Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T10:42:02.742Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The weeks before 100 persecutory delusions: the presence of many potential contributory causal factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 September 2019

Daniel Freeman*
Affiliation:
Professor of Clinical Psychology, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Anthony Morrison
Affiliation:
Professor of Clinical Psychology, Greater Manchester Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust; and Division of Psychology and Mental Health, University of Manchester, UK
Jessica C. Bird
Affiliation:
Research Clinical Psychologist, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Eleanor Chadwick
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Emily Bold
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Kathryn M. Taylor
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Rowan Diamond
Affiliation:
Research Clinical Psychologist, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Nicola Collett
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Emma Černis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Louise Isham
Affiliation:
Research Clinical Psychologist, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Rachel Lister
Affiliation:
Research Clinical Psychologist, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Miriam Kirkham
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Ashley-Louise Teale
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Eve Twivy
Affiliation:
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Felicity Waite
Affiliation:
Research Clinical Psychologist, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, UK
*
Correspondence: Daniel Freeman, Oxford Cognitive Approaches to Psychosis, University Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. Email: daniel.freeman@psych.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The period before the formation of a persecutory delusion may provide causal insights. Patient accounts are invaluable in informing this understanding.

Aims

To inform the understanding of delusion formation, we asked patients about the occurrence of potential causal factors – identified from a cognitive model – before delusion onset.

Method

A total of 100 patients with persecutory delusions completed a checklist about their subjective experiences in the weeks before belief onset. The checklist included items concerning worry, images, low self-esteem, poor sleep, mood dysregulation, dissociation, manic-type symptoms, aberrant salience, hallucinations, substance use and stressors. Time to reach certainty in the delusion was also assessed.

Results

Most commonly it took patients several months to reach delusion certainty (n = 30), although other patients took a few weeks (n = 24), years (n = 21), knew instantly (n = 17) or took a few days (n = 6). The most frequent experiences occurring before delusion onset were: low self-confidence (n = 84); excessive worry (n = 80); not feeling like normal self (n = 77); difficulties concentrating (n = 77); going over problems again and again (n = 75); being very negative about the self (n = 75); images of bad things happening (n = 75); and sleep problems (n = 75). The average number of experiences occurring was high (mean 23.5, s.d. = 8.7). The experiences clustered into six main types, with patients reporting an average of 5.4 (s.d. = 1.0) different types.

Conclusions

Patients report numerous different experiences in the period before full persecutory delusion onset that could be contributory causal factors, consistent with a complex multifactorial view of delusion occurrence. This study, however, relied on retrospective self-report and could not determine causality.

Declaration of interest

None.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-ncnd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2019

Many key insights into mental health disorders have been gained from patient accounts. Aaron Beck's intuition that ‘there is more on the surface than meets the eye’ helped him recognise that patients' conscious negative thoughts were central to the cause of depression.Reference Beck1 Beck noted in his influential descriptive paper on depression that ‘The thinking-disorder typology outlined is similar to that described in studies of schizophrenia’. Theorists seeking to pinpoint a central core dysfunction in schizophrenia often cite patient accounts. Kapur, in outlining the hypothesis that delusions arise from dopamine-driven abnormal salience, writes: ‘patients report experiences such as, “‘I developed a greater awareness of…My senses were sharpened. I became fascinated by the little insignificant things around me’”; “Sights and sounds possessed a keenness that he had never experienced before”; “‘It was as if parts of my brain awoke, which had been dormant’”; or “‘My senses seemed alive…. Things seemed clearcut, I noticed things I had never noticed before’”’.Reference Kapur2 Patient subjective accounts can be an important part of the process of developing understanding of mental health difficulties. In this report we sought to gain potential causal insights by systematically assessing patient views about the period prior to the onset of their persecutory delusion.

The theoretical understanding of persecutory delusions

The questions asked of patients concerning the period before full delusion onset were informed by a multifactorial cognitive model that conceptualises persecutory delusions as unfounded threat beliefs.Reference Freeman3 The threat beliefs are hypothesised to explain subjectively anomalous (internal or external) experiences that occur at a time of stress. The types of anomalous internal experiences that can be misinterpreted are varied, including unexplained anxious arousal, dissociation, manic-type feelings and symptoms, aberrant salience and hallucinations. The anomalous experiences are appraised threateningly because of emotional processes (such as excessive worry, negative self-beliefs and images, and poor sleep) and reasoning biases (such as a failure to consider alternative explanations, jumping to conclusions). The causal mechanisms are clear in this theoretical account: worry brings implausible ideas to mind, keeps them there and elaborates the content; low self-esteem (negative self-beliefs) leads the person to feel inferior and vulnerable to harm from others; negative images (sometimes trauma-related) lead to overestimation of danger; subjectively anomalous internal states provoke fearful and unusual explanations; and disrupted sleep increases negative affect, mood dysregulation and the anomalous internal states. Cannabis, implicated in the occurrence of psychosis, has been shown to increase paranoia via producing negative affect and anomalous experiences.Reference Freeman, Dunn, Murray, Evans, Lister and Antley4 Most of the causal mechanisms highlighted are trans-diagnostic: they will exacerbate any type of mental health problem. The importance of shared aetiological causes is consistent with the idea of a general factor that increases liability for all major psychiatric disorder.Reference Selzam, Coleman, Caspi, Moffitt and Plomin5

Early signs studies

There is, of course, an empirical literature on prodromal symptoms and early warning signs for the occurrence of psychotic episodes in general, although not persecutory delusions in particular. It is an area of obvious methodological challenge (for example difficulties in defining onset and relapse, establishing temporal sequences, allowing for individual variability) and typically the studies have been observational and not guided by theory. For instance, the Early Signs Scale (ESS) was developed from open-ended questions to family members about the period before their relative's relapse.Reference Birchwood, Smith, MacMillan, Hogg, Prasad and Harvey6 The most commonly reported signs preceding relapse using this scale are sleep problems, anxiety, irritability and withdrawal. It is likely, however, that this method of item generation will have missed many experiences. In a prospective study using the ESS, JørgensenReference Jørgensen7 found that sleep problems, feeling unable to cope and anxiety occurred before the re-emergence of delusions in general in a group of 131 patients with schizophrenia.

Patients generally identify a number of factors contributing to the development of their experiences.Reference Carter, Read, Pyle and Morrison8 A few studies are exceptions in that they have been theoretically guided. For example, Bechdolf and colleaguesReference Bechdolf, Schultze-Lutter and Klosterkötter9 in a study of 27 patients with schizophrenia found that all recollected experiencing ‘basic symptoms’ (typically subtle changes in thought and perception) before relapse. Gumley et al Reference Gumley, MacBeth, Reilly, O'Grady, White and McLeod10 in a study of 83 patients with schizophrenia found that fear of relapse (for example ‘I have been worrying about my thoughts’ ‘I have been worrying about losing control’) predicted new episodes of psychosis. Møller & HusbyReference Møller and Husby11 argue that there are two core experiential dimensions of prodromes: ‘disturbance of perception of self’ (such as ‘Painful emotional indifference and distance to myself’) and ‘extreme preoccupation by and withdrawal to overvalues ideas’ (such as ‘Occupied by, and scrutinising, my inner world’). An alternative approach to the study of the period before psychotic episodes has been the focus on a subgroup of people who are at ‘ultra-high risk of psychosis’.Reference Yung, McGorry, McFarlane, Jackson, Patton and Rakkar12 In this work, the assessment of factors such as sleep disturbance and recent level of functioning have been found to predict subsequent transition to psychotic disorders.Reference Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter, Salokangas, Heinimaa, Linszen and Dingemans13 The occurrence of ‘delusional mood’ has also been highlighted by Jaspers and others.Reference Mishara and Fusar-Poli14

The current study

In this study we focused upon the period of onset of one specific psychotic experience – persecutory delusions – as factor analysis repeatedly finds the independence of psychotic experiences.Reference Peralta and Cuesta15,Reference Ronald, Sieradzka, Cardno, Haworth, McGuire and Freeman16 We used a specific theoretical model of persecutory delusions to guide the questions asked of patients.Reference Freeman3 Theoretical factors from the model for which self-report has validity were chosen. The questions were designed to ask about: worry, negative images, low self-esteem and depression, poor sleep, dissociation, mood dysregulation, manic-type symptoms, aberrant salience, hallucinations, substance use and stressors. We asked patients to recall the period before they were certain of their current persecutory beliefs. The objective was to systematically capture patient views on the occurrence of a wide range of putative causal factors in order to identify the most prevalent. We also sought to determine the main underlying dimensions present among these potentially overlapping factors.

Method

Participants

A total of 100 patients with persistent persecutory delusions in the context of non-affective psychosis took part in the study during the baseline assessment for the Feeling Safe Trial.Reference Freeman, Waite, Emsley, Kingdon, Davies and Fitzpatrick17 The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human patients were approved by an NHS Research Ethics Committee (South Central – Oxford B Research Ethics Committee; ref 15/SC/0508). Written informed consent was obtained from all patients. The inclusion criteria were: aged 16 years or above; persistent (at least 3 months) persecutory delusion (as defined by Freeman & GaretyReference Freeman and Garety18), currently held with at least 60% conviction; and a primary diagnosis of schizophrenia-spectrum psychosis (non-affective psychosis).

The exclusion criteria were: current receipt of another psychological therapy; insufficient comprehension of English; primary diagnosis of alcohol, drug or personality disorders; being treated in forensic services; diagnosis of organic syndrome; or a significant intellectual disability. Ten additional patients in the first cohort entering the trial said that they could not recall the period before delusion onset and therefore did not take part in this study; these individuals did not differ in age, n = 110, t(108) = −0.761, P = 0.448, or gender, n = 110, χ2(1) = 0.303, P = 0.582, from those who took part in the study.

Assessments

Basic demographic and clinical data were collected (for example age, gender, ethnicity, employment status, diagnosis from clinical records).

Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales – delusions (PSYRATS)

The PSYRATS – delusions scaleReference Haddock, McCarron, Tarrier and Faragher19 is a six-item multidimensional measure. It assesses the conviction, preoccupation, distress and disruption associated with a delusion. The delusion is rated over the last week. Higher scores indicate greater severity. The scale was also used to obtain a rating on a 0 (do not believe it) to 100 (absolutely convinced) percentage scale for how much the individual currently believed the persecutory belief.

Checklist

The 47-item checklist was developed for the study (the items can be seen in Table 1) to be used simply as a tool to capture patient descriptions. First, a list of categories was generated from the theoretical model. These were categories for which self-report would be possible: worry, negative images, low self-esteem and depression, poor sleep, dissociation, mood dysregulation, manic-type symptoms, aberrant salience, hallucinations, substance use and stressors. Second, item content for each category was produced drawing upon patient accounts, the published literature, the authors' clinical experience and existing scales measuring particular concepts (for example the Dunn Worry Questionnaire;Reference Freeman, Bird, Loe, Kingdon, Startup and Clark20 Aberrant Salience Inventory;Reference Cicero, Kerns and McCarthy21 Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale;Reference Bell, Halligan and Ellis22 Cambridge Depersonalisation ScaleReference Sierra and Berrios23). Two items also assessed the absence of any difficulties (such as ‘I was feeling perfectly fine’). At the top of the checklist, patients were informed that ‘This questionnaire asks about the weeks before you knew for sure that other people were trying to harm you’. Participants were first asked the length of time that it had taken to reach certainty in the delusion (years/several months/a few weeks/a few days/instantly). Then participants simply ticked the checklist items that had occurred (‘Please tick the box next to any statements below which describe experiences you had in the weeks before you were sure’).

Table 1 Endorsement of checklist items

Analysis

The main reporting was descriptive, providing the frequency of endorsement of each item and category. This was carried out with SPSS Version 22.0.24 To examine the presence of underlying dimensions connecting the checklist items, exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted in R25 using the package ‘psych’.Reference Revelle26 Factor analysis was appropriate as Bartlett's test of sphericity was significant (χ 2 = 29 763.2, d.f. = 990, P<0.001) and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) test of sampling adequacy was adequate (KMO = 0.67). The two items that assessed absence of any difficulties were not included in the factor analysis. As the item responses were binary (yes/no), EFA was conducted on the tetrachoric correlation matrix of the remaining 45 items with Oblimin rotation to allow for correlated factors. Parallel analysis and the scree plot were used to guide the number of factors to extract.Reference Goretzko, Pham and Bühner27,Reference Costello and Osborne28

Results

Participants

The average age of the participants was 42.1 years old (s.d. = 11.8). There were slightly more men (n = 59) than women (n = 41). The diagnoses from clinical records were schizophrenia (n = 65), schizoaffective disorder (n = 16), delusional disorder (n = 3), psychosis not otherwise specified (n = 16). The ethnicities were: White (n = 83), Black Caribbean (n = 7), Pakistani (n = 3), Black African (n = 2), Indian (n = 2), Black other (n = 1), Chinese (n = 1), and other (n = 1). Most participants were single (n = 69), with others married or in a civil partnership (n = 20), cohabiting (n = 1) or divorced (n = 10). The majority were unemployed (n = 76). Levels of the current persecutory delusions were high (mean PSYRATS total = 18.5, s.d. = 2.6), with the average degree of conviction with which the beliefs were now held being 87.7% (s.d. = 12.4). In total, 97 patients were currently prescribed antipsychotic medication.

Checklist endorsement

The endorsement of checklist items is shown in Table 1. The average number of checklist items endorsed (not including the two absence of changes items) was 23.46 (s.d. = 8.73) (minimum,  0, maximum,  40). The extent of endorsement did not vary by the five categories for the length of time of delusion onset (d.f. = 4.93, F = 0.78, P = 0.542). For example, those patients who reported instantly knowing that others were harming them still reported an average of 20.1 (s.d. = 10.9) checklist items, whereas those who took several months to reach certainty reported an average of 24.3 (s.d. = 8.4) checklist items. Only one patient did not endorse any of the checklist items (apart from the absence of changes items). The categories most endorsed were low self-esteem and depression (n = 95), worry (n = 94), aberrant salience (n = 92), manic symptoms (n = 87) and poor sleep (n = 85). All categories – apart from substance use and an absence of changes – were endorsed by a large majority of the patients.

Clustering of checklist items

The initial EFA identified that three items (bereavement, cannabis use and excessive alcohol drinking) did not cluster with the other items and were better explained as single items. The three items were removed and the EFA was repeated on the remaining 42 items. The EFA identified a six-factor structure that explained 58% of the variance. Items for each factor are shown in bold in Table 2 (items with cross-loadings over 0.3 are retained for descriptive purposes). The six factors represented ‘worry and negative self-thoughts’ (9 items), ‘aberrant salience and mania’ (9 items), ‘disorganised thoughts and emotions’ (8 items), ‘stressful events’ (6 items), ‘perceptual anomalies and sleep problems’ (5 items) and ‘dissociation’ (5 items). The correlations between the six factors were low (see Table 3), although there were cross-loadings above 0.3 for 14 out of 42 items, indicating overlap in the factors for a number of items. Patients endorsed items from an average of 5.41 (s.d. = 1.00, minimum, 0, maximum, 6) of the six different factors. A total of 62 patients endorsed items from all six factor types, 26 patients endorsed items from five factor types, 7 patients endorsed items from four factor types, 3 patients endorsed items from three factor types, 1 patient endorsed items from two factor types and 1 patient endorsed items from none of the factor types.

Table 2 Item loadings from the factor analysis

Table 3 Factor correlations

Discussion

Main findings

The study provides a unique snapshot from patients of the period before the onset of a current persecutory delusion. We consider that patient experiences, especially when systematically assessed, are an important part of the process of gaining understanding. For the majority of patients there was a lengthy period of figuring out what was occurring before they reached certainty in the delusional belief. And the period before delusion onset for all patients – apart from one – was clearly psychologically very charged. Most of the different types of potential causal factors identified from the theoretical model were occurring in most patients. Patients were dealing with low self-esteem, worry, poor sleep and stressors; a wide range of subjectively unusual experiences were also occurring, including dissociation, aberrant salience and hallucinations. The factor analysis indicated that there were six main clusters of experiences in the build up to the delusion, and each of these types occurred in the majority of the patients. Our view is that the snapshot captures a representation of the complex causal pattern involved in the occurrence of delusional beliefs.

The high rate of endorsement of the checklist items indicates that the theoretical model of persecutory delusions fits well with the experiences of patients. However, the potential causal effects on delusion formation of the factors reported by the patients cannot be determined in this study. Many of these factors may also have had reciprocal relationships with subthreshold psychotic experiences. There is also likely to be a degree of shared genetic propensity.Reference Shakoor, McGuire, Cardno, Freeman, Plomin and Ronald29 However it is notable that several of the factors identified as present before delusion formation are also established contributors to delusion maintenance. For example, there are data from randomised controlled trials that demonstrate that treating worry,Reference Freeman, Dunn, Startup, Pugh, Cordwell and Mander30 insomnia,Reference Freeman, Sheaves, Goodwin, Yu, Nickless and Harrison31 and low self-esteemReference Freeman, Pugh, Dunn, Evans, Sheaves and Waite32 all lead to reductions in paranoia.Reference Brown, Waite and Freeman33 Furthermore, a recent cross-sectional study with 1800 patients with psychosis show not only high rates of these difficulties but that patients would like them targeted in treatment.Reference Freeman, Taylor, Molodynski and Waite34 It is likely that many of the factors that maintain delusions are also involved in the causal picture at delusion formation.

Limitations

There are clear limitations to the study. The most obvious is that the study relied on retrospective self-report, with a variable length of time having passed since delusion onset. Recall may have been biased by the study questions, current presentation, difficulties in recollection and variability in the extent to which each experience is likely to be noticed at the time. A lengthy checklist was used but it was still only a limited number of experiences that were assessed. It would have been beneficial to have had patient input into the design of the checklist. The checklist was designed as a tool to capture patient experience and was not a validated questionnaire. If there was a prospective study – which is a challenging research method to carry out on this topic – then it would be sensible to use validated questionnaires for each factor rather than such a checklist.

Whether any of the experiences are specific to the development of a delusion or are simply implicated in most mental health conditions remains to be determined. The study group is reasonably representative of individuals currently seen in treatment services in the UK but it is unlikely to be representative of all presentations of persecutory delusions. Nonetheless we believe that the study provides a novel insight from patients into the complex, difficult and often confusing subjective experience of the period before the full onset of a persecutory delusion. It both informs our theoretical understanding but also highlights potential targets for intervention.

Funding

The study was funded by an NIHR Research Professorship awarded to D.F. (NIHR-RP-2014-05-003). It was also supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. This paper presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Acknowledgements

D.F. had full access to the data. The checklist data will be available for all reasonable requests from D.F.

References

1Beck, AT. Thinking and depression: idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1963; 9: 324–33.Google Scholar
2Kapur, S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 1323.Google Scholar
3Freeman, D. Persecutory delusions: a cognitive perspective on understanding and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry 2016; 3: 685–92.Google Scholar
4Freeman, D, Dunn, G, Murray, R, Evans, N, Lister, R, Antley, A, et al. How cannabis causes paranoia: using the intravenous administration of ∆9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) to identify key cognitive mechanisms leading to paranoia. Schizophr Bull 2015; 41: 391–9.Google Scholar
5Selzam, S, Coleman, J, Caspi, A, Moffitt, E, Plomin, R. A polygenic p factor for major psychiatric disorders. Transl Psychiatry 2018; 8: 205.Google Scholar
6Birchwood, M, Smith, J, MacMillan, F, Hogg, B, Prasad, R, Harvey, C, et al. Predicting relapse in schizophrenia: the development and implementation of an early signs monitoring system using patients and families as observers. Psychol Med 1989; 19: 649–56.Google Scholar
7Jørgensen, P. Schizophrenic delusions: the detection of warning signals. Schizophr Res 1998; 32: 1722.Google Scholar
8Carter, L, Read, J, Pyle, M, Morrison, A. ‘I believe I know better even than the psychiatrists what caused it’: exploring the development of causal beliefs in people experiencing psychosis. Community Ment Health J 2018; 54: 805–13.Google Scholar
9Bechdolf, A, Schultze-Lutter, F, Klosterkötter, J. Self-experienced vulnerability, prodromal symptoms and coping strategies preceding schizophrenic and depressive relapses. Eur Psychiatry 2002; 17: 384–93.Google Scholar
10Gumley, AI, MacBeth, A, Reilly, JD, O'Grady, M, White, RG, McLeod, H, et al. Fear of recurrence: results of a randomized trial of relapse detection in schizophrenia. Br J Clin Psychol 2015; 54: 4962.Google Scholar
11Møller, P, Husby, R. The initial prodrome in schizophrenia: searching for naturalistic core dimensions of experience and behavior. Schizophr Bull 2000; 26: 217–32.Google Scholar
12Yung, AR, McGorry, PD, McFarlane, CA, Jackson, HJ, Patton, GC, Rakkar, A. Monitoring and care of young people at incipient risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull 1996; 22: 283303.Google Scholar
13Ruhrmann, S, Schultze-Lutter, F, Salokangas, R, Heinimaa, M, Linszen, D, Dingemans, P, et al. Prediction of psychosis in adolescents and young adults at high risk: results from the prospective European prediction of psychosis study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 241–51.Google Scholar
14Mishara, AL, Fusar-Poli, P. The phenomenology and neurobiology of delusion formation during psychosis onset: Jaspers, Truman symptoms, and aberrant salience. Schizophr Bull 2013; 39: 278–86.Google Scholar
15Peralta, V, Cuesta, MJ. Dimensional structure of psychotic symptoms: an item-level analysis of SAPS and SANS symptoms in psychotic disorders. Schizophr Res 1999; 38: 1326.Google Scholar
16Ronald, A, Sieradzka, D, Cardno, A, Haworth, C, McGuire, P, Freeman, D. Characterization of psychotic experiences in adolescence using the Specific Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire (SPEQ): findings from a study of 5000 16 year old twins. Schizophr Bull 2014; 40: 868–77.Google Scholar
17Freeman, D, Waite, F, Emsley, R, Kingdon, D, Davies, L, Fitzpatrick, R, et al. The efficacy of a new translational treatment for persecutory delusions: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial (the Feeling Safe Study). Trials 2016; 17: 134.Google Scholar
18Freeman, D, Garety, PA. Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: does the definition need clarification? Br J Clin Psychol 2000; 39: 407–14.Google Scholar
19Haddock, G, McCarron, J, Tarrier, N, Faragher, FB. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS). Psychol Med 1999; 29: 879–89.Google Scholar
20Freeman, D, Bird, JC, Loe, BS, Kingdon, D, Startup, H, Clark, D, et al. The Dunn Worry Questionnaire and the Paranoia Worries Questionnaire: new assessments of worry. Psychol Med 2019; April 5 (Epub ahead of print).Google Scholar
21Cicero, DC, Kerns, JG, McCarthy, DM. The Aberrant Salience Inventory: a new measure of psychosis proneness. Psychol Assess 2010; 22: 688701.Google Scholar
22Bell, V, Halligan, PW, Ellis, HD. The Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS). Schizophr Bull 2006; 32: 366–77.Google Scholar
23Sierra, M, Berrios, GE. The Cambridge Depersonalization Scale: a new instrument for the measurement of depersonalization. Psychiatry Res 2000; 93: 153–64.Google Scholar
24IBM. SPSS Statistics Version 22. Release 22.0.0. IBM Corporation, 2013.Google Scholar
25R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2013.Google Scholar
26Revelle, W. Psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. Northwestern University, 2018 (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych).Google Scholar
27Goretzko, D, Pham, TTH, Bühner, M. Exploratory factor analysis: Current use, methodological developments and recommendations for good practice. Curr Psychol 2019: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00300-2.Google Scholar
28Costello, AB, Osborne, JW. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract Assess Res Eval 2005; 10: 19.Google Scholar
29Shakoor, S, McGuire, P, Cardno, A, Freeman, D, Plomin, R, Ronald, A. A shared genetic propensity underlies experiences of bullying victimization in late childhood and self-rated paranoid thinking in adolescence. Schizophr Bull 2015; 41: 754–63.Google Scholar
30Freeman, D, Dunn, G, Startup, H, Pugh, K, Cordwell, J, Mander, H, et al. Effects of cognitive behaviour therapy for worry on persecutory delusions in patients with psychosis (WIT): a parallel, single-blind, randomised controlled trial with a mediation analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2015; 2: 305–13.Google Scholar
31Freeman, D, Sheaves, B, Goodwin, G, Yu, L-M, Nickless, A, Harrison, P, et al. The effects of improving sleep on mental health (OASIS): a randomised controlled trial with mediation analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2017; 4: 749–58.Google Scholar
32Freeman, D, Pugh, K, Dunn, G, Evans, N, Sheaves, B, Waite, F, et al. An early phase II randomized controlled trial testing the effect on persecutory delusions of using CBT to reduce negative cognitions about the self. Schizophr Res 2014; 160: 186–92.Google Scholar
33Brown, P, Waite, F, Freeman, D. ‘Twisting the lion's tail’: manipulationist tests of causation for psychological mechanisms in the occurrence of delusions and hallucinations. Clin Psychol Rev 2018; 68: 2537.Google Scholar
34Freeman, D, Taylor, K, Molodynski, A, Waite, F. Treatable clinical intervention targets for patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2019; 211: 4450.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Endorsement of checklist items

Figure 1

Table 2 Item loadings from the factor analysis

Figure 2

Table 3 Factor correlations

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.