Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-16T03:09:24.451Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental psychopathology: Our welcoming, inclusive, and eclectic intellectual home

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 February 2024

Grazyna Kochanska*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Danming An
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Grazyna Kochanska; Email: grazyna-kochanska@uiowa.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The integrative nature of developmental psychopathology is its defining and most remarkable feature. Since its inception, often identified with the special issue of Child Development (Cichetti, 1984), this new discipline has shattered barriers and divisions that until then had artificially compartmentalized the study of human development, and perhaps even psychology in general, and it has proposed new ways of integrative thinking about development. One, developmental psychopathology has programmatically integrated research on typical or adaptive and atypical or maladaptive developmental processes and demonstrated how those inform each other. Two, developmental psychopathology has promoted bridges between developmental research and other disciplines. Three, less explicitly but equally importantly, developmental psychopathology has abolished conceptual and empirical barriers that had existed among various theories and perspectives within developmental psychology by creating a welcoming niche for research inspired by theories often historically seen as contradictory or incompatible. Ideas originating in psychoanalytic, learning, cognitive, ethological, and sociocultural theories all find a welcoming home and seamlessly coexist in heuristically productive harmony within developmental psychopathology, inform each other, and generate exciting questions and insights. This eclectic and conceptually inclusive nature is one reason for developmental psychopathology’s lasting appeal and inspirational power.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Over the last four decades, developmental psychopathology has remarkably transformed developmental and clinical sciences. The 1984 special issue of Child Development, with Dante Cicchetti as the guest editor, containing a collection of influential theoretical and empirical articles, ushered in the new discipline (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Garmezy et al., Reference Garmezy, Masten and Tellegen1984; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984). Soon after, in 1989, the journal Development and Psychopathology was established, and, under Dante Cicchetti’s editorship, has become the leading venue for the quickly emerging and rapidly burgeoning field.

Ever since, this new field’s integrative power has been perhaps its most remarkable feature. Developmental psychopathology has shattered past barriers and divisions that had artificially partitioned, fragmented, and compartmentalized the study of human development, and perhaps psychology in general. That integrative power has been reflected in at least three inter-related ways.

Integrating the study of typical and atypical development

One, as its defining feature, the new discipline has famously proposed that we learn about typical development by studying psychopathology and that we learn about psychopathology by studying typical development. Typical and atypical development were conceptualized as informing each other, producing extraordinarily innovative models of development. Constructs of multifinality, equfinality, developmental cascades, and risk and resilience have redefined our thinking about developmental processes, childhood disorders, and adaptative and maladaptive trajectories (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2009; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1993; Rutter & Sroufe, Reference Rutter and Sroufe2000). We illustrate this approach below with just a few examples selected from the very large field.

Researchers investigating Theory of Mind have made great strides by studying typically developing children and children with autism. Our understanding of typical and maladaptive parenting has been informed by the extremely influential paper by Belsky (Reference Belsky1984), originally situated in the child maltreatment framework, but exerting a lasting influence on the study of parenting broadly conceptualized (Taraban & Shaw, Reference Taraban and Shaw2018). The study of adaptive and dysfunctional parenting has been further fueled by research on non-maltreated and maltreated children (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2016). The “normal:abnormal” framework applied to developmental phenomena has provided nuanced insights into both externalizing and internalizing psychopathology. Studying irritability and temper loss, Wakschlag and colleagues have distinguished between normative manifestations of early anger, noncompliance, defiance or temper loss and manifestations that reflect early risk and emerging psychopathology, thus informing our understanding of emotion regulation and dysregulation (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Estabrook, Petitclerc, Henry, Burns, Perlman, Voss, Pine, Leibenluft and Briggs-Gowan2015). Studying early signs of fear, worry, and sadness, they provided a nuanced, developmentally informed analysis of early risk for future internalizing psychopathology (Bufferd et al., Reference Bufferd, Olino and Dougherty2023; Buss et al., Reference Buss, Davis, Kiel, Brooker, Beekman and Early2013; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Sherlock, Blackwell, Burns, Krogh-Jespersen, Gershon, Cella, Buss and Luby2023). The study of self-regulation in children from typical and at-risk, unstable, low-resource, harsh environments has provided insights into adaptive and maladaptive aspects of self-regulatory development (Sturge-Apple et al., Reference Sturge-Apple, Davies, Cicchetti, Hentges and Coe2016).

Integrating psychology with other disciplines

Two, developmental psychopathology catalyzed an integration of developmental psychology with other disciplines, such as psychiatry, neurobiology, neurophysiology, relationship sciences, or social psychology. Bridges with neuroscience, genetics, and biology have been among the most productive and heuristically powerful. Research on temperament (De Pauw & Mervielde, Reference De Pauw and Mervielde2010; Gartstein et al., Reference Gartstein, Putnam and Rothbart2012; Muris & Ollendick, Reference Muris and Ollendick2005; Nigg, Reference Nigg2006; Rothbart & Bates, Reference Rothbart, Bates, Damon, Lerner and Eisenberg2006; Shiner & Caspi, Reference Shiner and Caspi2003) and its role in both adaptive and at-risk developmental pathways elucidated biological foundations of internalizing (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Zeytinoglu, Valadez, Buzzell, Morales and Henderson2022; Kagan, Reference Kagan2022; Whalen et al., Reference Whalen, Sylvester and Luby2017) and externalizing psychopathology (Nigg, Reference Nigg2017). Research on early adversity, deprivation, and stress (Hostinar & Gunnar, Reference Hostinar and Gunnar2013; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Fox and Zeanah2023; Pollak & Smith, Reference Pollak and Smith2021) highlighted its biological consequences for children. Research on biological factors fueled the highly influential and heuristically fertile frameworks of diathesis-stress, differential susceptibility, and plasticity (Belsky & Pluess, Reference Belsky and Pluess2009; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Boyce, Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn2011).

Bridges with social psychology and relationship sciences have also been productive. Social psychological constructs, such as communal orientation (Clark & Mills, Reference Clark, Mills, Van Lange, Kruglanski and Higgins2012), interpersonal acceptance (Rohner & Lansford, Reference Rohner and Lansford2017), and reciprocity (Maccoby, Reference Maccoby1992), integrated with socialization frameworks, led to the notion of mutually responsive orientation (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Boldt and Goffin2019), and it, in turn, informed our understanding of socialization in both typical and at-risk environments (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Kim and Boldt2013, Reference Kochanska, Boldt and Goffin2019).

Integrating and welcoming developmental theories and perspectives historically seen as incompatible

Those first two ways in which developmental psychopathology eliminated old divisions are broadly acknowledged. But there is also a third way, perhaps the least appreciated: Less explicitly but equally importantly, developmental psychopathology has abolished the conceptual and empirical barriers and distinctions that had existed among various “grand” theories and perspectives within developmental psychology. That integration of theories and perspectives historically seen as incompatible is as remarkable and heuristically fertile as the first two forms.

Every year, as we teach courses on social-emotional development, we typically begin by contrasting various theoretical perspectives – for example, psychoanalytic, learning, ethological, cognitive, sociocultural theories – and then we describe their very different portrayals of development and different answers to the key developmental questions. We review the often-spirited historical arguments in the field and portray those various perspectives as contradictory and incompatible. This is a deliberate pedagogical exercise to pique students’ curiosity and convey the rich, diverse theoretical canvass and the fascinating history of our discipline.

We intentionally highlight contrasting views the various theories have offered on the key questions: What roles do nature and nurture play? What comprises nature and nurture? Who is more “in charge” of development – the parent or the child? How does development progress – is it incremental and continuous or does it happen in qualitatively different stages? What changes and what stays the same? What makes children different from one another? What is the role of past experiences? Can we predict the child’s future from his or her past?

Emphasizing contrasts among seemingly incompatible and contradictory views and positions historically staked out within various perspectives is pedagogically useful; and to some extent, it is a true portrayal of past diverse traditions in developmental science and psychology in general. And yet, such compartmentalized, segmented vision of our field has been vastly redefined – in fact, rendered moot – by the ascension of developmental psychopathology.

Developmental psychopathology has revolutionized the “big picture” due to the remarkable openness and richness of its conceptual framework that welcomes and seamlessly integrates diverse perspectives. Psychoanalytic, learning, ethological, biological, cognitive, and sociocultural ideas are all incorporated in a meaningful manner. Although historically those theories may have indeed been incompatible, in developmental psychopathology, they coexist in a remarkable conceptual harmony, inform and complement each other, resulting in new insights and a new synergy – a heuristically rich and complex portrayal of development and socialization.

Cicchetti’s original paper (1984), by meaningfully weaving in contributions of Freud, Erikson, and Piaget already implied that such would be the future of this new discipline. Other early papers in that issue, foundational to developmental psychopathology (e.g., Belsky, Reference Belsky1984; Sroufe & Rutter, Reference Sroufe and Rutter1984), and other works that appeared at about the same time (Masten & Garmezy, Reference Masten and Garmezy1985) evinced a similar integrative spirit. Consequently, developmental psychopathology has become an inclusive, welcoming, eclectic intellectual home for scholars representing a myriad conceptual perspectives and theories. Ever since, that richness, inclusiveness, and theoretical breadth have been a source of inspiration, and one reason for the continuing appeal and growing heuristic power of the developmental psychopathology framework.

Below, we present a few examples illustrating how developmental psychopathology has accommodated, incorporated, and productively drawn from very different historical perspectives on human development. We then briefly review the work in our laboratory, where we explicitly seek to take advantage of all three forms of developmental psychopathology’s integrative strategies: Studying typical and atypical development, drawing from other fields of psychology, and integrating multiple theories in an eclectic, conceptually comprehensive approach.

Psychoanalytic theories and developmental psychopathology

Although many of Freud’s specific notions have been dismissed and are often disparaged, derided, or ridiculed, his fundamental deeper ideas about human psyche – albeit substantially recast and transformed – have been echoed in many research themes in developmental psychopathology (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1986; Westen, Reference Westen1998). We appreciate Freud’s original emphasis on affective processes and his portrayal of the mind as not merely reflecting reality but actively transforming it to help cope with anxiety, fear, anger, and other aversive emotions through “defense mechanisms”. In developmental psychopathology, those topics are mirrored by our appreciation of emotions and emotion regulation and dysregulation, widely viewed as key transdiagnostic markers in research on origins of adaptive and maladaptive development.

We also embrace Freud’s beliefs in the significance, importance, and complexity of the early affective experiences that can frame future development through unconscious or partly conscious processes. Freud was the first to emphasize that early relational experiences, even if preverbal, unconscious, and represented in what we would call implicit, nondeclarative memory, are critically important. They are – again in today’s language – “developmentally privileged”, carried forward, and framing future trajectories, often in a complex and nonlinear manner. Within modern frameworks, developmental psychopathology scholars have highlighted the critical impact of early relationships, written extensively about how that process may occur, and delineated its various forms: direct, moderated effects, indirect effects; or cascades (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Boldt and Goffin2019; Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1986; Reference Sroufe2005; Reference Sroufe2013; Reference Sroufe, Cassidy and Shaver2016). We also revisit Freud’s belief that emotional experiences can be expressed in dreams, although we view the process through the lens of the contemporary attachment theory (Mikulincer et al., Reference Mikulincer, Shaver and Avihou-Kanza2011).

Freud’s follower, Erikson, transformed Freudian ideas in ways remarkably aligned with contemporary tenets of developmental psychopathology. We view development through a strikingly similar lens. We consider age-salient adaptation tasks (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe, Cassidy and Shaver2016), tied to developmentally changing challenges, carrying a potential for resilience, adaptation, and growth or a risk of maladaptation or failure, and setting the stage for, or framing future development.

Learning theories and developmental psychopathology

Classic learning research produced an important finding that fits well with developmental psychopathology. Studying effectiveness of punishment, Parke (Reference Parke1969) observed that punishment delivered by a “nurturant agent” – an experimenter who had first engaged in a friendly interaction with the child – was more effective than one delivered by a neutral agent. Although not much noticed at the time, that insightful result dovetails with current research on relationships as developmental contexts whose quality can powerfully moderate effects of parenting strategies (Bendel-Stenzel et al., Reference Bendel-Stenzel, An and Kochanska2023; Deater-Deckard et al., Reference Deater-Deckard, Ivy and Petrill2006; Kim & Kochanska, Reference Kim and Kochanska2012).

Learning theories have also inspired research by the Oregon Social Learning Center that has produced sophisticated microanalytic analyses of coercive family systems and nuanced descriptions of the process of entrenching or cascading mutually aversive and destructive parent-child dynamics. That research demonstrated how difficult, hard-to-manage children elicit harsh parental control that leads to more child defiance, and then to more parental coercion (Dishion & Patterson, Reference Dishion, Patterson, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; Scaramella & Leve, Reference Scaramella and Leve2004; Tiberio et al., Reference Tiberio, Capaldi, Kerr, Bertrand, Pears and Owen2016). Over time, those dynamics cascade, resulting in an entrenched, escalating mutually adversarial parent-child relationship, and finally, in maladaptive child outcomes. That approach remains prominent and influential in developmental psychopathology, and widely accepted as elucidating origins of externalizing psychopathology in children and youth and informing treatment and prevention.

Ethology and developmental psychopathology

Ethological theories – most prominently, Bowlby’s attachment theory – have been uniquely synergistic with questions and constructs of developmental psychopathology (Sroufe et al., Reference Sroufe, Carlson, Levy and Egeland1999; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe1986, Reference Sroufe, Cassidy and Shaver2016); Bowlby could be legitimately seen as its forerunner, and the construct of attachment has been enormously influential in developmental psychopathology. Bowlby had recast psychodynamic tenets on the importance of early relationships in an evolutionary framework (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1969/1982). Ever since, attachment theory has remained a conceptually, empirically, and heuristically powerful force in developmental psychopathology, as it embodies its multiple key principles.

Attachment is an evolutionarily based proximity-regulating biobehavioral system, amenable to rigorous measurement; secure attachment provides the child with confidence in protection and helps manage threat, stress, and distress at the behavioral, emotional, and physiological levels. Analyses of secure and insecure attachment organizations, representing examples of adaptive and maladaptive developmental processes informing each other, have had an enormous impact on research on risk and resilience across the lifespan (e.g., Cassidy & Shaver, Reference Cassidy and Shaver2016; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Simpson and Berlin2021; Thompson, Reference Thompson and Lamb2015, Reference Thompson, Cassidy and Shaver2016). The view of development in attachment theory, depicting indirect, complex, nonlinear probabilistic effects, highlighting adaptive and maladaptive trajectories in development, is fully consistent with contemporary views in developmental psychopathology (Kochanska & Kim, Reference Kochanska and Kim2012; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2005, Reference Sroufe, Cassidy and Shaver2016).

More recently, attachment’s role has been broadened to include also another developmental goal: To promote the child’s positive, receptive orientation toward the parent. A secure attachment renders the child receptive to parental influence and eager to embrace it. It inaugurates positive parent-child socialization dynamics, the parent-child implicitly cooperative interpersonal set, permeated with mutual good will and infused with shared positive feelings. Such orientation is especially significant at the beginning of the second year, as it coincides with the typical onset of parental control and discipline, and child compliance and noncompliance. By contrast, an insecure attachment can launch the dyad on a mutually adversarial and resentful trajectory, with the parent and the child becoming increasingly antagonistic (An et al., Reference An, Kochanska, Yeager, Sivagurunathan, Praska, Campbell and Shin2021; Goffin et al., Reference Goffin, Boldt and Kochanska2018; Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Kim and Boldt2015; Shaver et al., Reference Shaver, Mikulincer, Gross, Stern, Cassidy, Cassidy and Shaver2016; Thompson, Reference Thompson, Cassidy and Shaver2016; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Kondo-Ikemura, Posada, Richters, Gunnar and Sroufe1990).

Cognitive theories and information-processing theories and developmental psychopathology

In recent decades, cognitive theories have found a particularly heuristically fertile niche in developmental psychopathology, due to their conceptual and empirical interface with attachment theory’s key constructs of Internal Working Models (IWMs). Research on representations, implicit and explicit memory, scripts, procedural knowledge, and mentalization – studied in both parents and children – has become one of the most dynamic hubs in developmental psychopathology.

Research on parents’ cognitive processing and mentalization, with the foci on reflective functioning (Katznelson, Reference Katznelson2014; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017; Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2008), mind-mindedness (McMahon & Bernier, Reference McMahon and Bernier2017; Meins, Reference Meins1997, Reference Meins1999, Reference Meins2013; Slade, Reference Slade2005), relational schemas (Sher-Censor, Reference Sher-Censor2015), secure base scripts (Fraley et al., Reference Fraley, Roisman and Haltigan2013; Groh & Haydon, Reference Groh and Haydon2018; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Fraley, Groh, Steele, Vaughn, Bost, Verissimo and Roisman2015), and attributions (Snarr et al., Reference Snarr, Slep and Grande2009) has substantially informed our understanding of intergenerational links between parents’ emotional histories and their adaptive and maladaptive parenting (An et al., Reference An, Bendel-Stenzel and Kochanska2022). Research that has applied well-established infant cognition paradigms, such as violation of expectations, to children’s representations of parents, provided a unique, innovative window into infants’ IWMs of the caregiver, self, and others (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Dweck, Chen, Stern, Ok and Barth2010). Memory paradigms have also proved useful (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Spritz and Crnic1996; Kirsh & Cassidy, Reference Kirsh and Cassidy1997). Rigorously studied children’s narratives elucidate their representations of parents, self, and relationships (Toth et al., Reference Toth, Cicchetti, Macfie and Emde1997, Reference Toth, Cicchetti and Kim2002, Reference Toth, Rogosch, Sturge-Apple and Cicchetti2009). Several systematic reviews summarize this emerging and vibrant field (Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Jones and Shaver2013; Dykas & Cassidy, Reference Dykas and Cassidy2011; Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Boldt and Goffin2019; Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Rice and Cassidy2015).

Research on social-information processing, implemented to explain mechanisms of children’s aggression, represents another example of a cognitive perspective finding an important key niche in developmental psychopathology. This elegant, highly influential, and heuristically productive application of social cognition and attributional theories has elucidated how biased information processing explains origins of aggression in children and youth, leading to cascading risks to peer rejection and entrenched social problems (Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1994). Notably, those early biases, due largely to experiences of early abusive care, coalesce into a broad defensive set that frames maladaptive outcomes into adult age (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Bai, Godwin, Lansford, Bates, Pettit and Jones2022).

Sociocultural and ecological theories and developmental psychopathology

Developmental psychopathology has also incorporated extensions beyond individual-level factors to emphasize the roles of contexts and social environment in the unfolding adaptive and maladaptive trajectories. Drawing from Bronfenbrenner’s model of development as embedded in multilayered ecological systems (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Lerner and Damon2006), theories focused on broader contexts have found a welcoming niche in developmental psychopathology, and they provided rich tools and resources for understanding adaptive and maladaptive trajectories of development and phenomena of risk and resilience.

Decades of research on developmental risk and resilience have highlighted the concepts of adverse childhood experiences and cumulative risk, and the ways to mitigate their negative impact and to promote resilience (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Sheridan, Belsky and McLaughlin2022). Much of the early focus on resilience has been on individuals’ sensitivity to the risk and protective factors in the environment (e.g., differential susceptibility). In the recent years, the definition of resilience has been extended to incorporate a multisystem perspective, which views resilience as a dynamic process that can exist both within the individual and in the environment, connecting individuals and families to the resources necessary for positive adaptation (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021). The process of resilience can occur at multiple levels and cascade across levels.

This evolving view of resilience has elucidated both research and intervention work in developmental psychopathology, especially in the current contexts of social justice, multiculturalism, and crises (e.g., pandemic, wars). A growing body of literature has examined the interplay between family dynamics and broader sociocultural contexts, and emphasized the crucial role of parental socialization in mitigating risks in the social environment, as well as the importance of building an equal, inclusive, and safe society and addressing structural barriers for promoting positive dynamics and adaptive functioning among families and children (Dunbar et al., Reference Dunbar, Zeytinoglu and Leerkes2022; Eltanamly et al., Reference Eltanamly, Leijten, Jak and Overbeek2021; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Barbarin and Cassidy2022; Tyrell & Masten, Reference Tyrell and Masten2022).

Adopting an eclectic, integrative perspective in research on parental socialization of children’s adaptive and maladaptive developmental trajectories

Developmental psychopathology has offered researchers a rich set of perspectives and tools for comprehensively understanding children’s development. Below, we will briefly illustrate how the eclectic flexibility and inclusiveness afforded by developmental psychopathology have informed our research program. We address a perennial developmental question: Why do some children embrace their parents’ influence and embark on adaptive, positive developmental trajectories toward prosocial, internalized, rule-abiding conduct and robust social competence? Why do other children reject and resent their parents’ influence and embark on maladaptive paths toward callousness, disregard for conduct rules and others’ feelings, antisocial behavior, and impoverished competence?

Enriching the learning theories’ perspective on early traits and later adjustment: parent-child relationship as a foundation for multifinal cascades

In our work, we bridge children’s biologically-based traits with relationship science by examining multifinality in trajectories from early temperament to later adjustment in the context of family relationships. By integrating perspectives and constructs from the biological, learning, attachment, cognitive, and ecological theories, we aim to elucidate children’s adaptive and maladaptive developmental cascades.

Ever since Bell (Reference Bell1968) highlighted the importance of child effects in socialization, several biologically-based traits have been seen as early markers of risk for maladaptive development. Often referred to as “child difficulty”, those traits include anger proneness, challenging, hard-to-manage temperament, or poor regulation, and more recently, also molecular genetic markers (Brock et al., Reference Brock, Kochanska and Boldt2017). We have drawn from that research, deploying multilevel measures of child difficulty (behavioral, genetic, psychophysiological, and parent rated).

Further, we have drawn from the above-mentioned elegant body of research on coercive family systems, inspired by learning theories. That work has persuasively shown that “child difficulty” elicits harsh, power-assertive control, which in turn leads to children’s escalating negative socialization outcomes. We have noticed, however, growing evidence that shows that such negative cascade from child difficulty to parental power assertion to negative socialization outcomes is far from universal. In the language of developmental psychopathology, research has increasingly revealed substantial multifinality in paths that unfold from early child difficult temperament (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996; Kim & Kochanska, Reference Kim and Kochanska2021). Consequently, we explored potential factors that can alter such negative cascades.

We observed that the learning perspective was largely silent on the role of the early parent-child relationship in the first years of life, prior to the onset of control. To address this gap, we have next reached to Bowlby’s construct of attachment, as well as constructs of reciprocity, cooperation, and communal relationship from social psychology (Clark & Mills, Reference Clark, Mills, Van Lange, Kruglanski and Higgins2012; Maccoby, Reference Maccoby1992). We proposed that the quality of the early parent-child relationship (security, mutuality) in the first two years is key in determining whether the cascade from child difficulty to parental negative, harsh control to child maladaptive outcomes will – or will not – unfold. Specifically, we expected that such adversarial, maladaptive cascades depicted by the learning literature would be present in dyads whose early relationships are suboptimal, insecure, and negative, but absent or defused in dyads whose early relationships are optimal and secure.

Over three decades, we have obtained converging, remarkably consistent evidence, across multiple studies, designs, ages, and diverse measures of all constructs, supporting our model (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Boldt and Goffin2019; Kochanska & Kim, Reference Kochanska and Kim2012). Early relational experience does alter future socialization processes that unfold in parent-child dyads. Dyads with temperamentally difficult children are indeed at high risk, but only if they have a history of an early suboptimal relationship. This conclusion is consistent with tenets of developmental psychopathology, but rarely integrated with the learning perspectives. The early relationship is an organizing core in development, always integrated with later experience and never lost; it has a distinct, privileged impact, framing the child’s subsequent transactions with the environment (Fraley et al., Reference Fraley, Roisman and Haltigan2013; Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2005, Reference Sroufe2013, Reference Sroufe, Cassidy and Shaver2016). We have also incorporated the sociocultural perspective by testing our model in both low-risk community families and in high-risk mother-child dyads, struggling with a harsh ecology of poverty and multiple forms of adversity. Our model has informed a randomized intervention we deployed in the latter sample, targeting the quality of the mother-child relationship. The intervention exerted its primary influence on the cascade from child difficulty to maternal negative control to child maladjustment by weakening the first link: between child difficulty and maternal negative control (Brock & Kochanska, Reference Brock and Kochanska2016).

Mechanisms that account for multifinality: contributions of cognitive and information-processing theories and the constructs of internal working models (IWMs)

We are now asking the next generation of questions: Why and how do such divergent cascades emerge? What mechanisms account for such multifinality?

Here, we have drawn from cognitive and information-processing theories. We have proposed that the parent’s and the child’s representations of each other, evolving in early relationships – IWMs in attachment theory – are key mechanisms that account for the cascades of parent-child dynamics unfolding in suboptimal relationships, as compared to optimal ones.

In suboptimal relationships, the parent’s IWMs of the child are characterized by impoverished reflective functioning, poor mind-mindedness, negative relational schemas, and hostile attributions, and the child’s IWMs include representations of the parent as untrustworthy, unresponsive, rejecting, hostile, and unfair. By contrast, in optimal relationships, parents have reflective and positive views of their children and children perceive the parents as trustworthy, responsive, accepting, well-intentioned, and benevolent (Bretherton & Munholland, Reference Bretherton, Munholland, Cassidy and Shaver2008; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Sroufe and Egeland2004; Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Jones and Shaver2013; Dykas & Cassidy, Reference Dykas and Cassidy2011; Main et al., Reference Main, Kaplan and Cassidy1985; Thompson, Reference Thompson, Cassidy and Shaver2016; Toth et al., Reference Toth, Rogosch, Sturge-Apple and Cicchetti2009). Of note, children’s early representations of their parents generalize to other relationships, and more broadly to perceptions of safety and stress (Smith & Pollak, Reference Smith and Pollak2021; Thompson, Reference Thompson, Thompson, Simpson and Berlin2021).

We then integrated those concepts into a “dual-moderator” model, in which the parent’s IWM of the child moderates the links between child difficulty and parental control, and the child’s IWM of the parent moderates the links between parental control and developmental outcomes. For a parent with less reflective, negative, and hostile IWM of the child, the child’s difficult, angry, hard-to-manage traits easily trigger harsh, angry, negative, rejecting control (Scaramella & Leve, Reference Scaramella and Leve2004; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Dishion, Shaw, Wilson, Winter and Patterson2014, Reference Smith, Dishion, Shaw and Wilson2015). By contrast, for a parent with a rich, reflective, positive IWM of the child, the same child traits do not trigger negative control; indeed, they may even elicit supportive and empathic control (Dix, Reference Dix1991). Thus, the parent’s perception of the child accounts for the moderated link between child difficulty and parental negative control.

In turn, a child who has a negative IWM of the parent is biased to perceive parental control as hostile, unfair, mean-spirited, and arbitrary (Gershoff, Reference Gershoff2002; Grusec & Goodnow, Reference Grusec and Goodnow1994). The child then resents and rejects parental influence, ultimately leading to poor developmental outcomes, particularly disruptive behavior problems. By contrast, a child who has a positive, trusting IWM of the parent, comes to view control – even if firm – as benevolent, fair, and well-intentioned. That child willingly embraces socialization, entering a path to positive outcomes and competence – and as a result, the maladaptive cascade is “defused” (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Boldt and Goffin2019). Thus, the child’s perception of the parent accounts for the moderated link between parental harsh control and poor developmental outcomes. Preliminary evidence has supported such a “dual moderator” model (An & Kochanska, Reference An and Kochanska2020; Kochanska & An, Reference Kochanska and An2023).

The future of developmental psychopathology: research on parental socialization as a case in point

The promise of developmental psychopathology, as articulated in the visionary special issue (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984), has been fully realized. The new discipline has transformed how we think about adaptive and maladaptive development. We conclude with a few examples regarding the future of our area of interest – parental socialization.

We expect to see all three integrative directions to continue to fuel and inform research on socialization. Typical or adaptive and atypical or maladaptive trajectories of parenting and socialization will continue to be studied. For example, understanding parents’ early experiences, including parental history of trauma, can inform our understanding of both destructive, high-risk and positive, resilience-promoting parental early experiences (Narayan et al., Reference Narayan, Lieberman and Masten2021).

Bridges between developmental research and other disciplines will continue to be built. For example, the integration of parenting research with the flourishing disciplines of neuroscience and genetics will inform our understanding of infants’ and parents’ biologically-based traits that influence both the parent’s (Groh & Haydon, Reference Groh and Haydon2018) and the child’s (Slagt et al., Reference Slagt, Dubas, Deković and van Aken2016) roles in the socialization process.

Finally, various theories and perspectives, historically seen as incompatible, will continue to inform our understanding of parenting and complement each other. We will study early relationships, drawing from psychoanalytic and attachment theories, as critical for socialization process. We will continue to apply nuanced analyses of learning processes that occur in parent–child control interactions and to study how those processes are altered and framed by the context of the early parent-child relationship. We will make strides in our understanding of the parent’s and the child’s representations of each other, recruiting cognitive research methods and paradigms such as eye-tracking or explicit and implicit memory tasks (Dykas & Cassidy, Reference Dykas and Cassidy2011; Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Rice and Cassidy2015). Such integrated approach may elucidate some long-standing thorny questions, such as how children come to form generalized representations from their separate representations of each parent. We will examine sociocultural contexts in which socialization processes occur and study those contexts as sources of risk and resilience (Causadias, Reference Causadias2013).

The integrative spirit and inspirational power of developmental psychopathology have not only not diminished since 1984, but have kept growing, remaining an engine of progress. We envision a lasting bright future for our discipline across the full spectrum of research on human development.

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the grants from National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH63096, K02 MH01446) and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01 HD069171, R01 HD091047, and R01 HD110427) to Grazyna Kochanska. We thank all Child Lab team members and all the families participating in our research for their contributions over the years.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

An, D., Bendel-Stenzel, L. C., & Kochanska, G. (2022). Negative internal working models as mechanisms that link mothers’ and fathers’ personality with their parenting: A short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Personality, 90(6), 10041020. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12711 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
An, D., & Kochanska, G. (2020). Parents’ early representations of their children moderate socialization processes: Evidence from two studies. Development and Psychopathology, 34(3), 823840. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001546 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
An, D., Kochanska, G., Yeager, N. C., Sivagurunathan, N. M., Praska, R. L., Campbell, R. J., & Shin, S. Y. (2021). Children’s emerging receptive, positive orientation toward their parents in the network of early attachment relationships. Attachment and Human Development, 23(5), 687709. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1906722 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75(2), 8195. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025583 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55(1), 8396. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129836 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Spritz, B., & Crnic, K. (1996). Infant attachment security and affective-cognitive information processing at age 3. Psychological Science, 7(2), 111114. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00339.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendel-Stenzel, L., An, D., & Kochanska, G. (2023). Revisiting the debate on effects of parental power-assertive control in two longitudinal studies: Early attachment security as a moderator. Attachment and Human Development, 25(5), 461486. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2023.2262979 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment: Attachment and loss, vol. 1. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (2008). Internal working models in attachment relationships: Elaborating a central construct in attachment theory. In Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical application (2nd ed. pp. 102127). Guilford.Google Scholar
Brock, R. L., & Kochanska, G. (2016). Toward a developmentally informed approach to parenting interventions: Seeking hidden effects. Development and Psychopathology, 28(2), 583593. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000607 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, R. L., Kochanska, G., & Boldt, L. J. (2017). Interplay between children’s biobehavioral plasticity and interparental relationship in the origins of internalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 31(8), 10401050. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000335 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Lerner, R. M., & Damon, W. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Theoretical models of human development , 1, (6th ed. pp. 793828). Wiley.Google Scholar
Bufferd, S. J., Olino, T. M., & Dougherty, L. R. (2023). Quantifying severity of preschool-aged children’s internalizing behaviors: A daily diary analysis. Assessment, 30(1), 190209. https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911211046661 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, K. A., Davis, E. L., Kiel, E. J., Brooker, R. J., Beekman, C., & Early, M. C. (2013). Dysregulated fear predicts social wariness and social anxiety symptoms during kindergarten. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 42(5), 603616. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.769170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction of experience: A longitudinal study of representation and behavior. Child Development, 75(1), 6683. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00654.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J., Jones, J. D., & Shaver, P. R. (2013). Contributions of attachment theory and research: A framework for future research, translation, and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 14151434. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579413000692 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.) (2016). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd edn.) Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M. (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 13751398. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections, projections. Developmental Review, 13(4), 471502. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1993.1021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: Illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 187211. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033259 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579400007318 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2009). The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(1-2), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. R. (2012). A theory of communal (and exchange) relationships. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. W., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (pp. 232250). Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249222.n38 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Pauw, S. S. W., & Mervielde, I. (2010). Temperament, personality and developmental psychopathology: A review based on the conceptual dimensions underlying childhood traits. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 41(3), 313329. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-009-0171-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deater-Deckard, K., Ivy, L., & Petrill, S. A. (2006). Maternal warmth moderates the link between physical punishment and child externalizing problems: A parent - offspring behavior genetic analysis. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6(1), 5978. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327922par0601_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (2006). The development and ecology of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology. (vol. 3, 2nd ed. pp. 503541). Wiley.Google Scholar
Dix, T. H. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 325. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Bai, Y., Godwin, J., Lansford, J. E., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Jones, D. (2022). A defensive mindset: A pattern of social information processing that develops early and predicts life course outcomes. Child Development, 93(4), e357e378. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13751 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, A. S., Zeytinoglu, S., & Leerkes, E. M. (2022). When is parental suppression of black children’s negative emotions adaptive? The role of preparation for racial bias and children’s resting cardiac vagal tone. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 50(2), 163176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00779-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykas, J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: Theory and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1), 1946. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021367 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000611 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Sheridan, M. A., Belsky, J., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2022). Why and how does early adversity influence development? Toward an integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 447471. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579421001838 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eltanamly, H., Leijten, P., Jak, S., & Overbeek, G. (2021). Parenting in times of war: A meta-analysis and qualitative synthesis of war exposure, parenting, and child adjustment. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 22(1), 147160. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838019833001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., Zeytinoglu, S., Valadez, E. A., Buzzell, G. A., Morales, S., & Henderson, H. A. (2022). Annual Research Review: Developmental pathways linking early behavioral inhibition to later anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(4), 537561. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13702 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraley, R. C., Roisman, G. I., & Haltigan, J. D. (2013). The legacy of early experiences in development: Formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 109126. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 97111. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129837 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gartstein, M. A., Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2012). Etiology of preschool behavior problems: Contributions of temperament attributes in early childhood. Infant Mental Health Journal, 33(2), 197211. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 539579. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goffin, K. C., Boldt, L. J., & Kochanska, G. (2018). A secure base from which to cooperate: Security, child and parent willing stance, and adaptive and maladaptive outcomes in two longitudinal studies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(5), 10611075. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0352-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groh, A. M., & Haydon, K. C. (2018). Mothers’ neural and behavioral responses to their infants’ distress cues: The role of secure base script knowledge. Psychological Science, 29(2), 242253. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617730320 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline methods on the child’s internalization of values: A reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 419. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.1.4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hostinar, C. E., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). The developmental effects of early life stress: An overview of current theoretical frameworks. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(5), 400406. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413488889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C., Chen, F. S., Stern, H. L., Ok, S. J., & Barth, M. (2010). At the intersection of social and cognitive development: Internal working models of attachment in infancy. Cognitive Science, 34(5), 807825. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01112.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (2022). Temperamental and theoretical contributions to clinical psychology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 18(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071720-014404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katznelson, H. (2014). Reflective functioning: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(2), 107117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.12.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2012). Child temperament moderates effects of parent-child mutuality on self-regulation: A relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Development, 83(4), 12751289. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01778.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., & Kochanska, G. (2021). Family sociodemographic resources moderate the path from toddlers’ hard-to-manage temperament to parental control to disruptive behavior in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 33(1), 160172. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001664 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirsh, S. J., & Cassidy, J. (1997). Preschoolers’ attention to and memory for attachment-relevant information. Child Development, 68(6), 11431153. https://doi.org/10.2307/1132297 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & An, D. (2023). The parent’s and the child’s internal working models of each other moderate cascades from child difficulty to socialization outcomes: Preliminary evidence for dual moderation? Development and Psychopathology, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422001365 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Boldt, L. J., & Goffin, K. C. (2019). Early relational experience: A foundation for the unfolding dynamics of parent-child socialization. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 4147. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2012). Toward a new understanding of legacy of early attachments for future antisocial trajectories: Evidence from two longitudinal studies. Development and Psychopathology, 24(3), 783806. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000375 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., & Boldt, L. J. (2013). Origins of children’s externalizing behavior problems in low-income families: Toddlers’ willing stance toward their mothers as the missing link. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt1), 891901. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., & Boldt, L. J. (2015). (Positive) power to the child: The role of children’s willing stance toward parents in developmental cascades from toddler age to early preadolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4, Pt 1), 9871005. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000644 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luyten, P., Nijssens, L., Fonagy, P., & Mayes, L. C. (2017). Parental reflective functioning: Theory, research, and clinical applications. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 70(1), 174199. https://doi.org/10.1080/00797308.2016.1277901 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 10061017. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2), 66104. https://doi.org/10.2307/3333827 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Garmezy, N. (1985). Risk, vulnerability, and protective factors in developmental psychopathology. In Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 152). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-9820-2_1 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Lucke, C. M., Nelson, K. M., & Stallworthy, I. C. (2021). Resilience in development and psychopathology: Multisystem perspectives. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 521549. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-120307 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMahon, C. A., & Bernier, A. (2017). Twenty years of research on parental mind-mindedness: Empirical findings, theoretical and methodological challenges, and new directions. Developmental Review, 46, 5480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2017.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meins, E. (1997). Security of attachment and the social development of cognition. Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Meins, E. (1999). Sensitivity, security, and internal working models: Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment and Human Development, 1(3), 325342. https://doi.org/1080/14616739900134181 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E. (2013). Sensitive attunement to infants’ internal states: Operationalizing the construct of mind-mindedness. Attachment & Human Development, 15(5-6), 524544. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013.830388 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Avihou-Kanza, N. (2011). Individual differences in adult attachment are systematically related to dream narratives. Attachment and Human Development, 13(2), 105123. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2011.553918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8(4), 271289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narayan, A. J., Lieberman, A. F., & Masten, A. S. (2021). Intergenerational transmission and prevention of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs). Clinical Psychology Review, 85, 101997. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101997 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2023). Romania’s abandoned children: The effects of early profound psychosocial deprivation on the course of human development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 32(6), 515521. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214231201079 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3-4), 395422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parke, R. D. (1969). Effectiveness of punishment as an interaction of intensity, timing, agent nurturance, and cognitive structuring. Child Development, 40(1), 213235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollak, S. D., & Smith, K. E. (2021). Thinking clearly about biology and childhood adversity: Next steps for continued progress. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 14731477. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211031539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohner, R. P., & Lansford, J. E. (2017). Deep structure of the human affectional system: Introduction to interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 9(4), 426440. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.12219 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., & Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 99166). Wiley.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 265296. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579400003023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaramella, L. V., & Leve, L. D. (2004). Clarifying parent-child reciprocities during early childhood: The Early Childhood Coercion Model. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 7(2), 89107. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:CCFP.0000030287.13160.a3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2008). The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Social Development, 17(3), 737754. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00457.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., Gross, J. T., Stern, J. A., & Cassidy, J. A. (2016). A lifespan perspective on attachment and care for others: Empathy, altruism, and prosocial behavior. In Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed. pp. 878916). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sher-Censor, E. (2015). Five Minute Speech Sample in developmental research: A review. Developmental Review, 36, 127155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherman, L. J., Rice, K., & Cassidy, J. (2015). Infant capacities related to building internal working models of attachment figures: A theoretical and empirical review. Developmental Review, 37, 109141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.06.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 232. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 269281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slagt, M., Dubas, J. S., Deković, M., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2016). Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142(10), 10681110. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., & Wilson, M. N. (2015). Negative relational schemas predict the trajectory of coercive dynamics during early childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(4), 693703. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9936-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Winter, C. C., & Patterson, G. R. (2014). Coercive family process and early-onset conduct problems from age 2 to school entry. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4 pt1), 917932. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000169 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2021). Social relationships and children’s perceptions of adversity. Child Development Perspectives, 15(4), 228234. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12427 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snarr, J. D., Slep, A. M. S., & Grande, V. P. (2009). Validation of a new self-report measure of parental attributions. Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 390401. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016331 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1986). Appraisal: Bowlby’s contribution to psychoanalytic theory and developmental psychology; Attachment: Separation: Loss. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(6), 841849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00203.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (2005). Attachment and development: A prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attachment and Human Development, 7(4), 349367. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500365928 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (2013). The promise of developmental psychopathology: Past and present. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4, pt2), 12151224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000576 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (2016). The place of attachment in development. In Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed. pp. 9971011). Guilford.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E. A., Levy, A. K., & Egeland, B. (1999). Implications of attachment theory for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 11(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579499001923 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55(1), 1729. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129832 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, J. A., Barbarin, O., & Cassidy, J. (2022). Attachment perspectives on race, prejudice, and anti-racism: Introduction to the Special Issue. Attachment and Human Development, 24(3), 253259. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1976920 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., Cicchetti, D., Hentges, R. F., & Coe, J. L. (2016). Family instability and children’s effortful control in the context of poverty: Sometimes a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush. Development and Psychopathology, 29(3), 685696. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579416000407 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taraban, L., & Shaw, D. S. (2018). Parenting in context: Revisiting Belsky’s classic process of parenting model in early childhood. Developmental Review, 48, 5581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2015). Relationships, regulation, and early development. In Lamb, M. E. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Socioemotional processes , (vol. 3, 7th ed. pp. 201246). Wiley.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2016). Early attachment and later development: Reframing the questions. In Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (3rd ed. pp. 330348). Guilford Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2021). Internal working models as developing representations. In Thompson, R. A., Simpson, J. A., & Berlin, L. J. (Eds.), Attachment: The fundamental questions (pp. 128135). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A., Simpson, J. A., & Berlin, L. J. (Eds.) (2021). Attachment: The fundamental questions. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tiberio, S. S., Capaldi, D. M., Kerr, D. C. R., Bertrand, M., Pears, K. C., & Owen, L. (2016). Parenting and the development of effortful control from early childhood to early adolescence: A transactional developmental model. Development and Psychopathology, 28(3), 837853. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000341 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., & Kim, J. (2002). Relations among children’s perceptions of maternal behavior, attributional styles, and behavioral symptomatology in maltreated children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(5), 487501. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019868914685 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., Macfie, J., & Emde, R. N. (1997). Representations of self and other in the narratives of neglected, physically abused, and sexually abused preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 781796. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001430 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., Rogosch, F. A., Sturge-Apple, M., & Cicchetti, D. (2009). Maternal depression, children’s attachment security, and representational development: An organizational perspective. Child Development, 80(1), 192208. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01254.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyrell, F. A., & Masten, A. S. (2022). Father-child attachment in Black families: Risk and protective processes. Attachment & Human Development, 24(3), 274286. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1976923 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakschlag, L. S., Estabrook, R., Petitclerc, A., Henry, D., Burns, J. L., Perlman, S. B., Voss, J. L., Pine, D. S., Leibenluft, E., & Briggs-Gowan, M. L. (2015). Clinical implications of a dimensional approach: The normal: abnormal spectrum of early irritability. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(8), 626634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakschlag, L. S., Sherlock, P., Blackwell, C. K., Burns, J. L., Krogh-Jespersen, S., Gershon, R. C., Cella, D., Buss, K. A., & Luby, J. L. (2023). Modeling the normal: abnormal spectrum of early childhood internalizing behaviors: A clinical-developmental approach for the Multidimensional Assessment Profiles Internalizing Dimensions. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 32(S1). https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1987 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, E., Kondo-Ikemura, K., Posada, G., & Richters, J. E. (1990). Learning to love: Mechanisms and milestones. In Gunnar, M. R., & Sroufe, L. A. (Eds.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology. Self processes and development. (vol. 23, 217255). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Waters, T. E. A., Fraley, R. C., Groh, A. M., Steele, R. D., Vaughn, B. E., Bost, K. K., Verissimo, M., & Roisman, G. I. (2015). The latent structure of secure base script knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 51(6), 823830. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westen, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 333371. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.333 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalen, D. J., Sylvester, C. M., & Luby, J. L. (2017). Depression and anxiety in preschoolers. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 26(3), 503522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2017.02.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed