Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T16:03:15.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Contribution of Bilingualism to Cognitive Functioning and Regional Brain Volume in Normal and Abnormal Aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 November 2021

Valeria L. Torres
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, Florida, United States
Mónica Rosselli*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, Florida, United States Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States
David A. Loewenstein
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience and Aging, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, Florida, United States
Merike Lang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, Florida, United States
Idaly Vélez-Uribe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, Florida, United States
Fernanda Arruda
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, Florida, United States
Joshua Conniff
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, Florida, United States
Rosie E. Curiel
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and Center for Cognitive Neuroscience and Aging, Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, Miami, Florida, United States
Maria T. Greig
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Wien Center for Alzheimer's Disease and Memory Disorders, Mount Sinai Medical Center, Miami Beach, Florida, United States
Warren W. Barker
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Wien Center for Alzheimer's Disease and Memory Disorders, Mount Sinai Medical Center, Miami Beach, Florida, United States
Miriam J. Rodriguez
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Albizu University, Miami, Florida, United States
Malek Adjouadi
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Center for Advanced Technology and Education, College of Engineering, Florida International University, Miami, Florida, United States
David E. Vaillancourt
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States University of Florida Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, Gainesville Florida, United States
Russell Bauer
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States University of Florida College of Medicine, Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Gainesville, Florida, United States
Ranjan Duara
Affiliation:
Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Miami Beach and Gainesville, Florida, United States Wien Center for Alzheimer's Disease and Memory Disorders, Mount Sinai Medical Center, Miami Beach, Florida, United States
*
Author for correspondence: Mónica Rosselli, E-mail: mrossell@fau.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We examined the association between bilingualism, executive function (EF), and brain volume in older monolinguals and bilinguals who spoke English, Spanish, or both, and were cognitively normal (CN) or diagnosed with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) or dementia. Gray matter volume (GMV) was higher in language and EF brain regions among bilinguals, but no differences were found in memory regions. Neuropsychological performance did not vary across language groups over time; however, bilinguals exhibited reduced Stroop interference and lower scores on Digit Span Backwards and category fluency. Higher scores on Digit Span Backwards were associated with a younger age of English acquisition, and a greater degree of balanced bilingualism was associated with lower scores in category fluency. The initial age of cognitive decline did not differ between language groups. The influence of bilingualism appears to be reflected in increased GMV in language and EF regions, and to a lesser degree, in EF.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

One of the most significant controversies within the field of bilingualism research surrounds the idea that this dual-language capacity confers the speaker a cognitive advantage – namely, enhanced executive function (EF; Bialystok, Craik & Luk, Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2008; Bialystok, Poarch, Luo & Craik, Reference Bialystok, Poarch, Luo and Craik2014b; Costa, Hernández & Sebastián-Gallés, Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008). EF is an umbrella term conceptualized to include different components of central cognitive control such as flexibility and inhibition (Baddeley & Hitch, Reference Baddeley and Hitch1974; Jurado & Rosselli, Reference Jurado and Rosselli2007; Lezak, Reference Lezak1983). It is theorized that the advantage of bilinguals in these domains results from the need to continually inhibit one language to manage linguistic interference while speaking the other language (Bialystok & Craik, Reference Bialystok and Craik2010; Green, Reference Green1998). Another line of research portrays bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve (Stern, Reference Stern2009), thereby allowing bilinguals to maintain healthy cognitive function in aging, regardless of existing neuropathology, and delay the onset of cognitive impairment in aging (Perani, Farsad, Ballarini, Lubian, Malpetti, Fracchetti, Magnani, March & Abutalebi, Reference Perani, Farsad, Ballarini, Lubian, Malpetti, Fracchetti, Magnani, March and Abutalebi2017). However, not all research has found a bilingual advantage in the aging brain (Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri & Haan, Reference Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri and Haan2018; Zahodne, Schofield, Farrell, Stern & Manly, Reference Zahodne, Schofield, Farrell, Stern and Manly2014). This study aimed to explore this issue further.

Bilingualism and executive function

The theory that bilingualism enhances EF originates from the idea that the habitual use of two languages requires the extensive and continual use of cognitive control mechanisms. A bilingual individual must rely on these abilities for effective communication. Engaging in constant cognitive control practice might enhance inhibitory and switching mechanisms (Green, Reference Green1998; Rosselli & Ardila, Reference Rosselli, Ardila, Lantolf, Poehner and Swain2018). If these mechanisms are not language-specific, bilingualism should generate advantages in other specific cognitive domains related to inhibition and switching. These benefits have been described in tasks that require attentional control (Costa et al., Reference Costa, Hernández and Sebastián-Gallés2008), inhibition (Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Craik and Luk2008), and spatial tasks of working memory (Luo, Craik, Moreno & Bialystok, Reference Luo, Craik, Moreno and Bialystok2013).

Despite positive findings, there is evidence that fails to support enhanced EF in bilinguals (e.g., Hilchey & Klein, Reference Hilchey and Klein2011). A study by Paap and Greenberg (Reference Paap and Greenberg2013) tested the bilingual advantage in inhibitory control, monitoring, and switching in young adults. Their results showed little support for an executive processing advantage related to bilingualism and implied that in previous studies, the use of one task to evaluate EF might have led to a misinterpretation of findings. The authors also stressed the importance of adequately matching the study groups. In another study, Paap, Anders-Jefferson, Mikulinsky, Masuda, and Mason (Reference Paap, Anders-Jefferson, Mikulinsky, Masuda and Mason2019) did not find a bilingual advantage in four tasks of inhibitory control, and the authors highlighted the possibility that bilingualism is task-specific and part of a language-processing system. Furthermore, Antón, Carreiras, and Duñabeitia (Reference Antón, Carreiras and Duñabeitia2019) did not find a bilingual EF advantage in young adults using a large sample of monolingual and bilingual participants who underwent extensive EF testing.

A study by Sörman, Hansson, and Ljungberg (Reference Sörman, Hansson and Ljungberg2019) included two distinct adult bilingual samples to explore the effects of bilingualism and linguistic distance (Swedish–Finnish and Swedish–English bilinguals) on cognitive function. Results did not indicate a bilingual effect, and linguistic distance similarly failed to impact cognitive control. In a meta-analysis, Lehtonen, Soveri, Laine, Järvenpää, de Bruin, and Antfolk (Reference Lehtonen, Soveri, Laine, Järvenpää, de Bruin and Antfolk2018) considered a wide range of moderating variables (e.g., task paradigm, testing language, and group matching) and concluded that publication bias might be responsible for the associations between bilingualism and EF advantages.

Bilingualism has also been associated with disadvantages on verbal tests (e.g., fluency tests; Gollan, Montoya & Werner, Reference Gollan, Montoya and Werner2002; Lehtonen et al., Reference Lehtonen, Soveri, Laine, Järvenpää, de Bruin and Antfolk2018), presumably as a result of the increased linguistic interference between languages (Rosselli, Ardila, Araujo, Weekes, Caracciolo, Padilla & Ostrosky-Solís, Reference Rosselli, Ardila, Araujo, Weekes, Caracciolo, Padilla and Ostrosky-Solís2000) and reduced exposure to each language when both are used comparably (Lehtonen et al., Reference Lehtonen, Soveri, Laine, Järvenpää, de Bruin and Antfolk2018).

Considering the effects of bilingualism as “advantages” or “disadvantages” has been deemed inaccurate and reductionistic (Leivada, Westergaard, Duñabeitia & Rothman, Reference Leivada, Westergaard, Duñabeitia and Rothman2020). A shift to focus on the mechanisms and the variables underlying any bilingual adaptations/ameliorations (e.g., cognitive vs. brain reserve) has been proposed.

Bilingualism and reserve

Bilingualism may contribute to cognitive (Stern, Reference Stern2009) or brain reserve (Katzman, Reference Katzman1993). Cognitive reserve is an active and modifiable mechanism by which the brain attempts to cope in cases of changes or damage through the use of preexisting cognitive processes (Stern, Reference Stern2009; Cognitive abilities, education, occupation, exercise, or social engagement are experiences that may contribute to the development of cognitive reserve (Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri, Vuoksimaa & the Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup, Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri and Vuoksimaa2020). Brain reserve is a passive mechanism attempting to cope with damage and is derived from neuronal count or overall brain volume; the functional capacity to deal with brain injury (e.g., neurodegenerative diseases) varies across individuals (Stern, Reference Stern2009). An additional distinction between the types of reserve relates to the aim of the activity. For instance, active attempts to increase reserve, such as exercise differs from passive attempts (e.g., education) wherein the individual participates in the activity without the goal of increasing reserve. Accordingly, bilingualism may be considered passive; individuals may have learned a second language because of their circumstances. However, the use of two languages in daily life eventually necessitates active control (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2021). Greater demands on language control systems in bilinguals might increase reserve, leading to a delay in the onset of neurodegenerative clinical conditions such as dementia (Bialystok, Craik, Binns, Ossher & Freedman, Reference Bialystok, Craik, Binns, Ossher and Freedman2014a).

Cognitive reserve in bilinguals may result from an interaction between brain and behavioral adaptations resulting from the active and constant use of two languages (Grant, Dennis & Li, Reference Grant, Dennis and Li2014). The brain bases would correspond to quantifiable neuroanatomical modifications in gray matter volume (GMV), white matter volume (WMV), or cortical thickness. The behavioral bases would manifest as performance changes in tasks that depend on EF, attention, and switching. The extrapolation effect from language (specific) to other cognitive EF (domain-general) suggests an overlap of the cognitive control and language control networks (Abutalebi & Green, Reference Abutalebi and Green2016). The mechanisms underlying the overlap of functions is unknown; however, it may result from bilinguals’ increased switching practice. Changes in bilinguals’ complexity of EEG neural networks (greater sample entropy) with greater brain signal complexity are believed to index the ability to rapidly switch between brain states (Grundy, Anderson & Bialystok, Reference Grundy, Anderson and Bialystok2017). The cortical vs. subcortical and gray matter vs. white matter structural brain adaptations of bilingualism may be associated with the type of dual language experience, and individual factors within bilingual groups should be considered. For instance, initial exposure to a second language could cause gray matter cortical and subcortical changes, whereas increased experience in highly immersed bilinguals is related to additional structural changes in brain connectivity and subcortical regions (Pliatsikas, Reference Pliatsikas2020). Furthermore, researchers have also suggested that varying ages of second language acquisition may result in different adaptations when learning new tasks, and overall, the importance of considering individual differences and examining bilingualism across development is emphasized (Hernandez, Claussenius-Kalman, Ronderos & Vaughn, Reference Hernandez, Claussenius-Kalman, Ronderos and Vaughn2018; Hernandez, Claussenius-Kalman, Ronderos, Castilla-Earls, Sun, Weiss & Young, Reference Hernandez, Claussenius-Kalman, Ronderos, Castilla-Earls, Sun, Weiss and Young2019).

As a result of higher cognitive reserve, bilinguals may display symptoms of dementia at a later age than monolinguals (Fischer & Schweizer, Reference Fischer and Schweizer2014; Perani & Abutalebi, Reference Perani and Abutalebi2015; Perani et al., Reference Perani, Farsad, Ballarini, Lubian, Malpetti, Fracchetti, Magnani, March and Abutalebi2017). For example, Alladi, Bak, Duggirala, Surampudi, Shailaja, Shukla, and Kaul (Reference Alladi, Bak, Duggirala, Surampudi, Shailaja, Shukla and Kaul2013) reported that in their sample of bilinguals (some spoke more than two languages), there was a 4.5-year delay compared to monolinguals in the onset of AD, frontotemporal, and vascular dementia, after controlling for education. The bilingual group in this study reported speaking a wide range of languages, and as emphasized by Paap, Johnson, and Sawi (Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2016), it included a higher proportion of men and individuals from an urban setting with higher education. Similarly, Bialystok, Craik, and Freedman (Reference Bialystok, Craik and Freedman2007) reported a four-year delay in dementia symptoms for bilinguals compared to monolinguals. Lastly, Woumans, Santens, Sieben, Versijpt, Stevens, and Duyck (Reference Woumans, Santens, Sieben, Versijpt, Stevens and Duyck2015) reported that bilinguals exhibited a delay of 4.6 years in symptom manifestation and 4.8 years in AD diagnosis compared to monolinguals.

The mechanism underlying the delay in dementia diagnosis for bilinguals may result due to a compensatory strengthening of brain circuits involved in executive control (EC), which in turn strengthens frontostriatal and frontoparietal networks (Gold, Reference Gold2015). This explanation, therefore, lends support to the EF and cognitive reserve bilingual advantages, with a strong EF-bilingualism association acting as the underlying mechanism in the bilingualism-reserve link. The additional effort put forth by bilinguals to control their languages may be part of a general EC system, explaining the enhanced EF in bilinguals. Consequently, these enhanced abilities would compensate for the missing resources resulting from a neurodegenerative disease.

Consistent with the theory of cognitive reserve, aging bilinguals exhibit increased damage in several brain regions. Schweizer, Ware, Fischer, Craik, and Bialystok (Reference Schweizer, Ware, Fischer, Craik and Bialystok2012) found that bilinguals with AD had higher brain atrophy in the temporal horn (an area used to distinguish AD patients from healthy adults) compared to a group of matched monolinguals. Recently, Costumero, Marin-Marin, Calabria, Belloch, Escudero, Baquero, Hernandez, Ruiz de Miras, Costa, Parcet, and Ávila (Reference Costumero, Marin-Marin, Calabria, Belloch, Escudero, Baquero, Hernandez, Ruiz de Miras, Costa, Parcet and Ávila2020) compared matched samples of monolinguals (Spanish) and bilinguals (Spanish–Catalan) and found that MCI bilinguals exhibited higher brain atrophy despite similar performance on cognitive tests. These differences were found in the lingual and supramarginal gyri, which are typically affected in AD (Schwindt & Black, Reference Schwindt and Black2009).

Recently, Heim, Stumme, Bittner, Jockwitz, Amunts, and Caspers (Reference Heim, Stumme, Bittner, Jockwitz, Amunts and Caspers2019) examined GMV in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in a large sample of monolingual (n = 224) and bilingual (n = 175) participants. Results suggested that bilinguals had higher GMV in the left IPL and left IFG than monolinguals, but only at a younger age, with the normal decline associated with aging occurring faster in bilinguals but differing across regions, appearing later in the IPL than the IFG. The authors concluded that the increased ‘reserve’ in linguistic areas diminished with age at a faster pace than the ‘reserve’ in nonlinguistic areas. Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow, and Phillips (Reference Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow and Phillips2018) reported similar findings when comparing MCI and AD multilingual (of which over half were bilinguals, and the rest spoke three or more languages) and monolingual participants. Multilingual AD patients had reduced cortical thickness and lower tissue density in AD-related regions (implying higher cognitive reserve), as well as higher GMV in areas associated with language and cognitive control (e.g., bilateral IFG and right ventromedial prefrontal cortex, among others). Of note, this study obtained similar results with a non-immigrant MCI sample; however, these researchers did not include a healthy control group. Finally, Costumero et al. (Reference Costumero, Marin-Marin, Calabria, Belloch, Escudero, Baquero, Hernandez, Ruiz de Miras, Costa, Parcet and Ávila2020) also reported lower GMV in MCI bilinguals compared to monolinguals in the lingual and supramarginal gyri (brain regions known to be affected by AD).

Despite these findings, several studies fail to support an association between bilingualism and cognitive reserve. Crane, Gibbons, Arani, Nguyen, Rhoads, McCurry, Launer, Masaki, and White (Reference Crane, Gibbons, Arani, Nguyen, Rhoads, McCurry, Launer, Masaki and White2009) found that second language (L2) writing fluency did not protect against cognitive decline. Similarly, Yeung, St John, Menec, and Tyas (Reference Yeung, St John, Menec and Tyas2014) did not find a link between bilingualism and dementia risk in a 5-year longitudinal study. Zahodne et al. (Reference Zahodne, Schofield, Farrell, Stern and Manly2014) could not determine a protective effect of bilingualism on cognitive decline or in the conversion to dementia in bilingual immigrants. Nevertheless, bilinguals performed better at baseline on memory and EF tasks. Finally, Mungas et al. (Reference Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri and Haan2018) did not report a relationship between bilingualism and the rate of cognitive decline in a large longitudinal study with a sample of monolinguals and bilinguals.

Study aims

In general, research appears to suggest that the beneficial effects of bilingualism in aging are associated with its protective and enhancing influence over brain networks and regions related to EF and language (Luk, Bialystok, Craik & Grady, Reference Luk, Bialystok, Craik and Grady2011). Numerous studies do not support a delay in the onset of symptoms or dementia diagnosis, and instead, emphasize the inconsistent findings and methodological concerns (Mukadam, Sommerlad & Livingston, Reference Mukadam, Sommerlad and Livingston2017; Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, St John, Menec and Tyas2014).

The present study sought to include aspects of bilingualism research that have been emphasized by investigators, and to overcome previous limitations as follows: a) the inclusion of the age of acquisition of the L2 (Luk et al., Reference Luk, Bialystok, Craik and Grady2011); b) ensuring that all bilingual participants were proficient in the same languages; c) the inclusion of a monolingual control group; d) utilizing longitudinal data to explore changes associated with disease progression (Calvo, García, Manoiloff & Ibáñez, Reference Calvo, García, Manoiloff and Ibáñez2016); e) the use of more than one EF measure derived from separate tasks to minimize the possibility that performance differences are task-specific (Paap, Johnson & Sawi, Reference Paap, Johnson and Sawi2015); and f) the inclusion of neuroimaging assessments (van den Noort, Vermeire, Bosch, Staudte, Krajenbrink, Jaswetz, Struys, Yeo, Barisch, Perriard, Lee & Lim, Reference van den Noort, Vermeire, Bosch, Staudte, Krajenbrink, Jaswetz, Struys, Yeo, Barisch, Perriard, Lee and Lim2019), particularly of structural neuroimaging that could identify brain differences between bilinguals and monolinguals while removing the influence of task-related factors (García-Pentón, Fernandez Garcia, Costello, Duñabeitia & Carreiras, Reference García-Pentón, Fernandez Garcia, Costello, Duñabeitia and Carreiras2016).

This research analyzed differences between monolinguals and bilinguals classified as cognitively normal (CN) or diagnosed with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) or dementia in the GMV of memory-related regions and frontal regions associated with EF and language. Additionally, the effects of bilingualism on EF scores during two visits in cognitively normal (CN) and Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) participants were examined. Four EF tasks were used: Digit Span Backwards (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2014a); Trail Making Test difference score (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1986, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1993); Stroop Color-Word Interference (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935; Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe & Leber, Reference Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe and Leber1989), and category fluency average scores. To move beyond monolingual and bilingual comparisons, a Bilingualism Index (BI) and the age of acquisition of English (within the Spanish–English bilingual sample) were used to predict EF performance and GMV.

Consistent with previous findings (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow and Phillips2018), it was predicted that bilinguals would exhibit greater GMV in bilateral EF and language regions, but a higher degree of GMV loss in memory-related regions (for the MCI and dementia groups). Additionally, it was expected that bilinguals would outperform monolinguals on EF tasks except those with a strong verbal component (e.g., category fluency), as research suggests that the cross-language interference is associated with lower scores on verbal tasks (Rosselli et al., Reference Rosselli, Ardila, Araujo, Weekes, Caracciolo, Padilla and Ostrosky-Solís2000). Additionally, a BI value representing a balance in linguistic proficiency was expected to be the most significant predictor of EF performance, as previous findings support an association between these individual bilingual components and EF performance (Rosselli, Loewenstein, Curiel, Penate, Torres, Lang, Greig, Barker & Duara, Reference Rosselli, Loewenstein, Curiel, Penate, Torres, Lang, Greig, Barker and Duara2019; Yow & Li, Reference Yow and Li2015). Lastly, bilinguals were expected to be older at the onset of cognitive symptoms, as was suggested by previous reports (Alladi et al., Reference Alladi, Bak, Duggirala, Surampudi, Shailaja, Shukla and Kaul2013; Woumans et al., Reference Woumans, Santens, Sieben, Versijpt, Stevens and Duyck2015).

Method

Participants

Recruitment took place during memory screenings and events at community senior centers. Participants were part of the 1Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center (ADRC), a 5-year longitudinal IRB-approved study (Mount Sinai Medical Center-IRB) that began in 2015 at Mount Sinai Medical Center in Miami Beach, Florida. Two samples were included: a) CN, MCI, or dementia participants with Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) data; and b) CN or MCI participants with longitudinal neuropsychological data. Table 1 provides the demographic characteristics of the imaging and longitudinal samples. Participants were included if they were born in the US or a Spanish-speaking Latin American country.

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of the Imaging and Longitudinal Samples

Note. MCI = Mild Cognitive Impairment; M = Monolingual; B = Bilingual; EA = European American.

The first sample included 214 participants with MRI data (Mage = 71.21, SD = 7.23), of which 75 were CN, 106 were diagnosed with MCI, and 33 with dementia (see Diagnostic Criteria below). Within this sample, 124 participants were classified as bilinguals; 115 of these reported Spanish as their first language (L1) and English as L2 (Spanish–English bilinguals), 8 were English–Spanish bilinguals, and one was a simultaneous bilingual. Out of the 90 monolingual participants, 72 were English monolinguals, and 18 were Spanish monolinguals (see below for a description of the language groups). The diagnostic groups did not differ in the monolingual and bilingual distribution, χ 2 (2, N = 214) = .59, p = .75.

The 72 English monolinguals and 13 bilinguals were born in the US (39.72% of the sample). The rest of the sample comprised 111 bilinguals who were immigrants from the following countries (including Puerto Rico): Cuba (37.85%), Colombia (9.81%), Argentina (3.74%), Nicaragua (1.87%), Venezuela (1.40%), Puerto Rico (1.40%), Peru (1.40%), Chile (0.93%), Ecuador (0.93%), Dominican Republic (0.47%), and Guatemala (0.47%). The average age of immigration to the US was 25.51 (SD = 16.47), and these individuals had lived in the US for an average of 45.55 years (SD = 15.83). For the Spanish monolingual group, the average age of immigration to the US was 36.28 (SD = 11.25), with an average duration of residence of 35.44 years (SD = 11.97). The bilingual group reported an average age of immigration of 23.76 (SD = 16.55) years, and this group had lived in the US for an average of 47.19 years (SD = 15.82).

Participants’ occupations were compared across the language groups to examine potential socioeconomic differences. Occupations were classified according to Szreter's (Reference Szreter1984) scale as follows: Unskilled Manual Labor, Skilled Manual Labor, or Professional Labor. These data were available for 149 participants; 85.9% of participants reported occupations of Professional Labor, 10.1% were determined to have performed Skilled Manual Labor, and 4% were classified as having performed Unskilled Labor. Occupational categories did not differ between the monolingual and bilingual groups, p > .05.

For the longitudinal analyses, 171 participants (64.9% female) with Visit 1 (V1) and Visit 2 (V2) neuropsychological evaluations were initially included. This subsample comprised 66 CN, 87 MCI, and 18 dementia participants. Within the dementia group, there were six monolingual and 12 bilingual participants. Due to the limited number of available V2 data for the dementia sample and the uneven language group distribution, this diagnostic group was excluded from the longitudinal analyses. The final sample included 153 participants (64.7% female; 66 CN and 87 MCI) with a mean age of 70.97 (SD = 6.93) and 63 monolinguals and 90 bilinguals. Within the bilingual group, there were 83 Spanish–English bilinguals and 7 English–Spanish bilinguals. Within the monolingual group, 50 were monolingual English speakers, and 13 were Spanish monolinguals.

Participants were assessed with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, and for eligible participants, MRI scans were completed at Mount Sinai Medical Center during V1. Participants were seen by a clinician and were accompanied by an informant. With the informant's report, the clinician answered the following question: “Based on the clinician's judgment, at what age did the cognitive decline begin?”. All participants and their informants gave informed consent.

The time between V1 and V2 ranged from 10 to 33 months (M = 14.04, SD = 3.34). This variable did not differ between diagnostic or language groups, p > .05. Moreover, Spearman's correlations suggested that the visit interval was not associated with neuropsychological change scores from V2 and V1; therefore, it was not included in the analyses.

Exclusion criteria

Participants who met the following criteria were excluded: a) presence of major psychiatric disorders such as psychosis, bipolar, or unipolar disorders; b) missing Language Experience Acquisition Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q; Marian, Blumenfeld & Kaushanskaya, Reference Marian, Blumenfeld and Kaushanskaya2007) data; and c) first and second languages besides English or Spanish.

Diagnostic criteria

Groups were carefully classified by the following diagnostic criteria as outlined in our previous papers (Loewenstein, Curiel, DeKosky, Rosselli, Bauer, Grieg-Custo, Penate, Li, Lizagarra, Golde, Adjouadi & Duara, Reference Loewenstein, Curiel, DeKosky, Rosselli, Bauer, Grieg-Custo, Penate, Li, Lizagarra, Golde, Adjouadi and Duara2017; Rosselli et al., Reference Rosselli, Loewenstein, Curiel, Penate, Torres, Lang, Greig, Barker and Duara2019; Torres, Rosselli, Loewenstein, Curiel, Vélez Uribe, Lang, Arruda, Penate, Vaillancourt, Greig, Barker, Bauer & Duara, Reference Torres, Rosselli, Loewenstein, Curiel, Vélez Uribe, Lang, Arruda, Penate, Vaillancourt, Greig, Barker, Bauer and Duara2019) and which has been adopted as stringent and standard diagnostic criteria (Brooks & Loewenstein, Reference Brooks and Loewenstein2010).

Cognitively Normal (CN)

The CN group and their collateral informants did not report memory deficits, cognitive decline, or any impairment in daily function after an extensive interview, including the Global Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR; Morris, Reference Morris1993). All CN participants received a Global CDR score of 0. Also, the CN group received additional standard neuropsychological assessment and had standard neuropsychological measure scores less than 1 SD below expected levels related to age, education, and language-related norms on the following measures: a) Delayed Recall of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt, Reference Brandt1991; Cherner, Suarez, PLazzaretto, Fortuny, Mindt, Dawes, Marcotte, Grant & Heaton, Reference Cherner, Suarez, Lazzaretto, Fortuny, Mindt, Dawes, Marcotte, Grant and Heaton2007), b) category and letter fluency (Benton & Hamsher, Reference Benton and Hamsher1976), c) Block Design of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2014a, Reference Wechsler2014b), and d) Trail-Making Test B (TMT-B; Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1993).

Mild Cognitive Impairment

The MCI group met Petersen's criteria (Petersen, Reference Petersen2004), had memory complaints confirmed by a reliable collateral informant after an extensive interview with an experienced examiner including the CDR.

To meet the criteria for MCI, the participant had to obtain a Global CDR score of .5 and meet the criteria for a Minor Neurocognitive disorder by Diagnostics and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), including the lack of social and/or functional impairment. In addition, MCI patients had independent neuropsychological testing and had scores on the HVLT-R (Brandt, Reference Brandt1991) or NACC story delayed recall (Weintraub, Salmon, Mercaldo, Ferris, Graff-Radford, Chui, Cummings, DeCarli, Foster, Galasko, Peskind, Dietrich, Beekly, Kukull & Morris, Reference Weintraub, Salmon, Mercaldo, Ferris, Graff-Radford, Chui, Cummings, DeCarli, Foster, Galasko, Peskind, Dietrich, Beekly, Kukull and Morris2009) of 1.5 SD or greater, below what is expected using the same normative data listed for the CN group above. Other non-memory measures (e.g., TMT-B and category fluency) could be 1.5 SD or greater, above or below the mean.

Dementia

Individuals diagnosed with dementia had the same extensive clinical interview described above with a CDR Global score of 1.0, met criteria for a Major Neurocognitive disorder by Diagnostics and Statistical Manual and Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and demonstrated impairment in social or occupational function. In addition, scores were more than 2 SD below the mean relative to age, education, and language-related norms, as described above. The dementia group also met the criteria for Major Neurocognitive disorder, and clinically, this group also met the criteria for probable AD (McKhann, Knopman, Chertkow, Hyman, Jack, Kawas, Klunk, Koroshetz, Manly, Mayeux, Mohs, Morris, Rossor, Scheltens, Carrillo, Thies, Weintraub & Phelps, Reference McKhann, Knopman, Chertkow, Hyman, Jack, Kawas, Klunk, Koroshetz, Manly, Mayeux, Mohs, Morris, Rossor, Scheltens, Carrillo, Thies, Weintraub and Phelps2011).

Materials

Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q)

The LEAP-Q (Marian et al., Reference Marian, Blumenfeld and Kaushanskaya2007) was used to create monolingual and bilingual groups for language group comparisons. If a participant reported an average English or Spanish proficiency of 3 (“fair”) or more in speaking, understanding, and reading, they were considered bilingual; otherwise, they were classified as monolinguals. The order and age of language acquisition were determined from participant responses.

A Bilingualism Index (BI) and the age of acquisition of L2 of Spanish–English bilingual participants were used as variables related to bilingualism.

Bilingualism Index (BI)

The lower average LEAP-Q proficiency score (speaking, understanding, and reading in one language, English, or Spanish) was divided by the higher average LEAP-Q proficiency score (speaking, understanding, and reading in the other language). Participants rated their proficiency on a 0 to 10 Likert scale (0 = none, 1 = very low, 2 = low, 3 = fair, 4 = slightly less than adequate, 5 = adequate, 6 = slightly more than adequate, 7 = good, 8 = very good, 9 = excellent, 10 = perfect). The index resulted in scores ranging from zero (monolingual) to one (bilingual), providing information about the balance of an individual's bilingual abilities.

Rosselli et al. (Reference Rosselli, Loewenstein, Curiel, Penate, Torres, Lang, Greig, Barker and Duara2019) used the same measure in their study, and Gollan, Salmon, Montoya, and Galasko (Reference Gollan, Salmon, Montoya and Galasko2011) originally developed a similar BI using scores from the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass & Weintraub, Reference Kaplan, Goodglass and Weintraub1983), dividing the proportion of pictures named correctly in one language by the proportion of pictures named correctly in the other language.

Digit Span Backwards

Participants are read a sequence of one-digit numbers and asked to repeat the sequence in the reverse order. The Digit Span Backwards is considered a task of EF, specifically, working memory (WM; Hilbert, Nakagawa, Puci, Zech & Bühner, Reference Hilbert, Nakagawa, Puci, Zech and Bühner2015; Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter & Wager, Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000).

Trail Making Test (TMT)

The TMT consists of parts A and B. During Part A, participants are instructed to connect 25 circles numbered from 1 to 25 as quickly as possible. During Part B, participants are asked to connect circles with numbers and letters while alternating and maintaining numerical and alphabetic order (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1986). Participants are allowed 150 s for part A and 300 s for part B. Errors are corrected by the experimenter as soon as they occur. The time to complete TMT-B minus the time to complete TMT-A (TMT-B minus TMT-A) was used, as this difference score has been suggested to assess cognitive flexibility and switching without considering dexterity (Corrigan & Hinkeldey, Reference Corrigan and Hinkeldey1987; Kopp, Reference Kopp2011; Reitan, Reference Reitan1958).

Stroop Color-Word Interference Test

A measure of inhibitory control and interference, the Stroop test (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935; Trenerry et al., Reference Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe and Leber1989), requires participants to inhibit reading a word (a color) while correctly identifying the ink color of the text. Participants completed color (C), word (W), and color-word conditions (CW), with 45 s given for each. Predicted CW scores were calculated with the following formula: (WxC)/(W+C) and subtracted from the CW score. Stroop interference scores indicate the degree to which the participant can control interference.

Category Fluency

Participants are instructed to name as many animals, fruits, and vegetables as possible during 60 s per category. Incorrect words include proper names, numbers, repetitions, or words sharing similar roots. The total score of the three categories was averaged. This task involves language and EF, and lower scores are reported in AD than normal controls (Weakley & Schmitter-Edgecombe, Reference Weakley and Schmitter-Edgecombe2014).

Procedure

Monolingual European American participants were evaluated in English, and monolingual Hispanic participants were tested in Spanish. Bilingual participants selected their preferred language of evaluation (English or Spanish). In these cases, the neuropsychological examination was completed by fluent English–Spanish bilingual psychometricians. Out of 135 Hispanic participants, 71.9% were evaluated in Spanish. Please see Lang, Rosselli, Greig, Torres, Vélez-Uribe, Arruda, Barker, Garcia, Loewenstein, Curiel, and Duara (Reference Lang, Rosselli, Greig, Torres, Vélez-Uribe, Arruda, Barker, Garcia, Loewenstein, Curiel and Duara2021) for a complete description of the neuropsychological battery.

Spanish evaluations were completed with equivalent standardized neuropsychological tests and had appropriate age, education, and language normative data for the translated versions (Arango-Lasprilla, Rivera, Aguayo, Rodríguez, Garza, Saracho, Rodríguez-Agudelo, Aliaga, Weiler, Luna, Longoni, Ocampo-Barba, Galarza-Del-Angel, Panyavin, Guerra, Esenarro, García de la Cadena, Martínez & Perrin, Reference Arango-Lasprilla, Rivera, Aguayo, Rodríguez, Garza, Saracho, Rodríguez-Agudelo, Aliaga, Weiler, Luna, Longoni, Ocampo-Barba, Galarza-Del-Angel, Panyavin, Guerra, Esenarro, García de la Cadena, Martínez and Perrin2015a; Arango-Lasprilla, Rivera, Garza, Saracho, Rodríguez, Rodríguez-Agudelo, Aguayo, Schebela, Luna, Longoni, Martínez, Doyle, Ocampo-Barba, Galarza-Del-Angel, Aliaga, Bringas, Esenarro, García-Egan & Perrin, Reference Arango-Lasprilla, Rivera, Garza, Saracho, Rodríguez, Rodríguez-Agudelo, Aguayo, Schebela, Luna, Longoni, Martínez, Doyle, Ocampo-Barba, Galarza-Del-Angel, Aliaga, Bringas, Esenarro, García-Egan and Perrin2015b; Benson, de Felipe, Xiaodong & Sano, Reference Benson, de Felipe, Xiaodong and Sano2014; Golden, Reference Golden1999; Gollan, Weissberger, Runnqvist, Montoya & Cera, Reference Gollan, Weissberger, Runnqvist, Montoya and Cera2012; Ostrosky-Solís, López-Arango & Ardila, Reference Ostrosky-Solís, López-Arango and Ardila2000; Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Ubeda, Gramunt-Fombuena, Aguilar, Casas, Molinuevo, Robles, Rodríguez, Barquero, Antúnez, Martínez-Parra, Frank-García, Fernández, Molano, Alfonso, Sol, Blesa & NEURONORMA Study Team, Reference Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Ubeda, Gramunt-Fombuena, Aguilar, Casas, Molinuevo, Robles, Rodríguez, Barquero, Antúnez, Martínez-Parra, Frank-García, Fernández, Molano, Alfonso, Sol and Blesa2009a; Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Ubeda, Gramunt-Fombuena, Quintana, Aguilar, Molinuevo, Serradell, Robles, Barquero, Payno, Antúnez, Martínez-Parra, Frank-García, Fernández, Alfonso, Sol, Blesa & NEURONORMA Study Team, Reference Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Ubeda, Gramunt-Fombuena, Quintana, Aguilar, Molinuevo, Serradell, Robles, Barquero, Payno, Antúnez, Martínez-Parra, Frank-García, Fernández, Alfonso, Sol and Blesa2009b; Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Úbeda, Gramunt-Fombuena, Quintana-Aparicio, Aguilar, Badenes, Cerulla, Molinuevo, Ruiz, Robles, Barquero, Antúnez, Martínez-Parra, Frank-García, Fernández, Alfonso, Sol & Blesa, Reference Peña-Casanova, Quiñones-Úbeda, Gramunt-Fombuena, Quintana-Aparicio, Aguilar, Badenes, Cerulla, Molinuevo, Ruiz, Robles, Barquero, Antúnez, Martínez-Parra, Frank-García, Fernández, Alfonso, Sol and Blesa2009c; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2014b).

Imaging

MRI images were obtained using a Siemens Medical System Skyra 3 Tesla Scanner with Software version: “syngo MR E11”. Coil: “Siemens Head/Neck 20”. The Scanning Sequences used were 3D T1-W Magnetization-Prepared Rapid Gradient-Echo (MPRAGE.) Sagittal, three-dimensional images with resolution of 1 mm (~12 min., TR = 2150 ms, TE = 4.38 ms, TI = 1100 ms, 160 slices, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3) obtained from approximately 1 cm left of the skull to 1 cm right of the skull, allowing room for spatial reorientation along with defined anatomic coordinates. We employed visual inspection of the segmentation as outlined in the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative protocol. No segmentation issues were noted, and manual adjustment was not required. Brain parcellation was obtained utilizing a 3D T1-weighted sequence (Magnetization-Prepared Rapid Gradient-Echo -MPRAGE) with 1.0 mm isotropic resolution. FreeSurfer Version 5.3 software (Desikan, Segonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire, Hyman, Albert & Killiany, Reference Desikan, Segonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire, Hyman, Albert and Killiany2006) was used (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Curiel, DeKosky, Rosselli, Bauer, Grieg-Custo, Penate, Li, Lizagarra, Golde, Adjouadi and Duara2017).

No preprocessing is done on the MRI images, except for quality control carried out to check for shading which was corrected in some MRIs using an augmented statistical parametric method (SPM). The few MRIs that were affected by biased field were discarded from further analysis. Our datasets consisting of 3D MRIs through our web interface pipeline proved successful with regards to the Global Alzheimer's Association Interactive Network (GAAIN). To further validate our Neuro-Imaging Web Services Interface (NWSI) pipeline, the GAAIN centiloid initiative's data has been passed through the pipeline to ensure that the results are comparable to further improve the reach and usability of the centiloid data produced by NWIS. This entails comparing each portion of the process such as the DCM to NIfTI (NII) conversion, the MRI segmentation (GAAIN opted for SPM instead of FreeSurfer), the MRI registration.

Whole-brain grey matter combines multiple brain regions and is not useful for evaluating relationships with neuropsychological tests. In contrast, including all brain regions obtained from FreeSurfer as dependent measures would lead to an excess of comparisons. Given the specific neuropsychological measures employed and the evaluation of individuals at high risk of developing AD, we examined left and right brain regions from MRI scans that are associated with memory (hippocampi and entorhinal cortex; (Henneman, Sluimer, Barnes, van der Flier, Sluimer, Fox, Scheltens, Vrenken & Barkhof, Reference Henneman, Sluimer, Barnes, van der Flier, Sluimer, Fox, Scheltens, Vrenken and Barkhof2009; Leandrou, Mamais, Petroudi, Kyriacou, Reyes-Aldasoro & Pattichis, Reference Leandrou, Mamais, Petroudi, Kyriacou, Reyes-Aldasoro and Pattichis2018), EF (orbitofrontal cortex [OFC]; Bryden & Roesch, Reference Bryden and Roesch2015), and language (inferior frontal gyrus [IFG]; Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow and Phillips2018). For the OFC, the medial and lateral OFC were included. For the IFG, the pars opercularis, pars orbitalis, and pars triangularis were included. All GMV measurements were corrected for total individual intracranial volume.

Statistical analyses

IBM SPSS 25 was used. Two one-way Welch's ANOVAs were used to compare English and Spanish proficiency between the language groups, as the Levene statistic indicated unequal variances. Within the bilingual group, a Repeated Measures General Linear Model (GLM) was used to compare English and Spanish proficiency.

Six 2 (Language Group) x 3 (Diagnostic Group) univariate GLM analyses were used to compare the language and diagnostic groups on the GMV of bilateral regions associated with language (left and right IFG), EF (left and right OFC), and memory (left and right hippocampi and entorhinal cortices). Age, education, and sex were included as covariates. The Bonferroni correction was used to adjust for multiple comparisons; this resulted in alpha values of .025 for the two language regions and the two EF regions, and .013 for the four memory regions.

Four 2 x 2 x 2 mixed factorial GLM (2 between and 1 within factors) were used to compare the performance of the language (monolingual vs. bilingual) and diagnostic groups (CN and MCI) on the following neuropsychological tests over two visits: Digit Span Backwards (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2014a), TMT- B minus TMT-A (Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1993), Stroop interference (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935; Trenerry et al., Reference Trenerry, Crosson, DeBoe and Leber1989), and category fluency average scores. Age, education, and sex were included as covariates.

Spearman's rank correlations were used to explore the relationship between the EF scores during V1 and the GMV of left and right regions related to memory, language, and EF. Due to unequal language group sizes, Fisher r-to-z transformations were conducted to test whether the correlation coefficients differed between the groups (VassarStats; Lowry, Reference Lowryn.d.).

The predictive value of two variables related to bilingualism (Bilingualism Index [BI] and the English age of acquisition) was assessed using linear regressions in Spanish–English bilinguals. These analyses were conducted for EF tests and in GMV areas that exhibited language group differences in the previous GLM analyses. The bilingualism variables were used as predictors of the following: a) EF test performance in three tasks (Digit Span Backwards, Stroop interference, and category fluency; b) GMV in left IFG; and c) GMV in left and right OFC. These analyses included age, education, sex, language, and diagnostic group as predictors.

One 2X3 Univariate GLM compared the language groups using the age at which cognitive symptoms began. Age, education, and sex were included as covariates. Additionally, four linear regression analyses were used to examine the predictive value of GMV (IFG, OFC, hippocampi, and entorhinal cortices) and language group over the age of cognitive decline. A linear regression model was used to examine the relationship between GMV in areas that exhibited language group differences and Stroop interference scores (Del Maschio, Sulpizio, Gallo, Fedeli, Weekes & Abutalebi, Reference Del Maschio, Sulpizio, Gallo, Fedeli, Weekes and Abutalebi2018). Language group, age, education, sex, and normalized left IFG GMV were included as predictors of Stroop interference scores.

Fisher's Least Significant Difference (LSD) post hoc analyses were used.

Results

Language proficiency (LEAP-Q) of monolinguals and bilinguals

The average English and Spanish proficiency levels of the monolingual and bilingual groups did not differ. Within the bilingual group, Spanish proficiency was significantly higher (M = 9.16, SD = 1.21) than English proficiency (M = 8.32, SD = 1.66), F(1,123) = 19.25, p < .001, ηp2 = .135, with an average BI of .83 (SD = .16).

Language group

Imaging

For the imaging analyses, the diagnostic groups differed in sex, χ 2 (2, N = 214) = 9.22, p = .010, with a greater number of females in all groups. There were also group differences in education, F(2,211) = 3.87, p = .022, with the CN group reporting the highest years of education, followed by the MCI group, and lastly, the dementia group. No diagnostic group differences were found in age, ethnicity, or language group. The language groups differed in age, F(1,212) = 4.59, p = .033, with the monolingual group (M age = 72.49, SD = 7.91) older than the bilingual group (M age = 70.27, SD = 7.12). Education, sex, or diagnosis did not differ across groups.

The language groups were compared on the GMV of regions related to memory (hippocampi and entorhinal cortices), language (inferior frontal gyri; IFG), and EF (orbitofrontal cortices; OFC). See Table 2.

Table 2. Gray Matter Volume Across Diagnostic and Language Groups

Note. CN = Cognitively normal; MCI = Mild Cognitive Impairment; M = Monolingual; B = Bilingual; Left = Left Hemisphere; Right = Right Hemisphere; Hipp = Hippocampus; Ento = Entorhinal cortex; IFG = Inferior frontal gyrus; OFC = Orbitofrontal cortex.

Values presented as a percentage of intracranial volume.

Two 2X3 Univariate GLM with demographic variables as covariates were used to compare bilinguals and monolinguals on the GMV of left and right IFG across the three diagnostic groups. The only significant main effect was found between the language groups, F(1,205) = 11.88, p = .001, ηp2 = .055, with bilinguals exhibiting greater left IFG GMV than monolinguals. The main effect of diagnosis and the interaction between the language and the diagnostic groups were not significant, and age was a significant covariate, p < .001, whereas education and sex were not.

There was a main effect of language group in the GMV of the right IFG, F(1, 205) = 4.42, p = .037, ηp2 = .021, with bilinguals exhibiting larger volumes than monolinguals; however, this comparison lost significance after the Bonferroni correction. There was a significant main effect of diagnostic group, F(2,205) = 3.94, p = .021, ηp2 = .037. Post hoc analyses using Fisher's LSD indicated that the dementia group had lower IFG volume in the right hemisphere compared with the other two groups, ps < 0.05. No significant differences were observed between the MCI and CN groups. The interaction between diagnostic and language groups was not significant. Age and sex were significant covariates, p < .001.

For the OFC, two 2 × 3 Univariate GLM were used to compare the GMV of these regions between the language and diagnostic groups. A main effect of language group was found in the right and left OFC, F(1,205) = 11.52, p = .001, ηp2 = .053, and F(1, 205) = 8.56, p = .004, ηp2 = .04, respectively. Bilinguals exhibited higher GMV in these regions compared to monolinguals. In the right and left OFC, a main effect of diagnostic group was also found, F(2,205) = 9.92, p < .001, ηp2 = .09, and F(2,205) = 9.59, p < .001, ηp2 = .09, respectively. Post hoc analyses using Fisher's Least Significant Difference (LSD) indicated that the dementia group had the lowest GMV in the OFC of the right and left hemispheres compared to the other two groups, ps < .005. Significant interactions between the language and diagnostic groups were found in the right and left OFC, F(2,205) = 7.14, p = .001, ηp2 = .07, and F(2,205) = 5.73, p = .004, ηp2 = .05, respectively. Post hoc analyses indicated that the finding of higher GMV in the bilingual group compared with the monolingual group in the left and right OFC was significant only within the dementia group, ps < .05. Age was the only significant covariant of the right and left OFC, p = .001, and p < .001, respectively.

In addition to the language and EF areas, two 3 × 2 multivariate GLM were used to compare the language and diagnostic groups in the GMV of bilateral hippocampi and entorhinal cortices. No significant language group effects were found on either hemisphere for either region. In the left and in right hippocampi, there was a main effect of diagnostic group, F(2,205) = 22.73, p < .001, ηp2 = .18, and F(2, 205) = 18.71, p < .001, ηp2 = .15, respectively. Post hoc analyses indicated the dementia groups had the lowest GMV of bilateral hippocampi compared with the other groups, ps < .005, and the CN and MCI groups also differed significantly from each other, p < .001. Age and sex were significant covariates, ps < .001.

There was a main effect of diagnostic group in the GMV of the left and right entorhinal cortices, as well, F(2, 205) = 12.44, p < .001, ηp2 = .11, and F(2, 205) = 9.91, p < .001, ηp2 = .09, respectively. LSD post hoc analyses indicated that the left and right entorhinal cortex were significantly larger in the CN compared to the MCI and the dementia groups, ps < .001. The GMV of the left entorhinal cortex also differed significantly between the CN and MCI groups, p = .038. Age was a significant covariate, p < .001. No significant interactions between the language and diagnostic groups were found in these regions.

Associations between gray matter volume and executive function

For validity purposes, Spearman correlations were used to examine the relationship between the volume of the regions of interest (ROIs) and neuropsychological performance across the language groups. See Tables 36.

Table 3. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Hippocampi and Entorhinal Cortices in Monolinguals

Note. DSB = Digit Span Backwards; Stroop = Stroop Interference; Fluency = Category fluency; TMT = Trail Making Test B minus Trail Making Test A; Left = Left Hemisphere; Right = Right Hemisphere; Hipp. = Hippocampus; Ento. = Entorhinal cortex.

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Table 4. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Inferior Frontal Gyri and Orbitofrontal Cortices in Monolinguals

Note. DSB = Digit Span Backwards; Stroop = Stroop Interference; Fluency = Category fluency; TMT = Trail Making Test B minus Trail Making Test A; Left = Left Hemisphere; Right = Right Hemisphere; IFG = Inferior frontal gyrus; OFC = Orbitofrontal cortex.

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Table 5. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Hippocampi and Entorhinal Cortices in Bilinguals

Note. DSB = Digit Span Backwards; Stroop = Stroop Interference; Fluency = Category fluency; TMT = Trail Making Test B minus Trail Making Test A; Left = Left Hemisphere; Right = Right Hemisphere; Hipp. = Hippocampus; Ento. = Entorhinal cortex.

* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 6. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Inferior Frontal Gyri and Orbitofrontal Cortices in Bilinguals

Note. DSB = Digit Span Backwards; Stroop = Stroop Interference; Fluency = Category fluency; TMT = Trail Making Test B minus Trail Making Test A; L = Left Hemisphere; R = Right Hemisphere; IFG = Inferior frontal gyrus; OFC = Orbitofrontal cortex.

* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

For the monolingual group, significant positive correlations emerged between the Digit Span Backwards and the left IFG and between category fluency and bilateral hippocampi, entorhinal cortices, IFG, and OFC. In the bilingual group, the TMT-B minus TMT-A was negatively correlated with the GMV of the bilateral hippocampi, entorhinal, and OFC, and there were positive correlations between fluency and bilateral hippocampi, entorhinal, and left OFC. Stroop interference scores were not correlated with these regions.

Due to different sample sizes between the language groups, Fisher r-to-z transformations were performed. These analyses suggested that the TMT difference score correlation coefficients were not significantly different between monolinguals and bilinguals for the left hippocampus, right entorhinal cortex, or bilateral OFC. Differences were significant between the correlation coefficients from the right hippocampus, p = .03, and the left entorhinal cortex, p = .04.

Longitudinal analyses

For the longitudinal neuropsychological analyses, 153 participants (90 bilinguals) diagnosed as CN or MCI were included. The diagnostic groups were similar in age and education, but differences were found in sex, χ 2 (1, N = 153) = 4.62, p = .032. The language groups did not differ in age, education, or sex. See Table 7 for the neuropsychological scores across diagnostic and language groups.

Table 7. Neuropsychological Scores During Visit 1 (V1) and Visit 2 (V2) Across Diagnostic and Language Groups

Note. CN = Cognitively normal; MCI = Mild Cognitive Impairment; M = Monolingual; B = Bilingual; DSB = Digit Span Backwards; TMT = Trail Making Test B minus Trail Making Test A; Stroop = Stroop Interference; Fluency = Category fluency.

Four 2 x 2 x 2 mixed factorial GLM were used to compare the performance of the language and diagnostic groups over two time points on the Digit Span Backwards, Stroop interference, category fluency average, and TMT-B minus TMT-A scores.

Significant main effects for language and diagnostic groups emerged for Digit Span Backwards between groups, F(1,145) = 18.99, p <.001, ηp2 = .114, and F(1,145) = 4.54, p = .035, ηp2 = .030, respectively. In general, the monolingual group outperformed the bilingual group and controls surpassed MCI. No significant interactions were found between language group and time or between language group and diagnosis. See Figure 1.

Fig. 1. Digit Span Backwards Scores for V1 and V2 Across Diagnostic and Language Groups.

On Stroop interference scores, main effects of language and diagnostic groups were observed, F(1,139) = 6.66, p = .011, ηp2 = .046, and F(1,139) = 6.47, p = .012, ηp2 = .044, correspondingly. Overall, the bilingual and the MCI groups had reduced Stroop interference compared to the monolinguals and control groups. No interactions were significant. See Figure 2.

Fig. 2. Stroop Interference for V1 and V2 Across Diagnostic and Language Groups.

Main group effects were also found for category fluency average scores, F(1,127) = 4.36, p = .039, ηp2 = .033, with higher scores in the monolingual group and in the control group F(1,127) = 36.75, p < .001, ηp2 = .222. No interactions emerged. See Figure 3.

Fig. 3. Category Fluency Average on V1 and V2 Across Diagnostic and Language Groups.

No significant language group effects or interactions were found for TMT-B minus TMT-A scores.

Bilingualism characteristics

In addition to language group comparisons, the predictive value of the BI (assessing the degree of language balance) and the age of acquisition of English for CN or MCI participants who reported Spanish as their first language were examined.

Imaging

The initial analyses were performed with 115 Spanish–English bilinguals who had completed MRI scanning. This sample included CN, MCI, and dementia participants. Sex, diagnostic group, and variables were associated with bilingualism and therefore used as predictors, with the GMV of the left IFG and bilateral OFC used as dependent variables. The BI and English age of acquisition did not predict the GMV in these regions.

Executive function tests

These analyses were conducted with CN and MCI monolingual and bilingual participants (n = 111). The BI and English age of acquisition were used as predictors of V1 scores from Digit Span Backwards, Stroop interference, and category fluency average scores, as these measures exhibited language group differences.

There was a significant regression predicting category fluency average scores, F(5,105) = 14.68, p < .001, with the model predicting 38% of the variance as demonstrated by the adjusted R2. The BI was a significant predictor, β = -3.42, t = -2.30, p = .023, suggesting that this index, which reflects the balance in proficiency between languages, added significant predictive value to mean scores of category fluency over and above the other variables in the model.

A significant regression predicting Digit Span Backwards was also found, F(5, 103) = 3.64, p = .005, with the model predicting 11% of the variance. The age of acquisition of English was a significant predictor, β = -.06, t = -2.13, p = .035, adding significant predictive value to Digit Span Backwards scores over and above the other variables in the model.

Finally, the bilingualism variables did not predict Digit Span Backwards or Stroop interference.

Relationship between decline and GMV

As previous research has reported delays in the onset of cognitive decline for bilinguals, the estimated age at which participants began exhibiting cognitive symptoms was also examined. One hundred and fifty-seven participants with MCI or dementia (64 monolinguals and 93 bilinguals) were included in this analysis. There were no language group differences in the estimated age of the onset of cognitive decline. Linear regression analyses were conducted to examine the influence of the left and right GMV of OFC, IFG, hippocampi, and entorhinal cortices over the estimated age of onset of cognitive decline. Education and language group were included as predictors. The model including the left and right OFC as predictors was significant, F(4,133) = 2.74, p = .03, with the model explaining 4.8% of the variance demonstrated by the adjusted R2. However, none of the individual predictors reached statistical significance. The model that included the left and right IFG was also significant, F(4, 133) = 4.44, p = .002, and explained 9.1% of the total variance (adjusted R2), with no predictors reaching statistical significance. The model including the left and right hippocampi as predictors was significant, F(4, 133) = 3.89, p = .005, and explained 7.7% of the variance (adjusted R2), with no predictors reaching significance. Lastly, the model that included the left and right entorhinal cortices was not statistically significant.

Results of the multiple linear regression indicated that the Stroop interference scores were significantly predicted by the collective effect between language group, age, education, sex, and left IFG GMV, F(5, 202) = 2.28, p < .05, R2 = .053. However, the only individual predictor that was marginally significant was education (t = 1.93, p = .05).

Post-hoc analyses

Given that most evidence for cognitive reserve is reported in cognitively abnormal individuals (i.e., MCI and AD), the authors conducted post-hoc analyses in the MCI and dementia groups to examine whether there is a difference in the dissociation between GMV and task performance between monolingual and bilinguals. The monolingual and bilingual groups differed in age, F(1, 156) = 6.30, p = .013, therefore age was included as a covariate.

Univariate GLM comparing the groups in GMV of the selected regions revealed differences in the left and right IFG, F(1, 136) = 7.54, p = .007, and F(1, 136) = 3.94, p = .049. In both regions, bilinguals exhibited higher volume.

Additionally, univariate GLM were used to differences in task performance – namely, those in which language group differences were found in previous analyses (i.e., Stroop interference, Digit Span Backward, and Category Fluency). No significant differences were found.

Finally, to examine the association between GMV and task performance, three linear regression analyses were conducted using age, language group, diagnosis, and left and right IFG as predictors, and the Stroop interference, Digit Span Backward, and Category Fluency as dependent variables. The models predicting Stroop interference and Category fluency were not significant, while the model predicting Digit Span Backward was, F(5,132) = 3.96, p = .002, R2 = .13; diagnosis was the only significant predictor on the later model, t = -3.57, p < .001, suggesting that participants with a diagnosis of dementia have a higher chance than MCI participants to have low scores on Digit Span Backwards.

Discussion

This study compared monolingual and bilingual participants with normal and abnormal aging in GMV of regions associated with memory, language, and EF. Bilinguals exhibited higher GMV in areas associated with language and EF; however, no volumetric differences were found in areas related to memory. There was no evidence of a longitudinal (over an average period of 14 months) bilingual advantage on EF performance across CN and MCI monolingual and bilingual participants. However, there were general language group differences on overall EF scores, with monolinguals outperforming bilinguals on Digit Span Backwards and category fluency average, and bilinguals exhibiting reduced Stroop interference compared to monolinguals. A greater degree of balanced bilingualism was associated with reduced category fluency scores and acquiring English at a younger age was related to better performance on Digit Span Backwards. No differences in age of cognitive decline or a relationship between increased GMV and EF performance were found. Overall, the influence of bilingualism in this sample did not appear to contribute to cognitive reserve. However, moderate EF advantages in bilinguals were found. These findings are further discussed in the following sections.

Imaging

The bilingual group exhibited higher GMV in the left IFG and left and right OFC, but no differences were found in regions related to memory. It was expected that due to the increased demand for language control, bilinguals would exhibit higher GMV in frontal regions associated with language and executive control. Partially supporting the hypotheses, bilinguals had higher GMV in the left IFG independent of diagnosis. The left IFG, which corresponds to Broca's area, is strongly associated with language, and previous studies have reported higher volume and thickness in this region in bilingual individuals (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Stumme, Bittner, Jockwitz, Amunts and Caspers2019; Klein, Mok, Chen & Watkins, Reference Klein, Mok, Chen and Watkins2014), and activation differences in this region are reported in bilinguals depending on language proficiency (Golestani et al., Reference Golestani, Alario, Meriaux, Le Bihan, Dehaene and Pallier2006). Moreover, late bilinguals could activate distinct regions within Broca's area for native and second languages (Kim, Relkin, Lee & Hirsch, Reference Kim, Relkin, Lee and Hirsch1997). Recently, it has been suggested that a common neural network of activation subserving first and second languages exists and includes the inferior frontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the left basal ganglia, and the inferior parietal/supramarginal gyrus (Abutalebi & Green, Reference Abutalebi and Green2007; García-Pentón et al., Reference García-Pentón, Fernandez Garcia, Costello, Duñabeitia and Carreiras2016), while other views indicate that some structures are only involved with a weaker second language (Mouthon, Annoni & Khateb, Reference Mouthon, Annoni and Khateb2013). Therefore, the finding of increased GMV in the left IFG in bilinguals is congruent with previous research demonstrating an association between the active use of two languages and structural and functional brain plasticity. Previous findings suggest that age has a modulating effect on the bilingualism-brain relationship (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Stumme, Bittner, Jockwitz, Amunts and Caspers2019), with volumetric differences in the left IFG occurring earlier than in the IPL. While the present study did not examine the direct influence of age, this variable was significant in the analyses, suggesting that the neuroplastic effects of bilingualism vary across the lifespan. Furthermore, bilingualism predicted the GMV of the left IFG, with younger bilinguals more likely to exhibit higher GMV in this region.

In the left and right OFC, bilinguals exhibited higher GMV than monolinguals. However, this difference, influenced by clinical diagnosis, was only found in the dementia group.

The findings related to frontal language and EF regions partially support previous research suggesting that increased language experience and manipulation leads to neuroplastic changes (García-Pentón, Pérez Fernández, Iturria-Medina, Gillon-Dowens & Carreiras, Reference García-Pentón, Pérez Fernández, Iturria-Medina, Gillon-Dowens and Carreiras2014). Duncan et al. (Reference Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow and Phillips2018) also described that the bilingual experience might act as “exercise” for regions involved in control processes, ultimately leading to changes reflected in the increased GM density.

Besides frontal language and EF regions, it was also expected that consistent with the theory of cognitive reserve, the bilingual group, matched in demographic variables with a monolingual group and controlling for diagnosis, would have reduced GMV in memory regions; this was not found in the present study. The current results differ from the findings of Duncan et al. (Reference Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow and Phillips2018), which reported that bilinguals with AD had lower GMV in memory-related regions. It is important to note, however, that Duncan et al.'s sample was older than the present one, and the ROIs differed (these researchers selected the parahippocampal gyri and the rhinal sulci). Costumero et al. (Reference Costumero, Marin-Marin, Calabria, Belloch, Escudero, Baquero, Hernandez, Ruiz de Miras, Costa, Parcet and Ávila2020) also reported lower volume in MCI bilinguals compared to monolinguals in regions related to brain atrophy in dementia. Besides ROI differences, our bilingual sample consisted of mostly immigrants, while Costumero et al.'s sample included native-born bilinguals. Lastly, Schweizer et al. (Reference Schweizer, Ware, Fischer, Craik and Bialystok2012) analyzed computerized tomography scans for a sample with probable AD and described increased atrophy for bilinguals compared to monolinguals in areas related to AD. The discrepant results could be attributed to the evaluation of different age cohorts and the use of different neuroimaging techniques.

In general, the imaging data do support bilingualism as a factor for brain plasticity in specific areas of the aging brain. Although memory regions did not demonstrate language group differences, the larger volume in language regions (e.g., left IFG) could be attributed to the increased activation of these regions in bilinguals (Abutalebi & Green, Reference Abutalebi and Green2007; García-Pentón et al., Reference García-Pentón, Fernandez Garcia, Costello, Duñabeitia and Carreiras2016). The left IFG is commonly associated with syntactic and morphological language processing; however, it is also related to increased cognitive control demands, such as those needed to successfully complete Stroop-type tasks (Chen, Lei, Ding, Li & Chen, Reference Chen, Lei, Ding, Li and Chen2013). The use of shared prefrontal neural circuitry during visual (color identification under Stroop conditions) and language tasks of cognitive control processes (sentence comprehension under conditions of syntactic ambiguity) have been reported (January, Trueswell & Thompson-Schill, Reference January, Trueswell and Thompson-Schill2009). Our results seem to suggest that bilingualism could improve inhibitory control during Stroop tasks and also increase the size of regions involved with inhibitory control (Abutalebi & Green, Reference Abutalebi and Green2016). The behavioral (inhibitory control) and brain adaptations observed in the bilingual sample do not support the classical concept of cognitive reserve, but rather the concept of neural reserve appears to have influenced the findings. The structural brain adaptations observed in bilinguals may act as compensatory mechanisms for the increased demand for continuous efficient language control, providing the biological basis of the observed bilingualism-induced regional brain neuroplasticity (Pliatsikas, Reference Pliatsikas2020) and observed as better performance in inhibitory control tasks (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2017; Salvatierra & Rosselli, Reference Salvatierra and Rosselli2011).

Differences between EF and memory regions and their associations with neuropsychological performance between the language groups were found; greater TMT-B minus TMT-A differences scores were associated with reduced GMV of the right hippocampi and left entorhinal cortex in bilinguals. Few studies have examined TMT and GMV relationships. Nestor et al. (Reference Nestor, Nakamura, Niznikiewicz, Levitt, Newell, Shenton and McCarley2015) reported associations with faster TMT-B completion times and GMV of the left OFC and left middle orbital gyrus in healthy adults. In aging, TMT difference scores have been related to the volumes of the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and frontopolar cortex (Ruscheweyh et al., Reference Ruscheweyh, Deppe, Lohmann, Wersching, Korsukewitz, Duning, Bluhm, Stehling, Keller and Knecht2013). Differences between these results and the present ones might be related to different sample characteristics, with Ruscheweyh et al. excluding older adults with probable MCI, as well as different imaging methodology.

Longitudinal Analyses

EF performance during V1 and V2 in CN and MCI monolingual and bilingual participants was analyzed. Despite nonsignificant longitudinal differences in scores between the language groups, differences were found on Digit Span Backwards and category fluency average scores, with monolinguals outperforming bilinguals on these tasks. The category fluency findings are consistent with previous research, which typically report lower scores on verbal tasks in bilinguals resulting from increased linguistic interference (Rosselli et al., Reference Rosselli, Ardila, Araujo, Weekes, Caracciolo, Padilla and Ostrosky-Solís2000) and are in line with the hypothesis that monolinguals would have higher fluency scores than bilinguals. Performance on fluency tasks, particularly category fluency, is affected in AD (Weakley & Schmitter-Edgecombe, Reference Weakley and Schmitter-Edgecombe2014). Findings from the current study contribute to the literature describing the disadvantages exhibited on verbal tasks in bilingual CN and MCI individuals.

Findings from the Digit Span Backwards did not agree with the prediction that, as a result of the increased demands of bilingualism on executive control, bilinguals would outperform monolinguals. Yang (Reference Yang2017) reported better performance on Digit Span tasks in an intermediate bilingual group (determined by language proficiency and use), while this finding was not replicated in a high bilingual group. Yang suggested that the intermediate bilingual group developed stronger WM abilities because of the WM demands of bilingualism, while the high bilingual group no longer experienced the demands of language monitoring and memorization.

As predicted, Stroop interference scores followed a different trend from the other tasks; bilinguals exhibited reduced interference compared to monolinguals. There is previous research suggesting an interference advantage in bilinguals (Bialystok et al., Reference Bialystok, Poarch, Luo and Craik2014b), while other authors report only a weak advantage (Donnelly, Brooks & Homer, Reference Donnelly, Brooks and Homer2019) or no advantage (Lehtonen et al., Reference Lehtonen, Soveri, Laine, Järvenpää, de Bruin and Antfolk2018). In the current study, it appears that the inhibitory requirements of bilingualism may not be confined to language control, but instead may share resources with domain-general EF processes (Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2017; Green, Reference Green1998). Previous studies have suggested that bilingualism influences Stroop test scores (Rosselli, Ardila, Santisi, Arecco, Salvatierra, Conde & Lenis, Reference Rosselli, Ardila, Santisi, Arecco, Salvatierra, Conde and Lenis2002), and Suarez, Gollan, Heaton, Grant, Cherner, and HNRC Group (Reference Suarez, Gollan, Heaton, Grant and Cherner2014) reported that higher L2 fluency was associated with inhibitory advantages on the Stroop even when administered in the native language.

No language group differences in TMT difference scores were found. Research suggests a bilingual advantage on TMT (Suárez, Reference Suárez2013), while the opposite results are also reported (Kisser, Wendell, Spencer & Waldstein, Reference Kisser, Wendell, Spencer and Waldstein2012). In the Kisser et al. (Reference Kisser, Wendell, Spencer and Waldstein2012) study, the sample included undergraduate students (with ages between 18–44 years), while the Suárez (Reference Suárez2013) study included participants between the ages of 20–63. Furthermore, both studies used TMT-A and TMT-B separately as opposed to TMT difference scores, which is considered a better estimator of switching abilities (Kopp, Reference Kopp2011). Further research needs to examine the influence of aging in TMT completion times and related scores.

Because the analyses included only CN and MCI participants, changes in scores from V1 to V2 could be minimal and undetectable, and this could explain the present findings. As data collection is ongoing, it will be possible to explore whether considering data from subsequent visits can capture neuropsychological differences between the language groups. However, previous studies, such as Zahodne et al.'s (Reference Zahodne, Schofield, Farrell, Stern and Manly2014), did not find different rates of decline or dementia conversion between the language groups. Likewise, Mungas et al. (Reference Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri and Haan2018) did not find evidence of a bilingualism effect in cognitive decline.

Bilingualism variables

Besides the language group comparisons, additional aims of this study involved the identification of factors within Spanish–English bilinguals that were predictive of GMV and EF performance differences. To this end, the influence of a BI (reflecting the language proficiency balance) and the age of English acquisition was explored. These variables did not predict GMV, and the BI only predicted category fluency scores, with greater linguistic balance increasing the probability of lower scores. It appears that higher proficiency balance is accompanied by increased interference, and this resulted in reduced category fluency scores. Rosselli, Ardila, Lalwani, and Vélez-Uribe (Reference Rosselli, Ardila, Lalwani and Vélez-Uribe2016) reported that proficiency, more so than balance, was associated with EF advantages. The BI was expected to be a strong predictor of neuropsychological test performance and GMV. However, it appears that language proficiency balance was related only to the task with the strongest verbal component.

Likewise, the age of acquisition of L2 was not a significant predictor of GMV. However, a younger age of English acquisition was associated with better performance on the Digit Span Backwards task. Sörman et al. (Reference Sörman, Hansson and Ljungberg2019) included L2 proficiency in their language group comparisons and did not report its significant influence on executive control systems in 50–75-year-old adults. An earlier age of L2 acquisition is associated with reduced processing costs in bilinguals, and language balance is also suggested to play a significant role in EF advantages (Soveri, Rodriguez-Fornells & Laine, Reference Soveri, Rodriguez-Fornells and Laine2011; Yow & Li, Reference Yow and Li2015). Findings from this study support a mild influence of L2 age of acquisition and linguistic balance. It is likely that if there are EF advantages to be found in bilinguals, they do not appear to stem strongly from these characteristics of the bilingual experience.

Relationship between decline and GMV

The age at which cognitive decline began was not different between language groups. Additionally, the GMV of the selected regions was not a predictor of this variable, suggesting that, despite volumetric differences between the language groups, these do not influence the reported age of onset of cognitive decline in this sample. While this partly disagrees with several studies that report that bilinguals are older at the onset of dementia (Alladi et al., Reference Alladi, Bak, Duggirala, Surampudi, Shailaja, Shukla and Kaul2013; Woumans et al., Reference Woumans, Santens, Sieben, Versijpt, Stevens and Duyck2015), these findings are in line with results from other researchers (Mungas et al., Reference Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri and Haan2018; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Schofield, Farrell, Stern and Manly2014) who used prospective assessments to establish this age. The present findings, like those reported by Alladi et al. and Woumans et al., used retrospective analyses for the age of symptom onset, while Mungas et al. and Zahodne et al. followed participants over time. Furthermore, the current sample included Hispanic American individuals, and therefore had a greater degree of cultural and linguistic similarity with participants from the Mungas et al. and Zahodne et al. studies compared to Alladi et al.'s participants from India and Woumans et al.'s European sample. Moreover, we found that the higher GMV in the left IFG was not associated with reduced Stroop interference.

Because cognitive reserve is often reported in MCI and AD (e.g., Berkes, Calvo, Anderson & Bialystok, Reference Berkes, Calvo, Anderson and Bialystok2021; Ramakrishnan, Mekala, Mamidipudi, Yareeda, Mridula, Bak, Alladi & Kaul, Reference Ramakrishnan, Mekala, Mamidipudi, Yareeda, Mridula, Bak, Alladi and Kaul2017), the relationship between GMV and task performance was additionally examined in these cohorts removing the control (i.e., cognitively normal) group. Similar to the findings from GMV and age of decline, we did not find a relationship between the increased GMV in the left and right IFG and task performance, and other GMV regions were similar between the language groups. This suggests that, in this sample, the effect of greater GMV in the IFG is not reflected in cognitive advantages for bilinguals with MCI or dementia, at least with the cognitive tests used in this study. However, results from the current study suggest that there is brain reserve in the frontal lobe in bilinguals with abnormal aging that is not seen in monolinguals with an equivalent diagnosis.

The absence of bilingual cognitive reserve in the current study is in contradiction to previous research (Berkes, Calvo, Anderson & Bialystok, Reference Bialystok2021; Ramakrishnan et al., Reference Ramakrishnan, Mekala, Mamidipudi, Yareeda, Mridula, Bak, Alladi and Kaul2017). The discrepant results can be explained by methodological differences between studies. In the Berkes et al. study, bilingual participants were matched to monolingual participants on neural parameters derived from diffusion tensor imaging. Matched monolinguals had poorer clinical diagnoses than those predicted by chance from a theoretical null distribution, and poorer cognitive performances than matched bilinguals as measured by scores on the MMSE. In our study volumetric MRI was used as the biomarker predictor of several executive function tests instead of using a cognitive screening test (e.g., MMSE). Therefore, brain and cognitive parameters are not equivalent between the two studies. Future research should assess whether there is cognitive reserve in our sample using white matter integrity as a predictor. Ramakrishnan et al. (Reference Ramakrishnan, Mekala, Mamidipudi, Yareeda, Mridula, Bak, Alladi and Kaul2017) found that bilingual MCI patients had a clinical onset of cognitive complaints 7.4 years later than monolinguals. As opposed to our participants, 64% of bilinguals spoke more than 2 languages (multilingualism) compared to our study in which all bilinguals spoke only two languages (bilingualism). Delays in cognitive decline have been associated with the number of spoken languages (Chertkow et al., Reference Chertkow, Whitehead, Phillips, Wolfson, Atherton and Bergman2010; Kavé, Eyal, Shorek & Cohen-Mansfield, Reference Kavé, Eyal, Shorek and Cohen-Mansfield2008) and may explain the discrepancies.

Study strengths, limitations, and future directions

Limitations of the current study include an overrepresentation of females and MCI participants. V2 data for the dementia group was limited; therefore, the longitudinal neuropsychological performance was not assessed in this group. Future studies should include a greater number of participants diagnosed with dementia. Additionally, it remains to be seen whether similar results would be obtained when different types of bilinguals are included (i.e., weak vs. strong bilinguals; Ardila, Reference Ardila, Ardila and Ramos2007).

Category fluency tasks, which require greater lexical than executive demands, often demonstrate a monolingual advantage due to cross-linguistic interference in bilinguals (Paap et al., Reference Paap, Myuz, Anders, Bockelman, Mikulinsky and Sawi2017). Therefore, future studies should include other types of fluency tasks to overcome this limitation.

In the current study, the time between the two visits was highly variable. The 10–33-month interval likely influenced the longitudinal findings; however, the number of months between visits did not differ across our groups and was not associated with neuropsychological change scores. Zahodne et al.'s (Reference Zahodne, Schofield, Farrell, Stern and Manly2014) visit interval was 18–24 months, while Mungas et al.'s (Reference Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri and Haan2018) sample was evaluated every 12–15 months; therefore, in the present study, the broader range might partially explain the disparity of findings between these previous studies and the current one.

Our language groups were formed based on subjective self-assessments of language proficiency in speaking, understanding, and reading. Use of objective measures of linguistic abilities is likely more accurate in determining language proficiency. Several studies have used similar methods of classification (e.g., Mungas et al., Reference Mungas, Early, Glymour, Zeki Al Hazzouri and Haan2018; Woumans et al., Reference Woumans, Santens, Sieben, Versijpt, Stevens and Duyck2015); however, the use of interviews to ascertain language abilities in future research is essential. Additionally, while steps were taken to achieve a relatively homogenous bilingual sample (e.g., excluding participants who reported a second language besides English or Spanish), this group included individuals from 10 Spanish-speaking countries (excluding Puerto Rico), with the majority being immigrants. The influence of immigration continues to be a significant confound in bilingualism research (Fuller-Thomson & Kuh, Reference Fuller-Thomson and Kuh2014), and, when possible, future studies should replicate these findings with participants matched on immigration status. However, recent findings (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Nikelski, Pilon, Steffener, Chertkow and Phillips2018) regarding equivalent volumetric effects of bilingualism in immigrant and non-immigrant bilinguals with MCI suggest that immigration may not be as significant a factor as previously believed. Similarly, the monolingual group included individuals from different cultural backgrounds. Future research should include a homogenous sample of monolingual participants. While the language groups did not differ in occupation and years of education, employment data were not available for all participants. Furthermore, income information was not available for comparison. Therefore, it is possible that the measures employed did not capture other socioeconomic differences, and results should be interpreted with caution.

Another limitation was the retrospective nature of assessing the age at which cognitive decline began, as well as the limited number of participants with this information available. Alladi et al. (Reference Alladi, Bak, Duggirala, Surampudi, Shailaja, Shukla and Kaul2013) and Bialystok et al. (Reference Bialystok, Craik and Freedman2007) used a similar methodology to establish the age of symptom onset. However, as noted in Mukadam et al. (Reference Mukadam, Sommerlad and Livingston2017), these types of assessments are more likely to be influenced by other factors (e.g., education).

It could be argued that using a 1.5 SD cut-off for MCI might present a less stringent classification of the MCI diagnosis compared to the actuarial diagnostic method proposed by Jak and Bondi (Bondi, Edmonds, Jak, Clark, Delano-Wood, McDonald, Nation, Libon, Au, Galasko & Salmon, Reference Bondi, Edmonds, Jak, Clark, Delano-Wood, McDonald, Nation, Libon, Au, Galasko and Salmon2014; Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom, Salmon & Delis, Reference Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom, Salmon and Delis2009), which requires impairment of 1.0 SD or below the mean on two or more measures within a cognitive domain and have been shown to be more stringent than the conventional criteria for MCI. We utilized the most widely used approach of clinical diagnosis of MCI using a stringent clinical interview, an informant, and the use of a formal CDR Global score of .5. Given that we had two primary, albeit sensitive, memory measures and only a handful of additional measures representing other cognitive domains (with adequate, education, gender, and language norms), we augmented this approach with additional neuropsychological criteria. The actuarial diagnostic method proposed by Jak and Bondi (Bondi et al., Reference Bondi, Edmonds, Jak, Clark, Delano-Wood, McDonald, Nation, Libon, Au, Galasko and Salmon2014; Jak et al., Reference Jak, Bondi, Delano-Wood, Wierenga, Corey-Bloom, Salmon and Delis2009) requires impairment of 1.0 SD or below the mean on two or more measures within a cognitive domain. This is reasonable if there are multiple measures per cognitive domain (e.g., memory). However, a 1.5 SD cut-off is much more appropriate when each domain is represented by a modest number of measures, and it is employed in the vast majority of studies in AD research (see Brooks & Loewenstein, Reference Brooks and Loewenstein2010). FreeSurfer provides brain volumes for numerous regions in both the left and right hemispheres. Given the characteristics of our older sample, the study focused on specific brain regions (medial temporal and frontal) that have been previously associated with the neuropsychological measures employed. Nonetheless, additional brain regions and their connections are likely involved in bilingualism and may not be assessed using structural MRI. This limitation is worthy of further research. Moreover, arguments for a shift in reliance and brain plasticity towards subcortical regions with prolonged bilingual experience have been suggested (e.g., Grant et al., Reference Grant, Dennis and Li2014; Pliatsikas, Reference Pliatsikas2020); however, the current study did not include any of these representative brain regions (e.g., caudate nucleus, thalamus, and cerebellum). The current findings should be complemented by incorporating these regions in future research.

Despite these limitations, the strengths of this study included a large sample size (imaging sample n = 214 and longitudinal sample n = 153), the use of four tasks to assess the construct of EF, and the exclusion of individuals who were not US-born or Hispanics who did not have the shared experience of immigrating to the US. The bilingual sample included individuals with high Spanish and English proficiency; therefore, these results can be generalized to highly fluent Spanish–English bilinguals who are immigrants to the US and who have resided in the US for a large portion of their lives. Furthermore, the use of longitudinal analysis in bilingual research remains scarce. As the 1Florida ADRC study is ongoing, the cognitive and imaging trajectories of these samples will be addressed in future longitudinal studies.

Conclusion

In general, bilingualism appears to be associated with higher GMV in frontal regions related to language and EF, supporting the brain plasticity effect of the use of two languages. Results did not fully support the theory that bilingualism increases cognitive reserve; bilinguals were not older at the onset of cognitive decline, and there was no evidence of reduced GMV in regions related to memory. EF scores over time were not influenced by bilingualism; however, the bilingual group exhibited overall reduced interference on the Stroop test, while the monolingual group had higher scores on Digit Span Backwards and category fluency. A more balanced bilingualism predicted lower category fluency scores, and a younger age of English acquisition was associated with better performance on Digit Span Backwards. Overall, it appears that bilingualism does not serve to increase cognitive reserve, offer protection against age-related changes in temporal regions associated with memory, or moderate the initial age of cognitive decline. However, bilinguals exhibited smaller Stroop interference costs and exhibited higher GMV in regions associated with language and executive function – notably the left IFG (with a trending similar effect in the right IFG). This pattern of results suggests that lifelong bilingualism may contribute to neural reserve in aging within this cohort.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Florida Department of Health grant number 9AZ01 Florida Atlantic University, the National Institute of Aging Grants number 5 P50 AG047726602 1Florida Alzheimer's Disease Research Center and 5 P50 AG047726602 University of Miami and the National Science Foundation grant number CNS-1920182 Florida International University. None of the authors have conflict of interest involving this manuscript.

References

Abutalebi, J and Green, D (2007) Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control.. Journal of Neurolinguistics 20(3), 242275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J and Green, DW (2016) Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19, 689698. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alladi, S, Bak, TH, Duggirala, V, Surampudi, B, Shailaja, M, Shukla, AK and Kaul, S (2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology 81, 19381944. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Antón, E, Carreiras, M and Duñabeitia, JA (2019) The impact of bilingualism on executive functions and working memory in young adults. PLOS ONE 14, e0206770. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0206770CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arango-Lasprilla, JC, Rivera, D, Aguayo, A, Rodríguez, W, Garza, MT, Saracho, CP, Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y, Aliaga, A, Weiler, G, Luna, M, Longoni, M, Ocampo-Barba, N, Galarza-Del-Angel, J, Panyavin, I, Guerra, A, Esenarro, L, García de la Cadena, P, Martínez, C and Perrin, PB (2015a) Trail Making Test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37, 639661. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arango-Lasprilla, JC, Rivera, D, Garza, MT, Saracho, CP, Rodríguez, W, Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y, Aguayo, A, Schebela, S, Luna, M, Longoni, M, Martínez, C, Doyle, S, Ocampo-Barba, N, Galarza-Del-Angel, J, Aliaga, A, Bringas, M, Esenarro, L, García-Egan, P and Perrin, PB (2015b) Hopkins Verbal Learning Test- Revised: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 37, 699718. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ardila, A (2007) Bilingualism in the contemporary world. In Ardila, A and Ramos, E (eds), Speech and language disorders in bilinguals. New York: Nova Science Publishers, pp. 120.Google Scholar
Baddeley, AD and Hitch, G (1974) Working memory. In Bower GH (ed), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson, G, de Felipe, J, Xiaodong, L and Sano, M (2014) Performance of Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders in the United States on the Uniform Data Set. Alzheimer's & Dementia : The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association 10, S338S343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2013.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, AL and Hamsher, K (1976) Multilingual aphasia examination. (2nd ed). Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.Google Scholar
Berkes, M, Calvo, N, Anderson, JA and Bialystok, E (2021) Poorer clinical outcomes for older adult monolinguals when matched to bilinguals on brain health. Brain Structure and Function 226, 415424.10.1007/s00429-020-02185-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143, 233262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (2021) Bilingualism: Pathway to Cognitive Reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 25, 355364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, F and Freedman, M (2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia 45, 459464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, F and Luk, G (2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34, 859873. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E and Craik, FIM (2010) Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19, 1923. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409358571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM, Binns, MA, Ossher, L and Freedman, M (2014a) Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology 28, 290304. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Poarch, G, Luo, L and Craik, FIM (2014b) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging 29, 696705. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bondi, MW, Edmonds, EC, Jak, AJ, Clark, LR, Delano-Wood, L, McDonald, CR, Nation, DA, Libon, DJ, Au, R, Galasko, D and Salmon, DP (2014) Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and progression rates. Journal of Alzheimer's disease: JAD 42, 275289. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-140276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, J (1991) The Hopkins Verbal Learning test: Development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clinical Neuropsychologist 5, 125142. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049108403297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, LG and Loewenstein, DA (2010) Assessing the progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease: current trends and future directions. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 2, 28. https://doi.org/10.1186/alzrt52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryden, DW and Roesch, MR (2015) Executive Control Signals in Orbitofrontal Cortex during Response Inhibition. The Journal of Neuroscience 35, 39033914. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3587-14.2015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calvo, N, García, AM, Manoiloff, L and Ibáñez, A (2016) Bilingualism and cognitive Reserve: A critical overview and a plea for methodological knnovations. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Z, Lei, X, Ding, C, Li, H and Chen, A (2013) The neural mechanisms of semantic and response conflicts: An fMRI study of practice-related effects in the Stroop task. Neuroimage 66, 577584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherner, MM, Suarez, PP, Lazzaretto, DD, Fortuny, L, Mindt, M, Dawes, SS, Marcotte, T, Grant, I and Heaton, RR (2007) Demographically corrected norms for the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-revised and Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-revised in monolingual Spanish speakers from the U.S.–Mexico border region. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22, 343353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chertkow, H, Whitehead, V, Phillips, N, Wolfson, C, Atherton, J and Bergman, H (2010) Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders 24, 118125.10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181ca1221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, JD and Hinkeldey, NS (1987) Relationships between parts A and B of the Trail Making Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, 402409. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198707)43:4<402::aid-jclp2270430411>3.0.co;2-e3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A, Hernández, M and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 106, 5986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costumero, V, Marin-Marin, L, Calabria, M, Belloch, V, Escudero, J, Baquero, M, Hernandez, M, Ruiz de Miras, J, Costa, A, Parcet, MA and Ávila, C (2020) A cross-sectional and longitudinal study on the protective effect of bilingualism against dementia using brain atrophy and cognitive measures. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 12, 11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-020-0581-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crane, PK, Gibbons, LE, Arani, K, Nguyen, V, Rhoads, K, McCurry, SM, Launer, L, Masaki, K and White, L (2009) Midlife use of written Japanese and protection from late life dementia. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.) 20, 766774. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181b09332CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Maschio, N, Sulpizio, S, Gallo, F, Fedeli, D, Weekes, BS and Abutalebi, J (2018) Neuroplasticity across the lifespan and aging effects in bilinguals and monolinguals. Brain and Cognition 125, 118126.10.1016/j.bandc.2018.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, RS, Segonne, F, Fischl, B, Quinn, BT, Dickerson, BC, Blacker, D, Buckner, RL, Dale, AM, Maguire, RP, Hyman, BT, Albert, MS and Killiany, RJ (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968980.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnelly, S, Brooks, PJ and Homer, BD (2019) Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 11221147. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01567-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, HD, Nikelski, J, Pilon, R, Steffener, J, Chertkow, H and Phillips, NA (2018) Structural brain differences between monolingual and multilingual patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: Evidence for cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 109, 270282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, CE and Schweizer, TA (2014) How does speaking another language reduce the risk of dementia? Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 14, 469471. https://doi.org/10.1586/14737175.2014.892831CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller-Thomson, E and Kuh, D (2014) The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer's disease. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 52, 128130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.08.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Pentón, L, Fernandez Garcia, Y, Costello, B, Duñabeitia, JA and Carreiras, M (2016) The neuroanatomy of bilingualism: how to turn a hazy view into the full picture. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 31, 303327. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1068944CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Pentón, L, Pérez Fernández, A, Iturria-Medina, Y, Gillon-Dowens, M and Carreiras, M (2014) Anatomical connectivity changes in the bilingual brain. NeuroImage 84, 495504. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, BT (2015) Lifelong bilingualism and neural reserve against Alzheimer's disease: A review of findings and potential mechanisms. Behavioural Brain Research 0, 915. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golden, CJ (1999) Stroop: Test de colores y palabras. Madrid: TEAGoogle Scholar
Golestani, N, Alario, F-X, Meriaux, S, Le Bihan, D, Dehaene, S and Pallier, C (2006) Syntax production in bilinguals. Neuropsychologia 44(7), 10291040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, TH, Montoya, RI and Werner, GA (2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology 16, 562576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.562CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, TH, Salmon, DP, Montoya, RI and Galasko, DR (2011) Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia 49, 38263830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, TH, Weissberger, GH, Runnqvist, E, Montoya, RI and Cera, CM (2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England) 15, 594615. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000332Google Scholar
Grant, A, Dennis, NA and Li, P (2014) Cognitive control, cognitive reserve, and memory in the aging bilingual brain. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 1401. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW (1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 6781. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grundy, JG, Anderson, JAE and Bialystok, E (2017) Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1396, 183201. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, S, Stumme, J, Bittner, N, Jockwitz, C, Amunts, K and Caspers, S (2019) Bilingualism and “brain reserve”: A matter of age. Neurobiology of Aging 81, 157165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.05.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henneman, WJ, Sluimer, JD, Barnes, J, van der Flier, WM, Sluimer, IC, Fox, NC, Scheltens, P, Vrenken, H and Barkhof, F (2009) Hippocampal atrophy rates in Alzheimer disease: added value over whole brain volume measures. Neurology 72, 9991007. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000344568.09360.31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez, AE, Claussenius-Kalman, HL, Ronderos, J and Vaughn, KA (2018) Symbiosis, Parasitism and Bilingual Cognitive Control: A Neuroemergentist Perspective. Frontiers in Psychology 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez, AE, Claussenius-Kalman, HL, Ronderos, J, Castilla-Earls, AP, Sun, L, Weiss, SD and Young, DR (2019) Neuroemergentism: A Framework for Studying Cognition and the Brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics 49, 214223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.12.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilbert, S, Nakagawa, TT, Puci, P, Zech, A and Bühner, M (2015) The digit span backwards task: Verbal and visual cognitive strategies in working memory assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 31, 174180. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, MD and Klein, RM (2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18, 625658. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jak, AJ, Bondi, MW, Delano-Wood, L, Wierenga, C, Corey-Bloom, J, Salmon, DP and Delis, DC (2009) Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17, 368375. https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
January, D, Trueswell, JC and Thompson-Schill, SL (2009) Co-localization of Stroop and syntactic ambiguity resolution in Broca's area: Implications for the neural basis of sentence processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21, 24342444. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.21179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurado, MB and Rosselli, M (2007) The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review 17, 213233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9040-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E, Goodglass, H and Weintraub, S (1983) The Boston Naming Test. Lea & Febiger. Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
Katzman, R (1993) Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.43.1_Part_1.13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavé, G, Eyal, N, Shorek, A and Cohen-Mansfield, J (2008) Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and aging 23, 70.10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, KHS, Relkin, NR, Lee, K.-M. and Hirsch, J (1997) Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature 388, 171174. https://doi.org/10.1038/40623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kisser, JE, Wendell, CR, Spencer, RJ and Waldstein, SR (2012) Neuropsychological Performance of Native versus Non-native English Speakers. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27, 749755. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D, Mok, K, Chen, J.-K. and Watkins, KE (2014) Age of language learning shapes brain structure: A cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain and Language 131, 2024. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, B (2011) Neuropsychologists must keep their eyes on the reliability of difference measures. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS 17, 562563. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, M, Rosselli, M, Greig, MT, Torres, VL, Vélez-Uribe, I, Arruda, F, Barker, WW, Garcia, P, Loewenstein, DA, Curiel, RE and Duara, R (2021) Depression and the Diagnosis of MCI in a Culturally Diverse Sample in the United States. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: the Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists 36, 214230. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acz043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leandrou, S, Mamais, I, Petroudi, S, Kyriacou, PA, Reyes-Aldasoro, CC and Pattichis, CS (2018) Hippocampal and entorhinal cortex volume changes in Alzheimer's disease patients and mild cognitive impairment subjects. 2018 IEEE EMBS International Conference on Biomedical Health Informatics (BHI), 235238. https://doi.org/10.1109/BHI.2018.8333412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehtonen, M, Soveri, A, Laine, A, Järvenpää, J, de Bruin, A and Antfolk, J (2018) Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 144, 394425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leivada, E, Westergaard, M, Duñabeitia, JA and Rothman, J (2020) On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition:(How) should we proceed?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000358Google Scholar
Lezak, MD (1983) Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, DA, Curiel, RE, DeKosky, S, Rosselli, M, Bauer, R, Grieg-Custo, M, Penate, A, Li, C, Lizagarra, G, Golde, T, Adjouadi, M and Duara, R (2017) Recovery from Proactive Semantic Interference and MRI Volume: A Replication and Extension Study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease: JAD 59, 131139. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowry, R (n.d.) VassarStats: Website for Statistical Computation. Retrieved from http://vassarstats.net/Google Scholar
Luk, G, Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Grady, CL (2011) Lifelong Bilingualism Maintains White Matter Integrity in Older Adults. The Journal of Neuroscience 31, 1680816813. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, L, Craik, FIM, Moreno, S and Bialystok, E (2013) Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging 28, 2834. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030875CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V, Blumenfeld, HK and Kaushanskaya, M (2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR 50, 940967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, GM, Knopman, DS, Chertkow, H, Hyman, BT, Jack, CR Jr, Kawas, CH, Klunk, WE, Koroshetz, WJ, Manly, JJ, Mayeux, R, Mohs, RC, Morris, JC, Rossor, MN, Scheltens, P, Carrillo, MC, Thies, B, Weintraub, S and Phelps, CH (2011) The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia: the Journal of the Alzheimer's Association 7, 263269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH, Howerter, A and Wager, TD (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mouthon, M, Annoni, JM and Khateb, A (2013) The bilingual brain. Swiss Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 164, 266273. https://doi.org/10.4414/sanp.2013.00210Google Scholar
Mukadam, N, Sommerlad, A and Livingston, G (2017) The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease: JAD 58, 4554. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D, Early, DR, Glymour, MM, Zeki Al Hazzouri, A and Haan, MN (2018) Education, bilingualism, and cognitive trajectories: Sacramento Area Latino Aging Study (SALSA). Neuropsychology 32, 7788. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nestor, PG, Nakamura, M, Niznikiewicz, M, Levitt, JJ, Newell, DT, Shenton, ME and McCarley, RW (2015) Attentional Control and Intelligence: MRI Orbital Frontal Gray Matter and Neuropsychological Correlates. Behavioural Neurology 2015. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/354186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostrosky-Solís, F, López-Arango, G and Ardila, A (2000) Sensitivity and specificity of the Mini-Mental State Examination in a Spanish-speaking population. Applied Neuropsychology 7, 2531. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN0701_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, KR, Anders-Jefferson, R, Mikulinsky, R, Masuda, S and Mason, L (2019) On the encapsulation of bilingual language control. Journal of Memory and Language 105, 7692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, KR and Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66, 232258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Johnson, HA and Sawi, O (2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 69, 265278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Johnson, HA and Sawi, O (2016) Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 74, 305314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Myuz, HA, Anders, RT, Bockelman, MF, Mikulinsky, R and Sawi, OM (2017) No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 29, 89112. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1248436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña-Casanova, J, Quiñones-Ubeda, S, Gramunt-Fombuena, N, Aguilar, M, Casas, L, Molinuevo, JL, Robles, A, Rodríguez, D, Barquero, MS, Antúnez, C, Martínez-Parra, C, Frank-García, A, Fernández, M, Molano, A, Alfonso, V, Sol, JM, Blesa, R, & NEURONORMA Study Team (2009a) Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms for Boston naming test and token test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: the Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists 24, 343354. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña-Casanova, J, Quiñones-Ubeda, S, Gramunt-Fombuena, N, Quintana, M, Aguilar, M, Molinuevo, JL, Serradell, M, Robles, A, Barquero, MS, Payno, M, Antúnez, C, Martínez-Parra, C, Frank-García, A, Fernández, M, Alfonso, V, Sol, JM, Blesa, R, & NEURONORMA Study Team (2009b) Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms for the Stroop color-word interference test and the Tower of London-Drexel. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: the Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists 24, 413429. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña-Casanova, J, Quiñones-Úbeda, S, Gramunt-Fombuena, N, Quintana-Aparicio, M, Aguilar, M, Badenes, D, Cerulla, N, Molinuevo, JL, Ruiz, E, Robles, A, Barquero, MS, Antúnez, C, Martínez-Parra, C, Frank-García, A, Fernández, M, Alfonso, V, Sol, JM and Blesa, R (2009c) Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): norms for verbal fluency tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: the Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists 24, 395411. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perani, D and Abutalebi, J (2015) Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve. Current Opinion in Neurology 28, 618625. https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0000000000000267CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perani, D, Farsad, M, Ballarini, T, Lubian, F, Malpetti, M, Fracchetti, A, Magnani, G, March, A and Abutalebi, J (2017) The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer's dementia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, 16901695. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1610909114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, RC (2004) Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine 256, 183194. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pliatsikas, C (2020) Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 113 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000130Google Scholar
Ramakrishnan, S, Mekala, S, Mamidipudi, A, Yareeda, S, Mridula, R, Bak, TH, Alladi, S and Kaul, S (2017) Comparative Effects of Education and Bilingualism on the Onset of Mild Cognitive Impairment. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders 44, 222231. https://doi.org/10.1159/000479791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, RM (1958) Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills 8, 271276.10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, RM and Wolfson, D (1986) The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. In The neuropsychology handbook: Behavioral and clinical perspectives. New York, NY, US: Springer Publishing Co, pp. 134160.Google Scholar
Reitan, RM and Wolfson, D (1993) The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. 2nd ed. Tucson: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rosselli, M and Ardila, A (2018) Neuropsychology of Bilingualism (2018). In Lantolf, JP, Poehner, ME and Swain, M (eds), The Routledge handbook of sociocultural theory and second language development. Routledge.Google Scholar
Rosselli, M, Ardila, A, Araujo, K, Weekes, VA, Caracciolo, V, Padilla, M and Ostrosky-Solís, F (2000) Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology 7, 1724. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN0701_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosselli, M, Ardila, A, Lalwani, LN and Vélez-Uribe, I (2016) The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals*. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19, 489503. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosselli, M, Ardila, A, Santisi, MN, Arecco, MDR, Salvatierra, J, Conde, A and Lenis, B (2002) Stroop effect in Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS 8, 819827. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617702860106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosselli, M, Loewenstein, DA, Curiel, RE, Penate, A, Torres, VL, Lang, M, Greig, MT, Barker, WW and Duara, R (2019) Effects of Bilingualism on Verbal and Nonverbal Memory Measures in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS 25, 1528. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771800070XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruscheweyh, R, Deppe, M, Lohmann, H, Wersching, H, Korsukewitz, C, Duning, T, Bluhm, S, Stehling, C, Keller, SS and Knecht, S (2013) Executive performance is related to regional gray matter volume in healthy older individuals. Human brain mapping 34, 33333346. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22146CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatierra, JL and Rosselli, M (2011) The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism 15, 2637. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910371021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schweizer, TA, Ware, J, Fischer, CE, Craik, FIM and Bialystok, E (2012) Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: Evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 48, 991996. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwindt, GC and Black, SE (2009) Functional imaging studies of episodic memory in Alzheimer's disease: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage 45, 181190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sörman, DE, Hansson, P and Ljungberg, JK (2019) Different Features of Bilingualism in Relation to Executive Functioning. Frontiers in Psychology 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00269CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soveri, A, Rodriguez-Fornells, A and Laine, M (2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y (2009) Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 20152028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y, Arenaza-Urquijo, EM, Bartrés-Faz, D, Belleville, S, Cantilon, M, Chetelat, G, Ewers, M, Franzmeier, N, Kempermann, G, Kremen, WS, Okonkwo, O, Scarmeas, N, Soldan, A, Udeh-Momoh, C, Valenzuela, M, Vemuri, P, Vuoksimaa, E, & the Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup (2020) Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer's & Dementia: the Journal of the Alzheimer's Association 16, 13051311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stroop, JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suárez, PA (2013) The Role of Bilingualism on Neuropsychological Test Performance among Spanish Speakers Tested in Their Native Language (Doctoral dissertation). https://escholarship.org/uc/item/92f4233qGoogle Scholar
Suarez, PA, Gollan, TH, Heaton, R, Grant, I, Cherner, M, & HNRC Group (2014) Second-Language Fluency Predicts Native Language Stroop Effects: Evidence from Spanish–English Bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS 20, 342348. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714000058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szreter, SRS (1984) The genesis of the Registrar General's social classification of occupations. British Journal of Sociology 35, 523546. https://doi.org/10.2307/590433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres, VL, Rosselli, M, Loewenstein, DA, Curiel, RE, Vélez Uribe, I, Lang, M, Arruda, F, Penate, A, Vaillancourt, DE, Greig, MT, Barker, WW, Bauer, RM and Duara, R (2019) Types of errors on a semantic interference task in mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Neuropsychology 33, 670684. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000542CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trenerry, M, Crosson, B, DeBoe, J and Leber, W (1989) Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test manual. Adessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (PAR).Google Scholar
van den Noort, M, Vermeire, K, Bosch, P, Staudte, H, Krajenbrink, T, Jaswetz, L, Struys, E, Yeo, S, Barisch, P, Perriard, B, Lee, SH and Lim, S (2019) A Systematic Review on the Possible Relationship Between Bilingualism, Cognitive Decline, and the Onset of Dementia. Behavioral sciences (Basel, Switzerland) 9, 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9070081Google ScholarPubMed
Weakley, A and Schmitter-Edgecombe, M (2014) Analysis of Verbal Fluency Ability in Alzheimer's Disease: The Role of Clustering, Switching and Semantic Proximities. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 29, 256. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acu010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (2014a) Wechsler adult intelligence scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2014b) Escala Wechsler de Inteligencia para Adultos WAIS-IV. Manual Moderno Mexico.Google Scholar
Weintraub, S, Salmon, D, Mercaldo, N, Ferris, S, Graff-Radford, NR, Chui, H, Cummings, J, DeCarli, C, Foster, NL, Galasko, D, Peskind, E, Dietrich, W, Beekly, DL, Kukull, WA and Morris, JC (2009) The Alzheimer's Disease Centers' Uniform Data Set (UDS): the neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 23, 91101. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0b013e318191c7ddCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woumans, E, Santens, P, Sieben, A, Versijpt, J, Stevens, M and Duyck, W (2015) Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer's disease. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18, 568574. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891400087XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, E (2017) Bilinguals' Working Memory (WM) Advantage and Their Dual Language Practices. Brain Sciences 7. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7070086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, CM, St John, PD, Menec, V and Tyas, SL (2014) Is bilingualism associated with a lower risk of dementia in community-living older adults? Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 28, 326332. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0000000000000019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yow, WQ and Li, X (2015) Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: Why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, LB, Schofield, PW, Farrell, MT, Stern, Y and Manly, JJ (2014) Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology 28, 238246. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of the Imaging and Longitudinal Samples

Figure 1

Table 2. Gray Matter Volume Across Diagnostic and Language Groups

Figure 2

Table 3. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Hippocampi and Entorhinal Cortices in Monolinguals

Figure 3

Table 4. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Inferior Frontal Gyri and Orbitofrontal Cortices in Monolinguals

Figure 4

Table 5. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Hippocampi and Entorhinal Cortices in Bilinguals

Figure 5

Table 6. Correlations Between Neuropsychological Scores and Gray Matter Volume of Bilateral Inferior Frontal Gyri and Orbitofrontal Cortices in Bilinguals

Figure 6

Table 7. Neuropsychological Scores During Visit 1 (V1) and Visit 2 (V2) Across Diagnostic and Language Groups

Figure 7

Fig. 1. Digit Span Backwards Scores for V1 and V2 Across Diagnostic and Language Groups.

Figure 8

Fig. 2. Stroop Interference for V1 and V2 Across Diagnostic and Language Groups.

Figure 9

Fig. 3. Category Fluency Average on V1 and V2 Across Diagnostic and Language Groups.