Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:19:46.736Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using development and psychopathology principles to inform the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2021

Elisabeth Conradt*
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychology, Pediatrics, OB/GYN, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Sheila E. Crowell
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychology, Psychiatry, OB/GYN, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Dante Cicchetti
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota Institute of Child Development, Minneapolis, MN, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Elisabeth Conradt; E-mail: elisabeth.conradt@psych.utah.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In 2010, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) were developed to advance our understanding of the pathophysiology of mental illness across multiple levels of analysis, ranging from cells to circuits to self-report instruments. Several conceptual RDoC-informed manuscripts have highlighted the importance of studying how developmental processes give rise to psychopathology. However, there are few empirical studies that integrate the RDoC framework with development and psychopathology principles. This special issue was developed to fill this empirical gap. In this introduction to the special issue, we describe how the developmental psychopathology field predates and informs the RDoC framework. We highlight three important ways in which developmental psychopathology and the RDoC framework can mutually inform one another, leading to novel discoveries to identify, prevent, and treat mental health problems across the life span.

Type
Special Issue Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

In 1984, Cicchetti described diverging interests between psychiatrists, who diagnosed and treated individual patients, and academic researchers, who conducted controlled experiments in their laboratories with little thought to the real-world implications of their work (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984). While researchers thought carefully about developmental processes and attempted to study these in the laboratory, clinicians at the time rarely considered how the study of typical developmental processes could be used to understand emerging symptoms of psychopathology (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984). Clinicians and the researchers each made important contributions toward advancing the science and treatment of psychiatric disorders, but they had no common, developmentally informed scientific language they could use to share their discoveries. The field of development and psychopathology was created, in part, to bridge this gap between clinicians and academic psychologists by providing a theoretical framework to support the bidirectional communication between researchers and clinicians (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984).

In many ways, the 2010 advent of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) recapitulates this tension between clinical research and practice (Insel, Reference Insel2009; Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). RDoC was developed to provide a language for researchers to advance understanding of the pathophysiology of mental illness, one that is not constrained by diagnostic boundaries (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). It is expected that RDoC-informed research will result in novel methods of classifying mental illness based on a dimensional approach and that it could result in the development of an empirically informed system for classifying mental illness (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). Mirroring the lack of attention paid to developmental processes in the psychiatric literature in the 1980s, within the RDoC framework there is currently little consideration of basic developmental processes or the broader context in which children develop (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine and Hinshaw2020; Boyce et al., Reference Boyce, Frank, Jensen, Kessler, Nelson and Steinberg1998; Cicchetti & Aber, Reference Cicchetti and Aber1998; Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn, & Nock, Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015; Garber & Bradshaw, Reference Garber and Bradshaw2020). Although it is a central aim of RDoC to promote our understanding of neurodevelopmental origins of mental illness (Morris & Cuthbert, Reference Morris and Cuthbert2012), few empirical studies have attempted to integrate developmental principles into the existing RDoC framework (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine and Hinshaw2020; Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015; Garber & Bradshaw, Reference Garber and Bradshaw2020; Mittal & Wakschlag, Reference Mittal and Wakschlag2017; Smith & Pollak, Reference Smith and Pollak2020).

This lack of attention to developmental principles in RDoC does not mean that the RDoC perspective in children has been ignored. The origins of developmental and dimensional perspectives on psychopathology began in the 1960s when Achenbach began studying symptoms of child psychopathology (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1966; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2009), and laid the foundation for the field of development and psychopathology (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984). During this time Achenbach and colleagues were describing the importance of using dimensional methods of categorizing child behavior (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1966), Cicchetti advocated for the inclusion of a multiple levels of analysis perspective in studies of child behavior (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Cicchetti & Dawson, Reference Cicchetti and Dawson2002; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984), and the importance of identifying causal mechanisms was emphasized by Sroufe and Rutter (Rutter & Sroufe, Reference Rutter and Sroufe2000; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984). The development and psychopathology field predates RDoC and is therefore a field that is uniquely positioned to integrate life span development with the RDoC framework.

A key goal of the field of development and psychopathology was to, “yield a classification system informed by empirical study of individual development from the ground up, rather than simply a downward extension of adult categories of disturbance or acceptance of clinic-derived child entities.” (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2009). In other words, a simple transition from “RDoC for adults” to “RDoC for children” could hinder and confuse our understanding of pathophysiological mechanisms of psychopathology. In this editorial, we highlight three important ways in which a developmental psychopathology perspective can further inform RDoC and advance our understanding of the developmental origins of mental illness.

Improved Measurement of Adaptive Developmental Trajectories Will Inform Risk Trajectories

A hallmark of the development and psychopathology perspective is rigorous measurement of typical developmental trajectories, which can be used to inform how development goes awry (Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984). As articulated in the 1980s, “we can learn more about the normal functioning of an organism by studying its pathology and, likewise, more about its pathology by studying its normal condition.” (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984, p. 1). Children are not born with complex forms of psychopathology. Instead, symptoms of psychopathology “are complex products of development” (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2009). Developmental psychopathologists have been studying adaptive developmental trajectories of behavior, beginning in infancy, since the landmark Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children in 1975 (Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, Reference Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson and Collins2009). Data from the Minnesota Longitudinal Study allowed psychologists to study risk and resilience (Pianta, Egeland, & Hyatt, Reference Pianta, Egeland and Hyatt1986), continuities and discontinuities in development (Englund, Kuo, Puig, & Collins, Reference Englund, Kuo, Puig and Collins2011), and the dynamic ways in which parents and children interact to increase risk for psychopathology across the life span and across generations (Carlson, Jacobvitz, & Sroufe, Reference Carlson, Jacobvitz and Sroufe1995; Farber, Vaughn, & Egeland, Reference Farber, Vaughn and Egeland1981; Lorber & Egeland, Reference Lorber and Egeland2009; Roisman, Aguilar, & Egeland, Reference Roisman, Aguilar and Egeland2004). This study laid the foundation for developmental psychopathology principles to be empirically tested for the first time.

It is typically assumed that youth “grow into” psychopathology. Instead, developmental processes provide an opportunity to “grow out” of psychopathology risk, through improvements in self-regulation and other foundational skills (Cicchetti & Tucker, Reference Cicchetti and Tucker1994). For example, it is expected that toddlers will tantrum and fuss when they are prevented from playing with a desired (but perhaps dangerous) object. However, an adult who is enraged when his/her goals are blocked shows a more concerning pattern of behavior that could be indicative of significant psychopathology, such as a personality disorder. A development and psychopathology perspective shows that this kind of homotypic continuity exists and may help us identify vulnerable children before the emergence of complex forms of psychopathology (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015; Mittal & Wakschlag, Reference Mittal and Wakschlag2017; Rutter & Sroufe, Reference Rutter and Sroufe2000).

To prevent maladaptive trajectories, we need studies that begin in early childhood and that model typical and atypical trajectories of RDoC-informed traits. Exemplary work of this kind comes from research modeling the development of irritability in early childhood (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Estabrook, Petitclerc, Henry, Burns, Perlman and Briggs-Gowan2015, Reference Wakschlag, Krogh-Jespersen, Estabrook, Hlutkowsky, Anderson, Burns and Perlman2020). In this research, irritability – a construct that can be measured at birth and throughout the life span – is measured using a multiple levels of analysis perspective (neurobiology, psychophysiology, behavioral observation, parent report) to identify the specific constellation of symptoms that may predict psychopathology (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Estabrook, Petitclerc, Henry, Burns, Perlman and Briggs-Gowan2015, Reference Wakschlag, Krogh-Jespersen, Estabrook, Hlutkowsky, Anderson, Burns and Perlman2020). As described by Luby, Wakschlag, and colleagues, the ultimate goal of these studies is to develop data-driven algorithms that may help pediatricians and parents identify young children who may be vulnerable to neurodevelopmental delay and/or problem behavior (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Allen, Estabrook, Pine, Rogers, Krogh-Jespersen and Wakschlag2019). As described in this special issue, RDoC and developmentally informed data may be leveraged to generate mental health risk calculators for use in pediatric and mental health clinics to better identify and prevent emerging psychopathology in the first years of life (MacNeill et al., this issue).

Given that psychopathology is not present at birth, developmental psychopathologists have had to take a dimensional approach to modeling developmental risk trajectories (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1966; Mittal & Wakschlag, Reference Mittal and Wakschlag2017). Already in 1981 this study design was deemed a necessity when Cicchetti and Rizley wrote about the study of child maltreatment: “any classification or nosology represents a rather futile effort to divide what is in reality a continuum into discrete categories…failure to attend to this major source of heterogeneity runs the very real risk of ignoring a source of variation that is crucial to an appreciation of the differing etiologies, sequelae, intergenerational transmission patterns, and treatment responses of different types of maltreatment.” (Cicchetti & Rizley, Reference Cicchetti and Rizley1981, p. 2). To study the etiology of psychiatric disorders using an RDoC framework it may be beneficial to focus on the development of traits known to be expressed in both childhood and adulthood, such as impulsivity (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine and Hinshaw2020), irritability (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Estabrook, Petitclerc, Henry, Burns, Perlman and Briggs-Gowan2015), or emotion dysregulation (Beauchaine & Cicchetti, Reference Beauchaine and Cicchetti2019; Ostlund et al., Reference Ostlund, Vlisides-Henry, Crowell, Raby, Terrell, Brown and Conradt2019) to improve early identification of risk for psychopathology.

By studying developmental trajectories dimensionally we are better equipped to model the nonlinear and dynamic nature of these trajectories, which could help identify risk trajectories (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2009). For example, neurobehavioral disinhibition is a transdiagnostic construct (found across a wide range of disorders; Garvey, Avenevoli, & Anderson, Reference Garvey, Avenevoli and Anderson2016) that is elevated in adolescents with complex psychopathology, ranging from conduct disorder to cognitive impairment and problems with self-regulation (Iacono, Malone, & McGue, Reference Iacono, Malone and McGue2008). Fisher and colleagues found that prenatal substance exposure predicted increases in neurobehavioral disinhibition from age 8 to 14 and early life stress predicted executive functioning difficulties, a component of neurobehavioral disinhibition (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Lester, DeGarmo, Lagasse, Lin, Shankaran and Higgins2011). These findings show that we may be able to identify specific risk trajectories (in this example trajectories that include early life stress and prenatal substance exposure) when transdiagnostic traits are measured across sensitive periods of development.

By modeling adaptive and maladaptive developmental trajectories, developmental psychopathologists have found that different symptom clusters could have different developmental time courses (Casey, Oliveri, & Insel, Reference Casey, Oliveri and Insel2014; Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015). For example in this special issue Karalunas and colleagues use innovative longitudinal network models to show that for some children with working memory impairments, symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in childhood lead to risk for the development of depression in adolescence (Karalunas et al., this issue). It was only by modeling RDoC-informed constructs (working memory impairment) across childhood that these authors were able to identify specific factors that contributed to depression risk in adolescence. Therefore identifying continuities and discontinuities across development shows us that a complete understanding of the etiology of disorder using an RDoC approach should measure how these risk processes change over time (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Jamieson, Glenn and Nock2015; Lebowitz, Gee, Pine, & Silverman, Reference Lebowitz, Gee, Pine and Silverman2018), and consider equi- and multifinal processes.

Measurement of Equi- and Multifinality using RDoC-Relevant Constructs may Lead to Improvements in Early Identification and Prevention of Psychopathology

An explicit focus on dimensional constructs that originate early in development will allow us to better model processes of equi- and multifinality. Psychopathology is etiologically complex and many diverse developmental pathways can result in the same disorder, a concept known as equifinality (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). By definition, developmental studies that include repeated multilevel assessments of risk factors thought to predict psychopathology are needed to examine equifinal processes. For example, in this special issue Ostlund and colleagues examine how prenatal substance exposure, maternal hostility, and observed/parent report of temperament in infancy can be examined to predict externalizing and internalizing behavior in early childhood (Ostlund et al., this issue). They find specific equifinal pathways to risk for internalizing and externalizing behavior in preschoolers that include both prenatal substance exposure and maternal hostility (Ostlund et al., this issue).

With multifinal processes, a variety of different disorders may emerge as a result of similar early life histories (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996). Developmental psychopathologists may be particularly well suited to study multifinal processes from an RDoC framework since RDoC constructs are transdiagnostic and can often be measured in early childhood. For example, preschoolers with high irritability at age 4 are more likely to be diagnosed with generalized anxiety and separation anxiety disorders (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Estabrook, Petitclerc, Henry, Burns, Perlman and Briggs-Gowan2015). Studying these multifinal processes using relevant RDoC-informed developmentally appropriate constructs may lead to improved understanding of when and how trajectories shift from normative to clinically concerning.

Developmental psychopathology researchers have long exceled at conceptualizing and measuring the contribution of environmental influences on equi- and multifinality. As with developmental processes, environmental influences are not well described in RDoC. The processes that give rise to psychiatric disorders are etiologically complex and involve biological and environment interactions across time and levels of analysis. However, the utility of adding an “environment” matrix to RDoC has been called into question at the risk of oversimplifying these complex phenomena (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine and Hinshaw2020). Instead, researchers could develop RDoC-informed models that address developmental and environmental processes specific to traits known to give rise to psychopathology (Beauchaine & Hinshaw, Reference Beauchaine and Hinshaw2020). After all, biological markers of disease are rarely synonymous with expression of that disorder. In many instances Biology × Environment interactions account for a greater proportion of variance in an outcome than the main effect in isolation (Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge, & Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009). For example, in this special issue Liu and colleagues find that maternal symptoms of depression interact with infant high cortisol area under the curve to predict elevated symptoms of psychopathology 6 months later (Liu et al., this issue). Thus, developmental psychopathologists could make a major contribution to our understanding of how these complex multifinal risk trajectories influence and are influenced by environmental exposures.

Identification of Multilevel Mechanisms Implicated in the Development of Transdiagnostic Vulnerabilities May Reveal Causal Pathways

In addition to improved measurement of multilevel equi- and multifinal developmental processes, developmental psychopathologists can contribute to the RDoC agenda by measuring the mechanisms by which early expression of vulnerable RDoC constructs are related to more fixed psychopathology later in life. Developmental researchers excel at identifying whether putative biological mechanisms are predictors, correlates, markers, and/or consequences of psychopathology (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2009), also termed “intermediate psychological constructs” by Kozak and Cuthbert (Kozak & Cuthbert, Reference Kozak and Cuthbert2016). For example, in this issue Bush and colleagues demonstrate that a latent physiological profile comprising low resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia and elevated heart rate was predicted by early life stress and predicts poorer executive functioning in early childhood (Bush et al., this issue). Developmental psychopathologists can make important contributions to the study of etiopathophysiology of psychopathology because they have strong methods for examining vulnerability and risk factors that emerge before the expression of disorder.

Identification of a neurological mechanism is not equal to discovery of a biological cause of disorder. In other words, we should not “confuse biological mediation (the brain) with biological etiology” (Lilienfeld, Reference Lilienfeld2014). Developmental studies may illuminate whether early exposures lead to changes in the brain that make some children more vulnerable to psychopathology, or whether some children are born with these brain-based vulnerabilities. For example, in this special issue Beauchaine and colleagues show that for boys with high internalizing symptoms, more externalizing behavior is related to lower anterior cingulate cortex volumes (Beauchaine et al., this issue). Yet the “cause” of these symptoms may not be the anterior cingulate cortex. In another example from this issue, high exposure to racial discrimination is related to low working memory, which in turn predicts psychosis. However, the “cause” of psychosis is not low working memory, it may not even be exposure to racial discrimination but that exposure is clearly a risk factor (Vargas & Mittal, this issue).

If we are to make improvements in our identification of causal mechanisms it is essential that these mechanisms be validated in diverse samples. Putative mechanisms that operate in one racial or ethnic group may not function in the same way in another (Gatzke-Kopp, Reference Gatzke-Kopp2016). For example, blunted cortisol responses to stress mediated the effect of early life stress on psychopathology in African American adolescents, but not their European American counterparts (Conradt et al., Reference Conradt, Abar, Lester, LaGasse, Shankaran, Bada and Hammond2014). Reducing health disparities in psychopathology warrants inclusion of diverse racial and ethnic groups to ensure that interventions do not take a “one size fits all” approach by assuming that the pathways leading to psychopathology in a dominant racial group operate similarly for children who have been understudied in psychological research.

Developmental psychopathology researchers may come closer to identifying causal mechanisms through intervention research (Davis, Hankin, Swales, & Hoffman, Reference Davis, Hankin, Swales and Hoffman2018; Mackiewicz Seghete et al., Reference Mackiewicz Seghete, Graham, Lapidus, Jackson, Doyle, Feryn and Dimidjian2020). By including putative biological mechanisms in intervention design (Cicchetti & Gunnar, Reference Cicchetti and Gunnar2008), researchers can determine if their intervention targets change the biological mechanism of interest (Fisher, Gunnar, Dozier, Bruce, & Pears, Reference Fisher, Gunnar, Dozier, Bruce and Pears2006). For example, research with children who have been maltreated (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Toth, & Sturge-Apple, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch, Toth and Sturge-Apple2011), in foster care (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Gunnar, Dozier, Bruce and Pears2006), and children with child protective services involvement (Bernard, Hostinar, & Dozier, Reference Bernard, Hostinar and Dozier2015) has shown that early intervention leads to typical regulation of the neuroendocrine system. This important research allows us to move beyond descriptive studies of how a predictor is related to an outcome, either concurrently or even prospectively, to consider mechanism of effect in human studies.

Conclusions

In the 1980s, Cicchetti described how clinicians and academicians rarely interact, which spawned the new field of development and psychopathology. RDoC in many ways is another iteration of this mutually informative process – a way for basic scientists studying psychopathology to produce rigorous and reproducible research that will lead to improvements in early identification, prevention, and treatment of mental illness. As others also have articulated, we believe developmental psychopathologists will be critical in these important efforts, and in many ways, the roots of RDoC were firmly planted decades prior by developmental psychopathologists (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1966; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984). In this editorial, we highlighted how the early origins of developmental psychopathology included RDoC principles before the RDoC framework was developed. We described here three important ways in which developmental psychopathology and RDoC can mutually inform one another.

In this special issue, we aim to broaden our understanding of how RDoC approaches can be leveraged to better understand the mechanisms and processes implicated in the development of psychopathology. We provide examples of studies in this special issue that exemplify how development and psychopathology conceptualizations can improve upon the existing RDoC framework. Studies in this issue are diverse and identify transdiagnostic symptoms (like sleep), transdiagnostic risk factors (like maternal hostility and racial discrimination), applications of RDoC to parents, and markers, mechanisms, and processes of risk before the expression of the disorder is apparent (Brown et al., this issue; Humphreys et al., this issue; Karalunas et al., this issue; Ostlund et al., this issue; Vargas et al., this issue). We believe that research presented in this special issue generates novel research questions espoused by development and psychopathology researchers to advance the developmental science of RDoC.

Funding Statement

This manuscript was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under the following awards: R21MH109777 (to S.C. and E.C.), R01MH119070 (to S.C. and E.C.), and T32MH015755-39 (to D.C.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or the National Institutes of Health.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1966). The classification of children's psychiatric symptoms: A factor-analytic study. Psychological Monographs, 80, 137. doi:10.1037/h0093906CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T., & Cicchetti, D. (2019). Emotion dysregulation and emerging psychopathology: A transdiagnostic, transdisciplinary perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 31, 799804. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000671CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2020). RDoc and psychopathology among youth: Misplaced assumptions and an agenda for future research. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 49, 322340. doi:10.1080/15374416.2020.1750022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P., Klein, D. N., Crowell, S. E., Derbidge, C., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2009). Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A biology × Sex × environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 735770. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000418CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, K., Hostinar, C. E., & Dozier, M. (2015). Intervention effects on diurnal cortisol rhythms of child protective services–referred infants in early childhood: Preschool follow-up results of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 169, 112. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.2369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyce, W. T., Frank, E., Jensen, P. S., Kessler, R. C., Nelson, C. A., & Steinberg, L. (1998). Social context in developmental psychopathology: Recommendations for the future research from the MacArthur network on psychopathology and development. The MacArthur foundation network on psychopathology and development. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 143164. doi:10.1017/s0954579498001552CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. A., Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). A developmental investigation of inattentiveness and hyperactivity. Child Development, 66, 37. doi:10.2307/1131189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Oliveri, M. E., & Insel, T. (2014). A neurodevelopmental perspective on the research domain criteria (RDoc) framework. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 350353. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1998). Contextualism and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 137141. doi.org/10.1017/s0945979498001540CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Dawson, G. (2002). Editorial: Multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 417420. doi:10.1017/S0954579402003012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2008). Integrating biological processes into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 737743. doi:10.1017/s0954579408000357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rizley, R. (1981). Developmental perspectives on the etiology, intergenerational transmission, and sequelae of child maltreatment. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1981, 3155. doi:10.1002/cd.23219811104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597600. doi:10.1017/S0954579400007318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Toth, S. L., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2011). Normalizing the development of cortisol regulation in maltreated infants through preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 789800. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 533549. doi:10.1017/s0954579400004673CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conradt, E., Abar, B., Lester, B. M., LaGasse, L. L., Shankaran, S., Bada, H., … Hammond, J. A. (2014). Cortisol reactivity to social stress as a mediator of early adversity on risk and adaptive outcomes. Child Development, 85, 22792298. doi:10.1111/cdev.12316Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Hankin, B. L., Swales, D. A., & Hoffman, M. C. (2018). An experimental test of the fetal programming hypothesis: Can we reduce child ontogenetic vulnerability to psychopathology by decreasing maternal depression? Development and Psychopathology, 30, 787806. doi:10.1017/S0954579418000470CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Englund, M. M., Kuo, S. I.-C., Puig, J., & Collins, W. A. (2011). Early roots of adult competence: The significance of close relationships from infancy to early adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 490496. doi:10.1177/0165025411422994CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farber, E. A., Vaughn, B., & Egeland, B. (1981). The relationship of prenatal maternal anxiety to infant behavior and mother-infant interaction during the first six months of life. Early Human Development, 5, 267277. doi:10.1016/0378-3782(81)90034-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, P. A., Gunnar, M. R., Dozier, M., Bruce, J., & Pears, K. C. (2006). Effects of therapeutic interventions for foster children on behavioral problems, caregiver attachment, and stress regulatory neural systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 215225. doi:10.1196/annals.1376.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, P. A., Lester, B. M., DeGarmo, D. S., Lagasse, L. L., Lin, H., Shankaran, S., … Higgins, R. (2011). The combined effects of prenatal drug exposure and early adversity on neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 777788. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, J. C., Jamieson, J. P., Glenn, C. R., & Nock, M. K. (2015). How developmental psychopathology theory and research Can inform the research domain criteria (RDoc) project. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44, 280290. doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.873981CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garber, J., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2020). Developmental psychopathology and the research domain criteria: Friend or Foe? Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 49, 341352. doi:10.1080/15374416.2020.1753205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garvey, M., Avenevoli, S., & Anderson, K. (2016). The national institute of mental health research domain criteria and clinical research in child and adolescent psychiatry. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55, 9398. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2015.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M. (2016). Diversity and representation: Key issues for psychophysiological science: Diversity and representation. Psychophysiology, 53, 313. doi:10.1111/psyp.12566CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacono, W. G., Malone, S. M., & McGue, M. (2008). Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 325348. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T. R. (2009). Disruptive insights in psychiatry: Transforming a clinical discipline. Journal of Clinical Investigations, 119, 700705. doi:10.1172/JCI38832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T. R., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoc): Toward a New classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 748751. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozak, M. J., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2016). The NIMH research domain criteria initiative: Background, issues, and pragmatics: NIMH research domain criteria initiative. Psychophysiology, 53, 286297. doi:10.1111/psyp.12518CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebowitz, E. R., Gee, D. G., Pine, D. S., & Silverman, W. K. (2018). Implications of the research domain criteria project for childhood anxiety and its disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 64, 99109. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2018.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2014). The research domain criteria (RDoc): An analysis of methodological and conceptual challenges. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 62, 129139. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2014.07.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorber, M. F., & Egeland, B. (2009). Infancy parenting and externalizing psychopathology from childhood through adulthood: Developmental trends. Developmental Psychology, 45, 909912. doi:10.1037/a0015675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J., Allen, N., Estabrook, R., Pine, D. S., Rogers, C., Krogh-Jespersen, S., … Wakschlag, L. (2019). Mapping infant neurodevelopmental precursors of mental disorders: How synthetic cohorts & computational approaches can be used to enhance prediction of early childhood psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 123, 103484. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2019.103484CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackiewicz Seghete, K. L., Graham, A. M., Lapidus, J. A., Jackson, E. L. A., Doyle, O. J., Feryn, A. B., … Dimidjian, S. (2020). Protocol for a mechanistic study of mindfulness based cognitive therapy during pregnancy. Health Psychology, 39, 758766. doi:10.1037/hea0000870CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittal, V. A., & Wakschlag, L. S. (2017). Research domain criteria (RDoc) grows up: Strengthening neurodevelopment investigation within the RDoc framework. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216, 3035. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, S. E., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2012). Research domain criteria: Cognitive systems, neural circuits, and dimensions of behavior. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 2937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostlund, B. D., Vlisides-Henry, R. D., Crowell, S. E., Raby, K. L., Terrell, S., Brown, M. A., … Conradt, E. (2019). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation: Part II. Developmental origins of newborn neurobehavior. Development and Psychopathology, 31, 833846. doi:10.1017/S0954579419000440CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pianta, R. C., Egeland, B., & Hyatt, A. (1986). Maternal relationship history as an indicator of developmental risk. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 385398. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.1986.tb03471.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roisman, G. I., Aguilar, B., & Egeland, B. (2004). Antisocial behavior in the transition to adulthood: The independent and interactive roles of developmental history and emerging developmental tasks. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 857871. doi:10.1017/S0954579404040040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 265296. doi:10.1017/s0954579400003023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2020). Early life stress and development: Potential mechanisms for adverse outcomes. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 12, 34. doi:10.1186/s11689-020-09337-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (2009). The concept of development in developmental psychopathology. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 178183. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00103.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B. R., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2009). The development of the person: The minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakschlag, L. S., Estabrook, R., Petitclerc, A., Henry, D., Burns, J. L., Perlman, S. B., … Briggs-Gowan, M. L. (2015). Clinical implications of a dimensional approach: The normal:Abnormal Spectrum of early irritability. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 626634. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakschlag, L. S., Krogh-Jespersen, S., Estabrook, R., Hlutkowsky, C. O., Anderson, E. L., Burns, J., … Perlman, S. B. (2020). The early childhood irritability-related impairment interview (E-CRI): A novel method for assessing young children's developmentally impairing irritability. Behavior Therapy, 51, 294309. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2019.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed