considered as measures that capture mental or psychological health; however, we only included measures of general quality of life, and if possible we used mental health subscales; we did not consider studies where quality of life focused on physical symptoms or was measured only in relation to a specific physical condition. We considered only studies that used a controlled intervention design and included a non-FV-consuming control group that allowed assessment of the consumption of FV as a distinct intervention. We did not consider baseline assessments in a pre-post study design to provide sufficient control in this respect. There were no exclusions based on study setting, country or length of follow-up. Only studies that were published and published in English were considered.

Study selection

Searches were conducted by one reviewer (KMA), and search results were then screened independently by two reviewers (KMA, OAA and/or DFS) on title and abstract against our inclusion criteria. All articles of potential relevance were gained in full and screened independently by two reviewers (KMA, LRB, OAA and/or DFS).

Data extraction

Data were extracted and tabulated from all included studies by two independent reviewers (KMA, LRB, OAA and/or DFS), on: number of participants; participant details, including clinical profiles; FV intervention/exposure, including duration; comparator; outcomes assessed and risk of bias; and data reported, including authors conclusions. Discordances were resolved by discussion between reviewers.

Risk of bias

Risk of bias in each study was assessed using the domains and methods suggested by the Cochrane Collaboration(Reference Higgins, Thomas and Chandler28). The domains assessed were randomisation; allocation concealment; blinding of participants and researchers; use of intention-to-treat (ITT) analyses based on number randomised; degree of study non-completion; incomplete outcome reporting and other. Each domain was considered at ‘low’, ‘high’ or ‘unclear’ risk of bias, dependent on the study details as published, either in included papers or from papers detailing the original trial. For the domains randomisation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants and researchers and the use of ITT analyses, judgements of low risk were given where details of the methods used and no contra-indications were found, and judgements of high risk were given where contra-indications resulting in potential bias were found. For the domain degree of study non-completion, judgements of low risk of bias were given for studies with 20 % or lower drop-out and judgements of high risk of bias were given for studies with more than 20 % drop-out. For the domain incomplete outcome reporting, judgements of low risk of bias were given where all measures reported in the Methods section were reported on in the Results section and judgements of high risk of bias were given where this was not the case. For all domains, an unclear risk of bias judgement was given where information was not reported or unclear. Risk of bias was assessed for each study by two independent reviewers (KMA, LRB, OAA and/or DFS). Discordances were resolved by discussion between reviewers. A-priori, we considered the blinding of participants and researchers and the use of ITT analyses to be the domains most likely to influence our outcomes due to their subjective nature. Studies were included in the review regardless of the risk of bias in each domain.

Analysis

Data were combined both narratively and statistically. Statistical combination was undertaken to provide estimations of effect sizes, although caution should be exercised, considering the low number of studies. Meta-analyses were conducted for three outcomes of mental health identified during the review process: psychological well-being, depressive symptomology and anxiety-related symptomology, with each outcome analysed separately. Each outcome was also analysed twice, once using data recorded at the end of the intervention (end-of-intervention data) and once using data on change from baseline to the end of the intervention (change-from-baseline data).

Mean and standard deviation data for each intervention and comparator group, corrected to ensure comparable direction, were analysed as standardised mean difference (SMD) (Cohen’s d) with 95 % CI, using unadjusted ITT data (based on number of participants randomised), where possible(Reference Deeks, Altman, Bradburn, Egger, Davey Smith and Altman30,Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Egger, Davey Smith and Altman31) . Estimates were made using fixed effect models primarily, due to low heterogeneity between studies. Random effects models were also applied as sensitivity analyses(Reference Deeks, Altman, Bradburn, Egger, Davey Smith and Altman30,Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Egger, Davey Smith and Altman31) . Effect size estimates were subsequently converted into meaningful units for each outcome, using the most commonly used measure for the outcome in the contributing studies and the standard deviations reported(Reference Higgins, Thomas and Chandler28, section 15·5). Where studies included multiple treatment or comparator groups, each treatment or comparator group was treated as an independent comparison, and participant numbers in single comparison groups were divided.

Authors were contacted by email for original data where these were missing from publications. Complete unadjusted data on baseline, end-of-intervention and change-from-baseline were obtained for the study by Smith & Rogers(Reference Smith and Rogers32). Where change-from-baseline data could not be obtained(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33), these data were calculated based on reported means and estimated from mean standard deviations. No other study used the same measures, so alternative estimates for standard deviations were not possible(Reference Furukawa, Barbui and Cipriani34).

Heterogeneity between studies was investigated using Higgins’ I2 statistic(Reference Higgins and Thompson35,Reference Higgins, Thompson and Deeks36) . The I2 statistic can be interpreted such that 0–40 % heterogeneity might not be important, 30–60 % heterogeneity may represent moderate heterogeneity, 50–90 % heterogeneity may represent substantial heterogeneity and 75–100 % heterogeneity may represent considerable heterogeneity(Reference Higgins, Thomas and Chandler28), but caution is again urged given the low number of studies included in all analyses. Possible sources of heterogeneity were identified a-priori to include publication bias and risk of bias, but insufficient studies were available to allow the systematic consideration of these sources of bias in our analyses.

Analyses were undertaken in RevMan (www.cochrane.org) (version 5.4.1) and Stata, Stata Corp Inc. (version 17).

Results

Searches were conducted on the 16th September 2022, to result in the identification of 5136 articles of potential relevance. Title and abstract screening resulted in the retention of sixty-eight of these articles. All full texts were obtained and screened to result in the final inclusion of six articles in the review(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) . These articles detailed six studies, one of which included two relevant intervention groups(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17), and one included two relevant control groups(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37). Together, these studies enrolled a total of 691 individuals. The PRISMA flow diagram is given in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Characteristics of included studies

Methodological details of all studies are provided in Table 1. Three studies were designed to investigate mental health(Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) , the reports by De Leon et al.(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15) and Conner et al.(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17) were secondary reports of trials by Casperson et al.(Reference Casperson, Jahns and Temple39) and Brookie et al.(Reference Brookie, Mainvil and Carr40), respectively and the study by Plaisted et al.(Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) is described as a sub-study of a larger trial by Appel et al.(Reference Appel, Moore and Obarzanek41). A correction to the trial by Conner et al.(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) detailing concerns with randomisation has also been published(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard42), but these concerns do not render this trial ineligible for our review.

Table 1. Methodological details for all included studies

Data provided as means (and standard deviations). C, comparator; F, fruit; FV, fruit and vegetables; I, intervention; NR, not reported, Ppts: participants; V, vegetable.

* Additional detail gained from Casperson et al. 2021(Reference Casperson, Jahns and Temple39).

Additional detail gained from Conner et al. 2022(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard42).

Additional detail gained from Brookie et al. 2017(Reference Brookie, Mainvil and Carr40).

§ Additional detail gained from Appel et al. 1997(Reference Appel, Moore and Obarzanek41).

Studies included between 35 and 174 participants at trial conception, but only 27–171 participants provided data for the outcomes in this review. In all studies, the majority of participants was female with the exception of one study where the sample consisted of 36 % females(Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38). In three studies, participants were young adults(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) , and in three studies, participants were low consumers of FV or not reaching current recommendations(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) . None of the studies were undertaken specifically among depressive or anxiety-related clinical populations, or reported inclusion of individuals with depressive or anxiety-related conditions.

Studies tested an intervention which provided fruit(Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) , vegetables(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15), fruit and vegetables(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) , a fruit and vegetable juice(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33) or provided participants with a voucher for a greengrocers and used text-messaging and behaviour change techniques to encourage them to increase their FV intake(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17). Comparators were the provision of alternative foods (chocolate and crisps)(Reference Smith and Rogers32), sugar-free chewing gum(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17), an artificial coloured beverage(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33), vitamin C or placebo tablets(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37), nothing(Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) or an attention control(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15). Participants were randomised to result in intervention and control groups that were roughly equal in size, and compliance with the intervention or comparator was assessed and reported as good in four studies(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) . Interventions and comparisons lasted for 10 d(Reference Smith and Rogers32), 14 d(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17), 4 weeks(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37), 45 d over 9 weeks(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33) and 8 weeks(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) .

Studies measured psychological well-being(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) , depressive symptomology(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32) and anxiety-related symptomology(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33) , plus several additional mood, physical or mental attributes not considered suitable for inclusion in our review. Psychological well-being was assessed using the Subjective Happiness Scale(Reference Lyubomirsky and Lepper43), the Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale(Reference Tennant, Hiller and Fishwick44) and the 36-item short form Health Survey (SF-36)(45). Depressive symptomology was assessed using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale(Reference Zigmond and Snaith46) and the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale(Reference Radloff47). Anxiety-related symptomology was assessed using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale(Reference Zigmond and Snaith46) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory(Reference Beck, Epstein and Brown48).

Results from all studies are given in Table 2. Scores at baseline in all studies suggested good psychological well-being and low depressive and anxiety-related symptomology at study start. Studies reported small statistically significant increases in psychological well-being following FV consumption, and some reductions in depressive and anxiety-related symptomology, although effects were small and did not reach statistical significance in all studies. No improvements in these outcomes were reported in comparator groups, and in the study by Smith & Rogers(Reference Smith and Rogers32), marked increases in depressive symptomology scores were found in the comparator group following the consumption of alternative unhealthy foods. Authors of all studies concluded possible benefits for mental health from the consumption of FV. There are also suggestions that providing participants with FV may have a greater impact than simply asking them to consume more FV(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17), that effects may be greater in participants who have lower circulating levels of some of the biological compounds in FV(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) and that effects may occur quickly and then reduce over time(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37).

Table 2. Findings for all included studies (Mean values and standard deviations)

Data provided as means (and standard deviations). C, comparator; DGA, Dietary Guidelines for Americans; F, fruit; FV, fruit and vegetables; I, intervention; NR, not reported; V, vegetables.

* All end assessments made at the end of intervention.

Additional detail gained from Casperson et al. 2021(Reference Casperson, Jahns and Temple39).

De Leon et al. 2022(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15): Standard deviations for change were calculated from published standard errors.

§ Additional detail gained from Conner et al. 2022(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard42).

|| Conner et al. 2020(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37): Means and standard deviations for change were gained from Conner et al. 2022(Reference Appel, Moore and Obarzanek41).

Chiochetta et al. 2018(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33): Means and standard deviations for change were calculated based on reported means and estimated from mean standard deviations.

** Additional detail gained from Brookie et al. 2017(Reference Brookie, Mainvil and Carr40).

†† Conner et al. 2017(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17): Standard deviations for change were calculated from published CI.

‡‡ Smith & Rogers, 2014(Reference Smith and Rogers32): Published data are adjusted means and standard errors for baseline and end, and percentage change from baseline. Unadjusted means and standard deviations for analyses were gained from authors.

§§ Additional detail gained from Appel et al. 1997(Reference Appel, Moore and Obarzanek41).

Risk of bias

When assessing risk of bias, we considered three studies to have used randomisation methods, that were low in risk of bias(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32) and that resulting intervention and comparator groups were comparable in nature in three studies(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33) . Some concerns were noted for the study by Conner et al.(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37), where randomisation was not implemented as intended(Reference Appel, Moore and Obarzanek41) and level of education differed between randomisation groups, a variable known to be associated with healthy eating and mental health(Reference Verger, Lions and Ventelou25,Reference Vermeulen-Smit, Ten Have and Van Laar26) . Groups in the study by Plaisted et al.(Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) also differed in sex, another variable known to be associated with healthy eating and mental health(Reference Verger, Lions and Ventelou25,Reference Vermeulen-Smit, Ten Have and Van Laar26) . Allocation concealment was rarely reported. In all studies, blinding of participants was not possible, with the exception of the study by Chiochetta et al.(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33), where participants were blinded to juice received (FV v. non-FV). In one study(Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38), researchers were blinded, in three studies, the researchers undertaking the outcome assessments were not blinded(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) , while in the other two studies, blinding of researchers (or not) was not reported(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32) . Data were analysed on an ITT basis in only one study(Reference Smith and Rogers32). Number of participants with incomplete data was low in three studies(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) . All pre-specified outcomes were reported in all studies. One additional source of possible bias was also found, where in one study(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15) assessments of psychological well-being were not implemented until part way through running the trial. Risk of bias for all studies is given in Table 3.

Table 3. Risk of bias in all included studies

ITT, intention-to-treat.

* Additional detail gained from Casperson et al. 2021(Reference Casperson, Jahns and Temple39).

Additional detail gained from Conner et al. 2022(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard42).

Additional detail gained from Brookie et al. 2017(Reference Brookie, Mainvil and Carr40).

§ Additional detail gained from Appel et al. 1997(Reference Appel, Moore and Obarzanek41).

Statistical combination

Psychological well-being

Three studies(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) (one with two control groups)(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) contributed data for these analyses. Analyses of end-of-intervention data using fixed effect models (4 studies, 289 participants) revealed a SMD of 0·07 (95 % CI –0·17, 0·30), P = 0·58, I2 = 0 %. Analyses using random effects models found the same effects. Analyses of change-from-baseline data using fixed effect and random effects models (4 studies, 289 participants) revealed a SMD of 0·28 (95 % CI 0·05, 0·52), P = 0·02, I2 = 0 %. Forest plots for analyses on end-of-intervention and change-from-baseline data are provided in Fig. 2. One study used a control group who consumed vitamin C capsules to assess the possibility that the vitamin C in fruit was the mechanism by which FV impact mental health(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37). Analyses without this study on end-of-intervention data using fixed effect and random effects models (3 studies, 237 participants) revealed a SMD of 0·12 (95 % CI –0·14, 0·37), P = 0·37, I2 = 0 %. Analyses without this study on change-from-baseline data using fixed effect and random effects models (3 studies, 237 participants) revealed a SMD of 0·35 (95 % CI 0·09, 0·61), P = 0·01, I2 = 0 %. Effects favour FV, but effect sizes are small, and when converted into units on the SF-36 are equivalent to an improvement in psychological well-being from 0·8 (–1·9–3·4) units to 3·8 (–1·0 – 6·6) units on a 100-point scale.

Fig. 2. Forest plots for meta-analyses on psychological well-being, fixed effect models; panel a: end-of-intervention data, panel b: change-from-baseline data. Each Forest plot demonstrates effects in individual studies (effect size, 95 % CI and % contribution of the study to the overall result), while the standardised mean difference (SMD) and 95 % CI for all studies combined are represented by the diamond at the base. Effects to the right of the null line represent better psychological health.

Depressive symptomology

Two studies(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32) , one with two intervention groups(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17), contributed data. Analyses of end-of-intervention data using fixed effect models (3 studies, 271 participants) revealed a SMD of –0·15 (95 % CI –0·40, 0·10), P = 0·23, I2 = 47 %. Analyses using random effects models revealed a SMD of –0·14 (95 % CI –0·48, 0·20), P = 0·43, I2 = 47 %. The Forest plot for the analysis on end-of-intervention data is provided in Fig. 3, panel a. Analyses of change-from-baseline data using fixed effect and random effects models (3 studies, 271 participants) revealed a SMD of –0·21 (95 % CI –0·45, 0·04), P = 0·10, I2 = 0 %. Effects favour FV, but effect sizes are small, and when converted into units on the HADS are equivalent to an improvement in depressive symptomology of 0·4 (–0·1 to 0·9) units on a 21-point scale.

Fig. 3. Forest plots for meta-analyses, fixed effect models on end-of-intervention data; panel a: depressive symptomology, panel b: anxiety-related symptomology. Plots demonstrate effects in individual studies (effect size, 95 % CI and % contribution of the study to the overall result), while the standardised mean difference (SMD) and 95 % CI for all studies combined are represented by the diamond at the base. Effects to the right of the null line represent greater symptomology, so poorer mental health.

Anxiety-related symptomology

Three studies(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33) , one with two intervention groups(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17), contributed data. Analyses of end-of-intervention data using fixed effect models (4 studies, 298 participants) revealed a SMD of –0·15 (95 % CI –0·39, 0·08), P = 0·20, I2 = 71 %. Analyses using random effects models revealed a SMD of –0·06 (95 % CI –0·51, 0·38), P = 0·78, I2 = 71 %. The Forest plot for the analysis on end-of-intervention data is provided in Fig. 3, panel b. Analyses of change-from-baseline data using fixed effects models revealed a SMD of –0·10 (95 % CI –0·34, 0·13), P = 0·40, I2 = 39 %. Analyses using random effects models (4 studies, 298 participants) revealed a SMD of –0·08 (95 % CI –0·39, 0·23), P = 0·61, I2 = 39 %. Effects favour FV, but effect sizes are small, and when converted into units on the HADS are equivalent to an improvement in anxiety-related symptomology of 0·2 (–1·1 to 1·7) units on a 21-point scale.

Discussion

This review aimed to identify and summarise all published controlled intervention studies that have investigated an effect of FV consumption as a distinct intervention on mental health in adults. Only six studies were found, involving a total of 691 individuals. Studies used a range of FV interventions. Three studies reported on psychological well-being, two reported on depressive symptomology and three studies reported on anxiety-related symptomology. All studies were relevant to our research question, but mental health was a secondary outcome in some studies and risk of bias in the included studies was high. In only one study was it possible to blind participants(Reference Chiochetta, Ferreira and Moreira33), in only one study were researchers reported to be blinded(Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) and in only one study were data analysed on an ITT basis based on number randomised(Reference Smith and Rogers32). The evidence available to draw conclusions on the impacts of FV consumption on mental health is thus extremely limited.

Considering all eligible studies, our analyses revealed limited effects of FV consumption on three outcomes related to mental health. Psychological well-being, depressive symptomology and anxiety-related symptomology were all improved by FV consumption, but these effects are not statistically significant and effect sizes are small. Effects are equivalent to a 1- to 4-point improvement on a 100-point scale of psychological well-being and less than a single point improvement on a 21-point scale of depressive or anxiety-related symptomology. CI are also wide; thus, the effect estimates are imprecise, ranging from very small negative or null effects to small positive effects. Heterogeneity between studies was low in analyses on psychological well-being, suggesting these effects are consistent across the studies included, but heterogeneity was moderate to substantial in analyses on depressive and anxiety-related symptomology suggesting inconsistency. Measures of heterogeneity can be difficult to interpret where few studies are included in analyses. The low number of studies available also precluded any assessment of possible sources of heterogeneity.

We find some suggestion that effects in psychological well-being and depressive symptomology may be stronger if baseline scores are considered, that is, in change-from-baseline data. Effect sizes remain small but may be important at the upper end of our CI. Importantly, however, no studies were found that were specifically conducted in individuals with poor mental health, high depressive or anxiety-related symptomology, or a depressive or anxiety-related clinical condition. Further investigation in these populations may be of value.

Effects size estimates in psychological well-being were also found to increase if a study with a control group who consumed vitamin C capsules was removed from the analyses. These findings suggest that the vitamin C in FV may contribute to the effects of FV consumption on psychological well-being. A similar conclusion is offered by the authors of the primary study(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37), with the addition that vitamin C from whole FV may have a greater impact than that that may be gained from supplements, due to the provision of other biological compounds, such as fibre, folate or potassium, which may also impact psychological well-being(Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37). We were unable to investigate these mechanisms in our analyses due to the few studies available, but studies based on likely mechanisms could be important.

Taken together, our findings suggest small and imprecise effects of FV consumption on all three aspects of mental health, although very few, limited studies and few participants contribute to our review. Our conclusions are comparable with those of other reviews with regard to the limited number of intervention studies currently available, but our conclusions differ from the majority of these reviews(Reference Aucoin, LaChance and Naidoo5Reference Tuck, Farrow and Thomas10), from the studies included(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37,Reference Plaisted, Lin and Ard38) and from the majority of studies published in this area(Reference Conner, Brookie and Richardson12,Reference Ocean, Howley and Ensor16,Reference Smith, Stevenson and Dudley18,Reference Mujcic and Oswald19,Reference Hoare, Hockey and Ruusunen23,Reference Ahmed, Dupuis and Tyron49Reference Warner, Frye and Morrell51) , as we find very little evidence to suggest a beneficial effect. Some independent studies also report no association between FV consumption and mental health. For example, Kingsbury et al.(Reference Kingsbury, Dupuis and Jacka24) and Pengpid & Peltzer(Reference Pengpid and Peltzer52) report no associations in longitudinal analyses of cohort studies using multiple measures once confounding factors are controlled, although Pengpid & Peltzer(Reference Pengpid and Peltzer52) do report associations between vegetable consumption and one outcome measure. We were unable to investigate differing effects as a result of fruit v. vegetable consumption(Reference Tuck, Farrow and Thomas10,Reference Smith, Stevenson and Dudley18,Reference Pengpid and Peltzer52) , due to the limited number of studies available.

We were also unable to investigate differing effects as a result of the consumption of specific fruits, vegetables or FV forms(Reference Aucoin, LaChance and Naidoo5,Reference Głąbska, Guzek and Groele7,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Brookie, Best and Conner53,Reference Sun, Li and Li54) , the differing outcomes assessed(Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Warner, Frye and Morrell51,Reference Pengpid and Peltzer52) or the differing assessment time frames(Reference Ocean, Howley and Ensor16,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Mujcic and Oswald19,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) .

Further study may be desirable to provide more conclusive evidence. This evidence will only be gained from adequately powered randomised trials using appropriate control conditions and study procedures. Importantly, control conditions must allow the independent and isolated investigation of FV consumption, to consider not only aspects of diet and lifestyle but also comparable treatment of study participants by study researchers, for example, through researcher blinding and the use of comparable gift-giving, and the potential deleterious effects of the consumption of other (less healthy) foods(Reference Smith and Rogers32,Reference Sutliffe, Carnot and Fuhrman55) . Blinding of those making the outcome assessments is also important for subjective outcomes, and while it may not be possible to blind participants to an increased consumption of FV, it will be possible to blind participants to other study conditions and to study aims. Consideration of study non-completion will also be important where participants may fail to complete studies following both positive and negative experiences(Reference Brain, Burrows and Rollo56). Studies based on potential mechanisms will be of particular value, as will studies involving realistic and sustainable interventions that can be transferred to the public health setting, where outcomes are also assessed over sufficient time frames for meaningful interpretation. Considering our effects in change-from-baseline data, advantages may be gained from the study specifically of those with low mood at baseline or those presenting with clinical conditions. Some advantage may also be gained from a focus on those who are low consumers of FV(Reference De Leon, Jahns and Roemmich15,Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr17,Reference Conner, Fletcher and Haszard37) . Comparison with other dietary components, dietary interventions and other lifestyle interventions may also be of value. Recent meta-analyses of meta-analyses investigating the effects of physical activity on mental health, for example, report convincing beneficial effect size estimates of SMD = –0·50 (95 % CI –0·93, –0·06), I2 = 0 %, 92 studies, 4310 participants, for depressive symptomology and SMD = –0·38 (95 % CI –0·66, –0·11) I2 = 4 %, 306 studies, 10 755 participants, for anxiety-related symptomology, in non-clinical populations(Reference Rebar, Stanton and Geard57).

Limitations

Our review is limited by the very small number of studies available, the limited number of participants and the high risk of bias in most domains in many studies. Our searches were confined to articles published in English in full papers, and while we sought to include as many studies as possible, we only included studies that used a distinct FV consumption intervention; we did not look at multi-component interventions that were only effective for changing FV consumption, nor did we look at alternative interventions that may be expected to increase FV consumption, such as vegetable gardening(Reference Appleton, Hemingway and Saulais58). Consideration of these studies may enhance the evidence base, but confounding must remain a consideration. Our conclusions are limited by the very small number of studies available and the specific nature of these studies. Notably, the studies included in our review were conducted in predominantly female samples, of young age and from Western populations; thus, generalisability to other population groups may be limited. Due to the very small number of studies available in our analyses, we were also unable to investigate possible sources of heterogeneity or confounding.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we found very few controlled intervention studies that investigated the effects of FV consumption on psychological well-being, depressive symptomology and anxiety-related symptomology, and the majority of these studies were considered at high risk of bias on many important criteria. Thus, the evidence available to draw conclusions on the impacts of FV consumption on mental health is extremely limited. Meta-analyses suggest some benefits of FV consumption, but effects sizes are small and imprecise. Given the limited evidence available and the small size of effects, stronger evidence is needed before recommending FV consumption for mental health. Further research to provide more definitive conclusions would be of value.

Acknowledgements

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. The work was supported by Bournemouth University, UK. Bournemouth University had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

K. M. A. conceived the study idea and registered the protocol; K. M. A., O. A. A. and D. F. S. undertook the study; K. M. A. undertook all analyses; L. R. B. confirmed all procedures and findings; K. M. A. wrote the manuscript; all authors edited and confirmed the final draft.

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

Aune, D, Giovannucci, E, Boffetta, P, et al. (2017) Fruit and vegetable intake and the risk of cardiovascular disease, total cancer and all-cause mortality – a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Int J Epidemiol 46, 10291056.10.1093/ije/dyw319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyebode, O, Gordon-Dseagu, V, Walker, A, et al. (2014) Fruit and vegetable consumption and all-cause, cancer and CVD mortality: analysis of Health Survey for England data. J Epidemiol Community Health 68, 856862.Google Scholar
Tohill, BC (2005) Dietary Intake of Fruit and Vegetables and Management of Body-weight. Background Paper for the Joint FAO/WHO Workshop of Fruit and Vegetables for Health. Geneva: WHO. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/43145 Google Scholar
World Health Organisation (2003) Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Report of a Joint FAO/WHO Expert Consultation. Geneva: WHO. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/42665/WHO_TRS_916.pdf?sequence=1 Google Scholar
Aucoin, M, LaChance, L, Naidoo, U, et al. (2021) Diet and anxiety: a scoping review. Nutrients 13, 4418.Google Scholar
Dharmayani, PNA, Juergens, M, Allman-Farinelli, M, et al. (2021) Association between fruit and vegetable consumption and depression symptoms in young people and adults aged 15–45: a systematic review of cohort studies. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18, 780.10.3390/ijerph18020780CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Głąbska, D, Guzek, D, Groele, B, et al. (2020) Fruit and vegetable intake and mental health in adults: a systematic review. Nutrients 12, 115.Google Scholar
Liu, X, Yan, Y, Li, F, et al. (2016) Fruit and vegetable consumption and the risk of depression: a meta-analysis. Nutrition 32, 296302.Google Scholar
Saghafian, F, Malmir, H, Saneei, P, et al. (2018) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of depression: accumulative evidence from an updated systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological studies. Br J Nutr 119, 10871101.Google Scholar
Tuck, NJ, Farrow, C & Thomas, JM (2019) Assessing the effects of vegetable consumption on the mental health of healthy adults: a systematic review of prospective research. Am J Clin Nutr 110, 196211.10.1093/ajcn/nqz080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Global Burden of Disease Study (2018) Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Lancet 392, 17891858.Google Scholar
Conner, TS, Brookie, KL, Richardson, AC, et al. (2015) On carrots and curiosity: eating fruit and vegetables is associated with greater flourishing in daily life. Br J Health Psychol 20, 413427.Google Scholar
Mueller, M, Ganesh, R & Bonnes, S (2020) Gut Health = Mental Health? The impact of diet and dietary supplements on mood disorders. Curr Nutr Rep 9, 361368.10.1007/s13668-020-00340-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooney, C, McKinley, MC & Woodside, JV (2013) The potential role of fruit and vegetables in aspects of mental well-being: a review of the literature and future directions. Proc Nutr Soc 72, 420432.10.1017/S0029665113003388CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Leon, A, Jahns, L, Roemmich, JN, et al. (2022) Consumption of dietary guidelines for Americans types and amounts of vegetables increases mean subjective happiness scale scores: a randomized controlled trial. J Acad Nutr Diet 122, 13551362.10.1016/j.jand.2021.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ocean, N, Howley, P & Ensor, J (2019) Lettuce be happy: a longitudinal UK study on the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and well-being. Social Sci Med 222, 335345.10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.12.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conner, TS, Brookie, KL, Carr, AC, et al. (2017) Let them eat fruit! The effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on mental well-being in young adults: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 12, e0171206.10.1371/journal.pone.0171206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E, Stevenson, R, Dudley, L, et al. (2022) The relationship of health-related expectancies, fruit and vegetable intake, and positive mood: expectancies are important, but not in the way you expect. Br Food J 124, 885897.Google Scholar
Mujcic, R & Oswald, AJ (2016) Evolution of well-being and happiness after increases in consumption of fruit and vegetables. Am J Public Health 106, 15041510.10.2105/AJPH.2016.303260CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, BA, Horwath, CC & Conner, TS (2013) Many apples a day keep the blues away--daily experiences of negative and positive affect and food consumption in young adults. Br J Health Psychol 18, 782798.Google Scholar
Boehm, JK, Soo, J, Zevon, ES, et al. (2018) Longitudinal associations between mental well-being and the consumption of fruits and vegetables. Health Psychol 37, 959967.Google Scholar
Blanchflower, DG, Oswald, AJ & Stewart-Brown, S (2013) Is mental well-being linked to the consumption of fruit and vegetables? Social Indic Res 114, 785801.Google Scholar
Hoare, E, Hockey, M, Ruusunen, A, et al. (2018) Does fruit and vegetable consumption during adolescence predict adult depression? A longitudinal study of US adolescents. Front Psychiatr 9, 581.Google Scholar
Kingsbury, M, Dupuis, G, Jacka, F, et al. (2016) Associations between fruit and vegetable consumption and depressive symptoms: evidence from a national Canadian longitudinal survey. J Epidemiol Community Health 70, 155161.Google Scholar
Verger, P, Lions, C & Ventelou, B (2009) Is depression associated with health risk-related behaviour clusters in adults? Eur J Public Health 19, 618624.Google Scholar
Vermeulen-Smit, E, Ten Have, M, Van Laar, M, et al. (2015) Clustering of health risk behaviours and the relationship with mental disorders. J Affect Disord 171, 111119.Google Scholar
Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossyt, PM, et al. (2021) The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guidelines for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372, n71.Google Scholar
Higgins, JPT, Thomas, J, Chandler, J, et al. (editors) (2019) Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, 2nd ed. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2022) World Mental Health Report: Transforming Mental Health for All. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Deeks, JJ, Altman, DG & Bradburn, MJ (2001) Statistical methods for examining heterogeneity and combining results from several studies in meta-analysis. In Systematic Reviews in Health Care: Meta-Analysis in Context, pp. 285312 [Egger, M, Davey Smith, G & Altman, DG, editors]. London: BMJ Publishing Group.10.1002/9780470693926.ch15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egger, M & Davey Smith, G (2001) Principles of and procedures for systematic reviews. In Systematic Reviews in Health Care: Meta-Analysis in Context, pp. 2342 [Egger, M, Davey Smith, G & Altman, DG, editors]. London: BMJ Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Smith, AP & Rogers, R (2014) Positive effects of a healthy snack (fruit) v. an unhealthy snack (chocolate/crisps) on subjective reports of mental and physical health: a preliminary intervention study. Front Nutr 1, 10.Google Scholar
Chiochetta, M, Ferreira, EJ, Moreira, ITDS, et al. (2018) Green juice in human metabolism: a Randomized Trial. J Am Coll Nutr 27, 17.Google Scholar
Furukawa, TA, Barbui, C, Cipriani, A, et al. (2006) Imputing missing standard deviations in meta-analyses can provide accurate results. J Clin Epidemiol 59, 710.Google Scholar
Higgins, JPT & Thompson, SG (2002) Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 21, 15391558.10.1002/sim.1186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, JPT, Thompson, SG, Deeks, JJ, et al. (2004) Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557560.Google Scholar
Conner, TS, Fletcher, BD, Haszard, JJ, et al. (2020) KiwiC for vitality: results of a randomized placebo-controlled trial testing the effects of Kiwifruit or Vitamin C tablets on vitality in adults with low Vitamin C levels. Nutrients 12, 2898.Google Scholar
Plaisted, CS, Lin, P-H, Ard, JD, et al. (1999) The effects of dietary patterns on quality of life: a substudy of the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Trial. J Am Diet Assoc 99, S84S89.Google Scholar
Casperson, SL, Jahns, L, Temple, JL, et al. (2021) Consumption of a variety of vegetables to meet Dietary Guidelines for Americans’ recommendations does not induce sensitization of vegetable reinforcement among adults with overweight and obesity: a randomized controlled trial. J Nutr 151, 16651672.10.1093/jn/nxab049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brookie, KL, Mainvil, LA, Carr, AC, et al. (2017) The development and effectiveness of an ecological momentary intervention to increase daily fruit and vegetable consumption in low-consuming young adults. Appetite 108, 3241.Google Scholar
Appel, LJ, Moore, TJ & Obarzanek, E (1997) A clinical trial of the effects of dietary patterns on blood pressure. N Engl J Med 336, 11171124.Google Scholar
Conner, TS, Fletcher, BD, Haszard, JJ, et al. (2022) Correction: Conner et al. KiwiC for vitality: results of a placebo-controlled trial testing the effects of kiwifruit or vitamin C tablets on vitality in adults with low vitamin C levels. Nutrients 2020, 12, 2898. Nutrients 14, 4063.Google Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S & Lepper, HS (1999) A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation. Soc Indic Res 46, 137155.Google Scholar
Tennant, R, Hiller, L, Fishwick, R, et al. (2007) The Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes 5, 63.Google Scholar
International Resources Center for Health Care Assessment (1991) How to Score the MOS 36-item Short Form Health Survey (SF-36). Boston, MA: New England Medical Center Hospitals.Google Scholar
Zigmond, AS & Snaith, RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 67, 361370.Google Scholar
Radloff, LS (1977) The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Mental Meas 1, 385401.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Epstein, N, Brown, G, et al. (1988) An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56, 893897.Google Scholar
Ahmed, S, Dupuis, V, Tyron, M, et al. (2020) Intended and unintended consequences of a community-based fresh fruit and vegetable dietary intervention on the Flathead Reservation of the Confederated Salish and Kootenai Tribes. Front Public Health 8, 331.Google Scholar
Putra, ES, Wasita, B & Anantanyu, S (2018) A randomised trial on walking exercise and banana consumption on self-reported depression symptoms among female adolescents in Surakarta, Indonesia. Mal J Nutr 24, 467473.Google Scholar
Warner, RM, Frye, K, Morrell, JS, et al. (2017) Fruit and vegetable intake predicts positive affect. J Happiness Stud 18, 809826.Google Scholar
Pengpid, S & Peltzer, K (2019) Association between fruit/vegetable consumption and mental-health-related quality of life, major depression and generalized anxiety disorder: a longitudinal study in Thailand. Iran J Psychiatr Behav Sci 13, e88246.Google Scholar
Brookie, KL, Best, GI & Conner, TS (2018) Intake of raw fruits and vegetables is associated with better mental health than intake of processed fruits and vegetables. Front Psychol 9, 487.Google Scholar
Sun, J, Li, Z, Li, Y, et al. (2021) Intakes of specific categories of vegetables and fruits are inversely associated with depressive symptoms among adults. J Epidemiol 31, 210219.Google Scholar
Sutliffe, JT, Carnot, MJ, Fuhrman, JH, et al. (2018) A worksite nutrition intervention is effective at improving employee well-being: a pilot study. J Nutr Metab 2018, 8187203.Google Scholar
Brain, K, Burrows, TL, Rollo, ME, et al. (2019) The effect of a pilot dietary intervention on pain outcomes in patients attending a tertiary pain service. Nutrients 11, 181.10.3390/nu11010181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebar, AL, Stanton, R, Geard, D, et al. (2015) A meta-meta-analysis of the effect of physical activity on depression and anxiety in non-clinical adult populations. Health Psychol Rev 9, 366378.Google Scholar
Appleton, KM, Hemingway, A, Saulais, L, et al. (2016) Increasing vegetable intakes: rationale and systematic review of published interventions. Eur J Nutr 55, 869896.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Figure 1

Table 1. Methodological details for all included studies

Figure 2

Table 2. Findings for all included studies (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 3

Table 3. Risk of bias in all included studies

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Forest plots for meta-analyses on psychological well-being, fixed effect models; panel a: end-of-intervention data, panel b: change-from-baseline data. Each Forest plot demonstrates effects in individual studies (effect size, 95 % CI and % contribution of the study to the overall result), while the standardised mean difference (SMD) and 95 % CI for all studies combined are represented by the diamond at the base. Effects to the right of the null line represent better psychological health.

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Forest plots for meta-analyses, fixed effect models on end-of-intervention data; panel a: depressive symptomology, panel b: anxiety-related symptomology. Plots demonstrate effects in individual studies (effect size, 95 % CI and % contribution of the study to the overall result), while the standardised mean difference (SMD) and 95 % CI for all studies combined are represented by the diamond at the base. Effects to the right of the null line represent greater symptomology, so poorer mental health.