Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T07:46:13.984Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Uncertainty Driven Action (UDA) model: A foundation for unifying perspectives on design activity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2017

Philip Cash*
Affiliation:
Department Of Management EngineeringTechnical University of Denmark, DK-2800 Lyngby, Denmark
Melanie Kreye
Affiliation:
Department Of Management EngineeringTechnical University of Denmark, DK-2800 Lyngby, Denmark
*
Email address for correspondence: pcas@dtu.dk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper proposes the Uncertainty Driven Action (UDA) model, which unifies the fragmented literature on design activity. The UDA model conceptualises design activity as a process consisting of three core actions: information action, knowledge-sharing action, and representation action, which are linked via uncertainty perception. The foundations of the UDA model in the design literature are elaborated in terms of the three core actions and their links to designer cognition and behaviour, utilising definitions and concepts from Activity Theory. The practical relevance and theoretical contributions of the UDA model are discussed. This paper contributes to the design literature by offering a comprehensive formalisation of design activity of individual designers, which connects cognition and action, to provide a foundation for understanding previously disparate descriptions of design activity.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
Distributed as Open Access under a CC-BY 4.0 license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2017

1 Introduction

Design activity refers to a complex phenomenon enacted by a designer (Visser Reference Visser2009; Cash, Hicks & Culley Reference Cash, Hicks and Culley2015) and connecting information, knowledge (Sim & Duffy Reference Sim and Duffy2003), and object domains (Gero & Kannengiesser Reference Gero and Kannengiesser2004). The term itself has a number of possible definitions and uses in the literature. At its most general, it has been used to describe the whole process and practice of design with respect to the organisation (Pahl & Beitz Reference Pahl and Beitz1996), as well as specific stages or elements in the product development process e.g. embodiment design (Pugh Reference Pugh1989). Here, activity is framed with respect to a specific product development goal and describes organisational behaviours (Wynn & Clarkson Reference Wynn and Clarkson2005). In this context, normative development processes are driven by organisational rationalisations and decision making e.g. as in Decision Based Design (Hazelrigg Reference Hazelrigg1998). In contrast, authors such as Andreasen, Thorp Hansen & Cash (Reference Andreasen, Thorp Hansen and Cash2015) use design activity to describe the interface between design practice and reflective improvement. Here, the interaction between an individual’s external behaviour and internal cognition produces an emergent activity process (Evans & Stanovich Reference Evans and Stanovich2013). Despite these differing uses, design activity is predominantly applied with respect to the designer. For example, Sim & Duffy (Reference Sim and Duffy2003) describe an individual’s design activity as a knowledge-based input/output system moderated by the design goal. Hence a general definition of design activity is adopted: design activity is a goal directed system where cognition, behaviour, and motivation are integrated, with respect to the ‘bringing-into-being’ of a design artefact (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001; Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004; Cash et al. Reference Cash, Hicks and Culley2015). This definition builds on Activity Theory (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981), which describes human activity as the unity of behaviour and cognition (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004), and explicitly differentiates this work from the terminology commonly used to describe e.g. normative new product development processes with respect to the organisation.

Current models of design activity vary widely in terms of the levels and perspectives they address; from individual mental simulation (e.g. Ball & Christensen Reference Ball and Christensen2009), to overall work processes (e.g. Cardella, Atman & Adams Reference Cardella, Atman and Adams2006), and from conceptualisation (e.g. Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Thorp Hansen and Cash2015), to informational (e.g. Robinson Reference Robinson2010a ). These varied perspectives have provided significant insights into design (Horvath Reference Horvath2004), and form a core focus of design research (Cross Reference Cross2007). However, the lack of integration across perspectives and levels has led to a theoretical fragmentation of the literature (Love Reference Love2000, Reference Love2002), that must be addressed in order to further theory building in design (Papalambros Reference Papalambros2015). For example, co-evolution describes fundamental developments in a designer’s understanding of the problem/solution space (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001), while the Design Ontology (Storga et al. Reference Storga2010) describes a range of possible activities, such as planning and testing, which are themselves distinct from the activities described by Sim & Duffy (Reference Sim and Duffy2003). Although all three works deal with aspects of design activity, their theoretical integration remains unresolved. This raises questions as to the fundamental nature, drivers for, and components of design activity. Thus, fragmentation poses a significant challenge for design activity research (Cash et al. Reference Cash, Hicks and Culley2015), as well as the design research community more generally (Love Reference Love2002; Papalambros Reference Papalambros2015).

Fragmentation of the design activity literature has two main implications for the field. First, current models typically focus on specific aspects of activity, such as sketching (Scrivener, Ball & Tseng Reference Scrivener, Ball and Tseng2000), without providing a framework for linking these descriptions to, for example information seeking (Robinson Reference Robinson2010a ). Thus, developing a cohesive description of design activity directly building on a single current model is not possible. This fundamentally limits the descriptive and predictive power of research claims in this domain. For example, no single model captures the interaction between designer understanding (Oxman Reference Oxman1997), the subsequent actions taken (Sim & Duffy Reference Sim and Duffy2003), and the underpinning iterative learning process (Demirbaş & Demirkan Reference Demirbaş and Demirkan2003), in a single unitary sense connecting behaviour and cognition (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004). Second, fragmentation hinders theory development with regard to design activity. Specifically, the lack of a common theoretical model significantly hinders efforts to integrate, elaborate on, and explain diverse empirical findings (Briggs Reference Briggs2006). This has been criticised by Love (Reference Love2000) who highlights the importance of integrative theory in the design domain. As such, there is a need to unify the fragmented perspectives on design activity in a new foundational model in order to support future theoretical and empirical development in the field.

This paper addresses this need by proposing a cohesive model of design activity. The proposed Uncertainty Driven Action (UDA) model conceptualises overall ‘design activity’ based on a review of existing formalisms described in the literature. The UDA model describes three core actions: information action (dealing with data parts and their manipulation), knowledge-sharing action (dealing with understanding and its development), and representation action (dealing with external representations such as sketching). The UDA model links these core actions through uncertainty perception, which is defined as: a designers’ perceived lack of understanding with respect to the design task and its context (Ball et al. Reference Ball1997; Kreye, Goh & Newnes Reference Kreye, Goh and Newnes2011), and has been shown to connect some aspects of behaviour and cognition in the design domain (Wiltschnig, Christensen & Ball Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013; Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ). As such, this paper contributes a comprehensive formalisation of design activity that brings together prior theoretical and empirical work in the UDA model.

2 Approach

In order to connect disparate research on design activity and unify existing perspectives an analytical conceptual approach is needed (Wacker Reference Wacker1998). This links shared elements and underlying theory via logical relationship building (Barrick, Mount & Li Reference Barrick, Mount and Li2013). This relationship building follows a logic-based approach (Wacker Reference Wacker1998), which delivers new insights by logically developing relationships between defined concepts to form an internally consistent theory (Wacker Reference Wacker1998). This section presents this approach in two stages, first identifying current design activity formalisms to form the basis for initial conceptualisation i.e. defining the key concepts to be related, and second analysing these formalisms and their link to the wider activity literature in order to distil the UDA model i.e. relating the defined concepts to form an internally consistent theory.

2.1 Identifying current formalisms

To identify current formalisms of design activity, a review of relevant publications in design research was conducted. Specifically, the following journals were reviewed as a starting point: Design Science, Design Studies, Journal of Engineering Design (JED), and International Journal of Design (IJD). These journals represent interdisciplinary design research (Design Science, Design Studies), engineering design focused research (JED), and more industrial design focused research (IJD). Design Studies is the highest impact interdisciplinary design journal, while IJD and JED are the highest rated journals in their sub-domains. These journals thus provide the basis for identifying current formalisms of design activity and the research areas they represent bound the scope of the unification claims reported in this work.

The keywords used for this review were: theory, model, and framework, implemented separately via a full text search, from archive start to 2016. This resulted in 1426 responses for theory, 2007 for model, and 1042 for framework. All responses were filtered by the authors based on whether they explicitly described a ‘formalism’ of design activity. This assessment was based on self-identification by the original authors of the reviewed papers i.e. the original authors specifically describe their formalism as a model, theory or framework. For example, the Design Ontology (Storga et al. Reference Storga2010) was described by Storga et al. as a potential framework for understanding product development work. Similarly, the ontology presented by Sim & Duffy (Reference Sim and Duffy2003) was self-identified as a system model. Where works described formalisms from outside the design domain the original reference was followed up and also included in the review. For example, Beylier et al. (Reference Beylier2009) utilise Markus’ (Reference Markus2001) theory of knowledge reuse. Papers reporting single variable relationships or empirical results only were excluded at this stage. The initial inclusion criteria for the review were thus: (1) published in a major design research journal; (2) explicitly self-identify and describe a formalism (theory, model, framework) of design activity; (3) excluding single variable relationships or purely empirical characterisations. This resulted in the initial identification of 66 unique theories, frameworks, or models (henceforth referred to as formalisms for brevity). Although this list is not exhaustive, sufficient formalisms were reviewed such that common concepts could be robustly identified.

2.2 Analytical approach

A five-step approach was used to analyse the identified formalisms. First, it was necessary to establish at what level prior formalisms describe design activity. This builds on an understanding of human activity as multi-level consisting of activity, task, and action, linked to cognition (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981; Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). Activity describes a motivation directed aggregation of lower level ‘tasks’, which are themselves aggregations of lower level ‘actions’, which connect directly to human cognition (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). Here motivation shapes overall activity, and gives a direction to the system of goals and sub-goals directing tasks and actions respectively (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981). Motivation is long-term, situated, and of varying magnitude. It is possible to hold several motivations at a single time, such as the simultaneous motivations to design a creative product and to build a well-working team (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). Lower level goals and sub-goals are thus directed by motivations. Here goals and sub-goals are conscious conceptualisations of a desired future state (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). Specifically, the following definitions were applied in the analysis of existing formalisms based on Activity Theory (and are subsequently used throughout for consistency):

Activity level : subjectively distinct periods of behaviour associated with fulfilling a motivation (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981), which are not temporally distinguished. As more than one motivation can be simultaneously held by a designer more than one activity can be progressed at one time, as illustrated by the parallel arrows at this level in figure 1 (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). An example activity is creating a novel design concept.

Task level : temporally and subjectively distinct periods of behaviour, associated with fulfilling a goal under specific conditions (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981). Each higher-level activity can link a number of tasks, but the tasks themselves can only occur sequentially (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). An example task is brainstorming ideas on paper.

Figure 1. Human activity composed of multiple aggregate levels built on a generic, unitary action/cognition foundation.

Action level : temporally distinct periods of behaviour, associated with achieving immediate, defined sub-goals linked to completing the overarching task (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981; Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004). Actions form generic building blocks from which higher-level behaviours (tasks and activities) are composed. An example action is representing part of an idea via a sketch.

Cognitive level : a continuous process and structured system of processing units that describes the storage of concepts, propositions, or images etc. as well as the system of internal mental processes underpinning behaviour (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). Dominant mental processes also underpin the taxonomic grouping and classification of actions (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). An example mental process is embodied cognition.

A ‘slice’ through all four levels provides a description of human activity as the unity of multi-level behaviour and cognition in a given situation (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005), consistent with both cognitive (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004) and behavioural theory (Oliver Reference Oliver1980; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011). This conceptualisation of activity is illustrated in figure 1. Here, higher-level activity can be characterised with respect to unitary building blocks (highlighted in figure 1) at the interface between the action level and cognition level. These unitary building blocks define core actions, which interface directly with cognition and are grouped and taxonomically defined with respect to their dominant cognitive processes (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). This has two main implications for evaluation of current formalisms of design activity. First, dominant cognitive processes can be inferred from described behaviours (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996; Wilson Reference Wilson2002) even where such a link is not characterised or described explicitly. Thus all formalisms describing some type of action, task or activity were explicitly included in the presented analysis despite the fact that many of these focus on behavioural rather than unitary (i.e. connecting behaviour and cognition) descriptions. Second, although it is possible to infer cognition connected to behaviour the reverse is not necessarily possible. Formalisms that describe abstract characterisations of the logic underpinning design or cognitive processes that are only generically associated with behaviour cannot be linked to specific actions. For example, Hatchuel & Weil’s (Reference Hatchuel and Weil2002) C–K theory characterises reasoning in design and is only generally connected to behaviour. Thus it cannot be linked to specific actions. Similarly, problem/solution co-evolution (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001) provides an abstract formalism characterising understanding development in design but again does not connect to specific actions. Formalisms that provide only cognitive or abstract logical descriptions (15 formalisms) were thus excluded from this review because it is not possible to identify the specific unitary ‘action/cognition’ building blocks necessary for a unifying model of design activity. This follows the focus on activity as the unity of behaviour and cognition and introduces a fourth inclusion criterion: formalisms explicitly describe design behaviour or behaviour and cognition in a single model. Thus 51 formalisms were taken forward into the initial analysis (Section 3.1).

In the second step, the identified formalisms from Step 1 were examined in more depth on the action level. When existing formalisms did not explicitly define actions, the articles were further examined to identify the actions underpinning the model. This resulted in a diverse list of specific action sub-types. Formalisms were analysed at the action level for three main reasons. First, actions directly interface with cognition, and can be generically classified in terms of each action’s dominant cognitive process (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004). For example, gesture and sketching are distinct behaviours but can both be understood in terms of the representational aspects of external or embodied cognition (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996; Wilson Reference Wilson2002), and thus can be conceptualised as specific instantiations of a generic representation action (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004). This allows for a unifying understanding across the diverse descriptions found in the design literature. Second, the action level provides the foundation upon which the higher aggregate levels of task and activity are built (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). These higher levels connect strings of actions in any number of sequential configurations dependent on the subject and situation. Finally, actions can be described as a self-regulating system where the individual progresses through a cycle from formulating the goal and assessing its conditions, deciding on and executing the action, and evaluating the outcome to generate a new sub-goal formulation (Engeström Reference Engeström2000). This links to characterisations of behaviour and cognition, critical to understanding causation in this context (Oliver Reference Oliver1980; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011). Thus, it is possible to cohesively connect diverse descriptions of design activity, at the action level via generic actions, consistent with underlying theories of activity, behaviour, and cognition (Leont’ev Reference Leont’ev1981; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011).

In the third step, core actions were identified by clustering the specific action sub-types from Step 2 with respect to the dominant cognitive processes explicitly or implicitly linked to the action in the reviewed formalisms. This assessment was based on the logic that actions provide independently observable and generic building blocks suitable for identifying similarities between formalisms (Engeström Reference Engeström2000; Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004), and can be grouped and taxonomically classified with respect to their dominant cognitive processes (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). Where action sub-types were described purely in terms of behaviour with no explicitly suggested cognitive process, possible dominant cognitive processes were established based on additional literature describing the action sub-type. Thus each action found to be generic and common across formalisms i.e. core, were explored in depth. This enabled us to establish three core actions, which can provide a unifying understanding of design activity.

In the fourth step, connections were identified between the three core actions identified in Step 3. These connections were required because a ‘comprehensive’ model of activity consists of the following elements with respect to the self-regulating action system (Oliver Reference Oliver1980; Engeström Reference Engeström2000; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011): antecedent i.e. the cause or driver for an action, behaviour i.e. the external action itself, and consequence i.e. evaluation of the outcome. The identified core actions provide the behavioural elements and connections between these elements can be established via an antecedent/consequence mechanism. Antecedent/consequence thus forms the specific causal mechanism for the three core actions by defining how they interact with cognition to form a self-regulating system where goal outcomes can be evaluated (Engeström Reference Engeström2000). Such self-regulation typically links basic cognitive (Evans Reference Evans2008) and metacognitive processes (Schraw & Dennison Reference Schraw and Dennison1994). While basic cognitive processes are connected directly to action, metacognitive processes allow an individual to evaluate the limitations of their current knowledge or understanding as well as regulate basic cognitive processes (Schraw & Dennison Reference Schraw and Dennison1994). Metacognitive processes thus form a key bridge from a purely behaviour/cognitive model to one that can account for memory, knowledge, and understanding because they provide knowledge about cognition. They account for individuals’ understanding of their own knowledge state, which forms a key part of design co-evolution (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001). Metacognition further regulates basic cognitive processes and can thus provide a platform for connecting diverse core actions in a single model (Schraw & Dennison Reference Schraw and Dennison1994). Metacognitive processes have further been demonstrated as important to directing design activity in prior empirical research (Ball & Christensen Reference Ball and Christensen2009). Thus, while each core action must be connected to a dominant cognitive process, a metacognitive antecedent/consequence mechanism is needed to link these core actions.

In the fifth step, the core actions were combined with cognitive and metacognitive processes in a conceptual model to fulfil the research aim. This brings together and connects core actions via common causal mechanisms, consistent with fundamental theories of human activity (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011) and prior understanding of design activity. Thus, although each element in the model draws from extant literature the final model offers a novel conceptualisation of those elements. This provides a unifying model of design activity and a foundation for future design activity research.

The five steps are reported as follows: Steps 1 and 2 in Section 3.1, Step 3 in Sections 3.23.4, Step 4 in Section 4, and Step 5 in Section 5.

3 Existing formalisms of design activity

In this section, the core actions are first distilled from the reviewed formalisms before they are individually explored in more detail with respect to the wider design and activity literatures.

3.1 Overview and scope of current formalisms

Table 1. Overview of formalisms associated with design activity, categorised with respect to their related core actions

The 51 identified formalisms are summarised in Table 1, where they are grouped with respect to their constituent core actions. This grouping was not assumed a priori and is for table clarity only. Table 1 outlines the formalisms’ general view on design activity as well as each of the specific action sub-types they describe. Each action sub-type is labelled with respect to its core action, again for clarity: information action (I), knowledge-sharing action (KS), representation action (R). In addition, for each formalism, the cognitive process associated with action is highlighted (labelled as Cog in Table 1). This is either explicit i.e. the original authors state their perspective on the link to cognition with respect to a defined cognitive process (highlighted in bold in Table 1) or implicit i.e. a cognitive process is inferred from the authors’ writing, referencing, and behaviour descriptions because no explicit statement is given. For example, Kavakli & Gero (Reference Kavakli and Gero2001) describe external cognition as the foundation of their work, and cite other papers where this cognitive process has been explicitly highlighted although they themselves do not define an explicit theory of cognition. The 51 identified formalisms were evaluated with regard to the level of their descriptions (based on Figure 1), the characterisations of the action level, and in terms of the processes they describe.

Evaluating the formalisms in terms of level, it was found that current formalisms collectively describe a wide range of specific activities, tasks, and actions. No current formalism describes the full interaction across all levels necessary for understanding design activity completely (Figure 1). For example, one of the most extensive formalisms provided by Sim & Duffy (Reference Sim and Duffy2003) touches on representation but does not deal with sketching, gesture or many of the other representation sub-types. Current formalisms typically focus on the activity or task level, and deal with the designer, their surroundings, underlying cognition, and aggregate activity, but few connect generic actions and their dominant cognitive processes. Further, formalisms offer a large breadth in descriptions of specific action sub-types, ranging from general ‘external or physical expression’ (Hertz Reference Hertz1992; Singh & Gu Reference Singh and Gu2012) to highly detailed breakdowns of representational gesture, sketching, drawing, and writing (Suwa et al. Reference Suwa, Purcell and Gero1998; Sun et al. Reference Sun2014). Descriptions vary in perspectives on the processes underpinning activity and do not deal with core actions able to support wider unification and connection across formalisms. For example, Conversation Theory (Pask Reference Pask1975) focuses on knowledge sharing via language and does not provide for integrating information or representation actions such as those outlined by Vajna et al. (Reference Vajna2005) or Sun et al. (Reference Sun2014). Thus, current formalisms do not provide a basis for unification across levels, based on core actions.

Evaluating existing formalisms on the action level, stark differences were found with regard to described sub-types and their links. No single formalism covered all action sub-types or provided a framework able to synthesise the wide range of descriptions in the literature. This can be partially attributed to the fact that most reviewed formalisms (Table 1) describe design activity disconnected from cognition. Of 51 formalisms only 17 offer some description of the link between action and an explicitly framed cognitive process (Table 1). For example, while the Design Ontology (Storga et al. Reference Storga2010) provides an excellent overview of specific information and knowledge-sharing action sub-types, these are described in behavioural terms without being explicitly linked to a cognitive process. Similarly, Moscoso (Reference Moscoso2007) focuses on actions with respect to the manufacturing process, and thus provides behavioural descriptions without connecting these to cognition. While this general focus on behaviour does not diminish the contribution of the listed formalisms, it does mean that no current formalism is suitable for providing a unifying model of design activity.

Evaluating the formalisms in terms of the processes they describe it was found that most formalisms tend to either describe the antecedent for action or action itself but not both. For example, co-evolution offers detailed insight into the underpinning drivers for action (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001), but does not connect this to a fully realised framework of activity. Similarly, Sim & Duffy (Reference Sim and Duffy2003) and Storga et al. (Reference Storga2010) detail specific action sub-types but do not tie them to cognitive and metacognitive processes in terms of antecedent/consequence. Thus current characterisation of design activity, built on a self-regulating system connecting core actions and cognition, is incomplete (Oliver Reference Oliver1980; Engeström Reference Engeström2000; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011). This initial analysis results in three distinct limitations preventing the development of a unifying model of design activity based on current formalisms:

  1. (1) Lack of cohesive theoretical description of aggregate activity built on core actions linked to cognition.

  2. (2) Lack of common and generic core actions able to connect the varied descriptions of design activity currently found in the literature.

  3. (3) Incomplete theoretical description from antecedent-to-consequence connecting core actions and cognition.

Due to these limitations, it was necessary to identify core actions that enable unification of design activity. This was done in four phases. First, all explicit cognitive processes were clustered based on common theoretical elements. For example, a cluster of explicit cognitive processes could be identified by their common focus on describing processes where mental structures are externally represented, whether formalised as external cognition, embodied cognition or experiential cognition. This resulted in three clusters: information-based cognition including coordinated information processing (Suss & Thomson Reference Suss and Thomson2012), and information processing (Aurisicchio et al. Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2013), knowledge-based (group) cognition including memory-based cognition (Liikkanen & Perttula Reference Liikkanen and Perttula2010), group cognition (Pask Reference Pask1975; Dong Reference Dong2005; Dong et al. Reference Dong, Kleinsmann and Deken2013; Eris et al. Reference Eris, Martelaro and Badke-Schaub2014), and collective cognition (Kleinsmann et al. Reference Kleinsmann2012), and representation-based embodied cognition including external cognition (Van Der Lugt Reference Van Der Lugt2005; Bilda & Gero Reference Bilda and Gero2007; Aurisicchio et al. Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2013), embodied cognition (Daley Reference Daley1982; Oxman Reference Oxman1997; Viswanathan et al. Reference Viswanathan2014), experiential cognition (Demirbaş & Demirkan Reference Demirbaş and Demirkan2003; Gero & Kannengiesser Reference Gero and Kannengiesser2004; Singh & Gu Reference Singh and Gu2012), and thought/language cognition (Fox Reference Fox1981; Eris et al. Reference Eris, Martelaro and Badke-Schaub2014).

Second, the specific action sub-types associated with these three clusters were grouped using explicitly connected formalisms in order to distil core actions characterised by these dominant cognitive processes. For example, the specific action sub-types associated with the initial ‘representation-based embodied cognition’ cluster included formulation, synthesis, analysis, evaluation, documentation, reformulation, language-based representation, physical representation, idea manifestation, external expression, sketching, drawing, gesture, physically storing ideas, physically conveying information, representing ideas, and physically engaging attention (Table 1). Based on this an initial definition was developed for a core action of representation able to accommodate each of these specific action sub-types with respect to a common dominant cognitive process.

Third, this initial core action definition was evaluated with respect to all formalisms that implicitly connected to representation-based embodied cognition, and their associated specific action sub-types. Again these action sub-types were listed and incorporated into the core action conceptualisation where they could be connected to the underlying cognitive process. Where action sub-types did not fit with the underlying cognitive process connected a specific core action, they were compared with other cognitive process clusters. This was the case with, for example, Vajna et al. (Reference Vajna2005), where the action sub-types: evaluation, selection, replication, recombination, and mutation, were all framed within the original text as representation-based actions but were analogous to actions described with respect to formalisms explicitly connected to information-based cognition. This resulted in the identification of a set of action sub-types for each of the three cognitive process clusters.

Fourth, the sets of action sub-types were synthesised through a definition of the core action. These definitions were evaluated iteratively to eliminate possible overlap in action sub-types. This process resulted in three core action definitions based on the reviewed formalisms in Table 1: information action, knowledge-sharing action, and representation action. These core actions bring together the various formalisms from the literature in terms of common cognitive processes.

We propose the three core actions as a foundation for connecting current formalisms of design activity. However, despite the review and distillation of the core actions reported in this section, further refinement is still required due to the lack of definitive descriptions in the existing literature, the high variety of action sub-type descriptions, and relative lack of explicit links to dominant cognitive processes. The review was thus expanded to include the wider design literature where there are many empirical works not described with respect to a specific formalism. For example, Wasiak et al. (Reference Wasiak2010) provide an extensive list of specific action sub-types associated with information action, without explicitly connecting to a formalised model of design activity or to a specific cognitive process. The specific logic underpinning this expanded review is to establish the nature and scope of the core actions and their sub-types in the empirical design literature. This elaboration is necessary to ensure the robustness of the core actions by contrasting and integrating these additional works. Thus the nature of the core actions and their connection with the design and activity literatures via dominant cognitive process and a common metacognitive process is explored in the following sections, which expand the scope of the original review.

3.2 Information action

Information action can be defined as dealing with data parts and their manipulation (Court Reference Court1997). This type of action is associated with cognitive processes describing how data parts are identified, manipulated, and transformed into information and subsequent knowledge (Wilson Reference Wilson1999). This includes the collection, recording, reviewing, and filing of data parts (Blandin & Brown Reference Blandin and Brown1977) via various means including human and non-human (Robinson Reference Robinson2010a ), formal and informal (Fidel & Green Reference Fidel and Green2004). This type of action and its associated cognitive process deal with data parts detached from an individual’s beliefs (Belkin, Oddy & Brooks Reference Belkin, Oddy and Brooks1982; Song, Van Der Bij & Weggeman Reference Song, Van Der Bij and Weggeman2005). It has been shown to be a distinct and important action within design work (Cave & Noble Reference Cave and Noble1986; Puttre Reference Puttre1991), accounting for a significant portion of engineers’ and designers’ time (Court Reference Court1997; Robinson Reference Robinson2010a ). Further, identification and selection of data parts plays a critical role in ideation and design creativity (Gonçalves, Cardoso & Badke-Schaub Reference Gonçalves, Cardoso and Badke-Schaub2016). Some authors even characterise design primarily as an information transformation process where data parts are sought, reasoned about, and stored, via e.g. seeking and requesting (Aurisicchio, Bracewell & Wallace Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010; Wasiak et al. Reference Wasiak2010).

Information action has been generally characterised in terms of: finding data parts ${>}$ reasoning about/validating ${>}$ using (Belkin et al. Reference Belkin, Oddy and Brooks1982); driven by perceived need – connected to its role (Belkin et al. Reference Belkin, Oddy and Brooks1982). For example, Wasiak et al. (Reference Wasiak2010) and Cash, Hicks & Culley (Reference Cash, Hicks and Culley2013) characterise information action in the design process in terms of its role e.g. solving. However, research has typically focused on sources and media, such as the internet (Oh, Oh & Shah Reference Oh, Oh and Shah2009), or on aggregate information action with little link to design work, such as total information acquisition (Hult, Ketchen & Slater Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004). Thus although basic taxonomies of information action, such as that by Belkin et al. (Reference Belkin, Oddy and Brooks1982), do exist, and have been included in manifest descriptions such as those offered by Robinson (Reference Robinson2010b ) or Cash et al. (Reference Cash, Hicks and Culley2015), they have not been theoretically integrated with other core actions e.g. representation (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996).

Two main observations emerge from the literature on information action. First, new information is processed through reasoning or learning. It is then structured, and evaluated through experience (Tracey & Hutchinson Reference Tracey and Hutchinson2016) before entering the ‘human belief system’ (Song et al. Reference Song, Van Der Bij and Weggeman2005) to create knowledge. Thus information action must be distinguished from action related to knowledge sharing. Second, information action is connected to designers’ understanding of a situation via their perceived need (Borlund Reference Borlund2003). This has been linked to designer uncertainty perception by Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub (Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011). Here, designers seek to resolve their uncertainty perception via information action including seeking, gathering, and reasoning about data parts (Kim & Lee Reference Kim and Lee2016). This increases their understanding and thus gradually reduces uncertainty perception over time (Yu et al. Reference Yu2016). The link between uncertainty perception and information action stems back to describing design as a decision making process under uncertainty (Beheshti Reference Beheshti1993) where design progresses as design-related decisions are made and implemented (Wilson Reference Wilson1999; Suss & Thomson Reference Suss and Thomson2012). Thus, uncertainty perception forms a key motivator of information action described in the design literature. Thus, one key mechanism driving information action is the designer’s aim to reduce their uncertainty perception.

3.3 Knowledge-sharing action

Knowledge-sharing action can be defined as dealing with the creation and development of shared understanding (Dong Reference Dong2005). This type of action is associated with cognitive processes describing how knowledge is expressed with respect to an individual’s understanding and beliefs (Court Reference Court1997; Chiu, Hsu & Wang Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006), as well as the structural (i.e. social), relational, and procedural (i.e. shared language or vision) context of an action (Song et al. Reference Song, Van Der Bij and Weggeman2005; Chiu et al. Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006). Thus these actions and their associated cognitive process are fundamentally linked to an individual’s own understanding and beliefs, distinguishing them from information action (Song et al. Reference Song, Van Der Bij and Weggeman2005; Chiu et al. Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006). Knowledge-sharing action forms a critical part of idea sharing (Liikkanen & Perttula Reference Liikkanen and Perttula2010), group creativity (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ), group learning (Shull et al. Reference Shull2004), is underpinned by effective communication (Preston, Karahanna & Rowe Reference Preston, Karahanna and Rowe2006), and is often directed towards iteratively grounding shared understanding (Clark & Brennan Reference Clark and Brennan1991) where individuals seek to establish that shared knowledge is actually understood as intended. Thus knowledge-sharing action is characterised by its often discursive nature (Deken et al. Reference Deken2012) where exchanges bring together, fact, rationale, context, varied perspectives, and exploration (Eris Reference Eris2002; Aurisicchio et al. Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010). For example, Eris (Reference Eris2002) describes 22 specific types of question, while Aurisicchio et al. (Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010) categorise knowledge-sharing requests with respect to their objective e.g. comparison, their subject e.g. product or process, and their response type e.g. retrieval of information. Further, knowledge-sharing action underpins collaborative creative efforts where exchange fosters knowledge acquisition and creation within a group (Shull et al. Reference Shull2004), as highlighted in the recent work of Sauder & Jin (Reference Sauder and Jin2016).

Knowledge-sharing action connects to design formalisms at all levels of description (Maznevski & Chudoba Reference Maznevski and Chudoba2000; Dong Reference Dong2005). However, research has typically focused on understanding knowledge sharing in terms of its role in the development of shared understanding (Preston et al. Reference Preston, Karahanna and Rowe2006; Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008), or its manifestation through various communicative media (Cash & Maier Reference Cash and Maier2016). For example, Maznevski & Chudoba (Reference Maznevski and Chudoba2000) explore team effectiveness in relation to communication intensity; while Aurisicchio et al. (Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010) highlight the need to support designers in sharing and capturing knowledge. No single taxonomy of knowledge-sharing action is completely accepted (Eris Reference Eris2002; Aurisicchio, Bracewell & Wallace Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2006), and despite the fundamentality of knowledge sharing (Fairlie-Clarke & Muller Reference Fairlie-Clarke and Muller2003) it has not been formalised in generic terms or with respect to its relationship with the other core actions.

Two main observations emerge from the literature on knowledge-sharing action. First, it is critically linked to the development of shared understanding (Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008). For example, Conversation Theory (Pask Reference Pask1975) describes the gradual development of alignment in understanding expressed in terms of semantic coherence in word use, as illustrated in design by Dong (Reference Dong2005). Although this is also linked to a number of wider social processes (Busby Reference Busby2001; Chiu et al. Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006), shared context (Humayun & Gang Reference Humayun and Gang2013), and quality of communication (Maznevski & Chudoba Reference Maznevski and Chudoba2000), the development of shared understanding is a core characteristic of successful design teams (Dong Reference Dong2005). It is important to note that although knowledge-sharing action is typically part of an interpersonal exchange it can also be captured in asynchronous modes, where the addressee is unknown or simply imagined, such as in personal letters or journals (Clark & Brennan Reference Clark and Brennan1991; McAlpine, Cash & Hicks Reference McAlpine, Cash and Hicks2017). Thus, although this is a core part of team interaction, the actions themselves are undertaken by the individual. Second, knowledge-sharing action can refer to vision and identity (Chiu et al. Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006), concept understanding (Dong Reference Dong2005), solution understanding (Preston et al. Reference Preston, Karahanna and Rowe2006), and understanding of organisational elements e.g. team roles (Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008). Here, uncertainty perception is particularly connected to others’ understanding of these elements as a driver for knowledge sharing in practice (Clark & Brennan Reference Clark and Brennan1991; Deken et al. Reference Deken2012). Similarly the population of knowledge spaces e.g. problem/solution in co-evolution (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001), can be related to uncertainty perception via the work of Kreye et al. (Reference Kreye, Goh and Newnes2011) and Christensen & Ball (Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ) who connect knowledge sharing, uncertainty perception, and underlying cognitive processes. Here, uncertainty perception is again characterised as a general metacognitive process which drives knowledge-sharing action.

3.4 Representation action

Representation action can be defined as dealing with the perception and manipulation of external representations of information (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996; Wilson Reference Wilson2002). This type of action is associated with cognitive processes describing the interplay between internal and external representations (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996; Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). Representation action is often associated with knowledge structures and the exploration of the design space (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001; Hatchuel & Weil Reference Hatchuel and Weil2003), and provides a cognitively economic means of externalising ideas (Brun, Le Masson & Weil Reference Brun, Le Masson and Weil2016). This has been described via formalisms such as external (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996) and embodied (Wilson Reference Wilson2002) cognition, where an individual uses the interplay between internal/external representations to directly support cognition and develop understanding (Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996; Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). Representations have also been referred to as simulation in the design literature (Taura et al. Reference Taura2012; Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013), as such, representation is adopted here for clarity. Specifically, representation actions deal with external representations e.g. prototyping or computational modelling, distinct from internal mental simulation as described by Wiltschnig et al. (Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). The importance of representation action is highlighted in numerous contexts e.g. via gesture (Cash & Maier Reference Cash and Maier2016), prototyping (Sanders & Stappers Reference Sanders and Stappers2014), or sketching (Schön & Wiggins Reference Schön and Wiggins1992).

Representation action has been widely acknowledged as central to design work (Sim & Duffy Reference Sim and Duffy2003; Horvath Reference Horvath2004; Andreasen et al. Reference Andreasen, Thorp Hansen and Cash2015). In particular, research in this area has focused on understanding and describing representation (Dorst & Vermaas Reference Dorst and Vermaas2005), its relationship to reasoning about design (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001; Hatchuel & Weil Reference Hatchuel and Weil2003), and how it is realised in practice (Kan, Gero & Tang Reference Kan, Gero, Tang and Gero2011). For example, Kulkarni et al. (Reference Kulkarni, Summers, Vargas-Hernandez and Shah2000) examine how collaborative sketching can support idea generation/manifestation and joint representation in teams. Further, specific aspects of representation action have again been connected to uncertainty perception. For example, Gursoy & Ozkar (Reference Gursoy and Ozkar2015) show how penmanship and sketching help to reveal and resolve uncertainty perception while Scrivener et al. (Reference Scrivener, Ball and Tseng2000) connect sketching and uncertainty perception explicitly in two ways: first, uncertainty perception forms a trigger for strategic shifts within sketching; second, sketching can engender uncertainty perception by elucidating differences between the drawing and the designer’s own understanding and memory. Similarly, Gerber & Carroll (Reference Gerber and Carroll2012) highlight how prototyping also helps to elicit and resolve uncertainty perception, by allowing individuals to iteratively build knowledge and promote a sense of control. However, the centrality of representation action and its interconnection with information action and knowledge-sharing action are not fully captured in current design activity formalisms (Table 1).

Two main observations emerge from the literature on representation action. First, like descriptions of information or knowledge-sharing action, representation is typically described as a multi-faceted phenomenon, linking the design artefact (Gero Reference Gero1990), the specific knowledge being represented (Storga et al. Reference Storga2010), physical modality (Schön & Wiggins Reference Schön and Wiggins1992), and model granularity (Maier, Eckert & Clarkson Reference Maier, Eckert and Clarkson2017). Thus representation action is closely linked to knowledge sharing. For example, representational gesturing can both facilitate individual understanding via external cognition, and the development of team shared understanding through e.g. mirroring and modification (Stempfle & Badke-Schaub Reference Stempfle and Badke-Schaub2002; Cash & Maier Reference Cash and Maier2016). Second, representation is linked to improved understanding and ability to communicate within a team (Schön & Wiggins Reference Schön and Wiggins1992; Scaife & Rogers Reference Scaife and Rogers1996) e.g. by supporting the ConceptKnowledge interaction (Hatchuel & Weil Reference Hatchuel and Weil2003). Here, uncertainty perception is modified via representation surrounding the design artefact as illustrated by the work of Gerber & Carroll (Reference Gerber and Carroll2012) who describe uncertainty perception as a driver for representation action. Thus representation action is again linked to the driver: designer uncertainty perception.

4 Linking the three core actions through uncertainty perception

The three core actions identified and described in Section 3 conceptualise the unitary building blocks upon which a foundational understanding of design activity can be built. To link these unitary building blocks, a metacognitive element is needed (Section 2), suitable for connecting multiple cognitive processes, and thus connecting the three core actions. Key literature associated with each core action e.g. Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub (Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011) and Gerber & Carroll (Reference Gerber and Carroll2012), suggests that uncertainty perception forms a suitable metacognitive process for this purpose. Other possible concepts could be ambiguity (Ellsberg Reference Ellsberg2001) or risk attitudes (Davies Reference Davies2006); however, uncertainty perception is the concept most commonly described in the metacognitive literature (Schraw & Dennison Reference Schraw and Dennison1994; Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ) and had been described in relation to each of the three core actions within the design literature. As such, this section explores the potential for uncertainty perception to connect the core actions via a consistent antecedent/consequence mechanism.

Utilising uncertainty perception as a common causal mechanism has three main advantages. First, it robustly links cognition (Tversky & Kahneman Reference Tversky and Kahneman1974; Daft & Lengel Reference Daft and Lengel1986) and behaviour (Ball et al. Reference Ball1997). Uncertainty perception as a metacognitive process fulfils two key roles in connecting multiple cognitive processes and core actions: reflecting knowledge about cognition and regulating cognition itself. These roles of uncertainty perception have been described in the design literature by, for example, Gerber & Carroll (Reference Gerber and Carroll2012) who illustrate how uncertainty perception associated with declarative knowledge about self (sense of progress, self-belief etc.) and learning progression is affected by prototyping. Further, authors such as Deken et al. (Reference Deken2012) and Wiltschnig et al. (Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013) describe uncertainty perception as a driver for a range of specific action sub-types, while Blandin & Brown (Reference Blandin and Brown1977) and Hult et al. (Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004) described it as a driver for overall activity. Thus uncertainty perception provides a general metacognitive process that connects to memory and individual experience by incorporating knowledge about cognition as well as regulating own cognition and directing action.

Second, uncertainty perception can be characterised consistently across levels (Figure 1) and across the three core actions. In particular, it connects understanding/knowledge, memory, and perception, to activity progression, thus bridging cognition and action. Further, as highlighted in Sections 3.23.4 uncertainty perception, defined as a general metacognitive process, has been shown to be an important driver with respect to each of the core actions despite the different empirical and behavioural foci of design research. The characterisation of uncertainty perception is explicitly general i.e. the conceptualisation of uncertainty perception used by Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub (Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011) with respect to information action is analogous to the conceptualisation used by Wiltschnig et al. (Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013) with respect to representation action. Thus uncertainty perception provides a general mechanism that can be consistently characterised irrespective of specific behaviour or cognitive process, making it suitable for connecting multiple core actions associated with different dominant cognitive processes.

Third, uncertainty perception is fundamentally linked to an individual’s understanding of a situation (Tversky & Kahneman Reference Tversky and Kahneman1974), making it theoretically consistent with the cognitive processes identified with respect to the core actions. In summary, uncertainty perception thus forms a key antecedent/consequence mechanism consistent with descriptions in the literature (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004; Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005) and connects the three core actions, shaping their selection and sequential combination, and subsequently driving overall design activity.

An important distinction here is between the general concept of uncertainty, extant in a situation (extant uncertainty), and the metacognitive concept connected to an individual, in the sense of making him/her feel unsure or unconfident (uncertainty perception). Extant uncertainty can stem from organisational and social issues, technology development, or other aspects of a situation (Calantone & Rubera Reference Calantone and Rubera2012), and is a characteristic feature of design work, linked to the unknown nature of the task outcome (Tracey & Hutchinson Reference Tracey and Hutchinson2016). In addition there are a number of other concepts related to extant uncertainty, such as, ambiguity, equivocality, or ‘lack of knowledge’ (Suss & Thomson Reference Suss and Thomson2012). Ambiguity defines a situation where the available information or problem description does not give a consistent or coherent picture (Ellsberg Reference Ellsberg2001), while equivocality describes the possibility of there being multiple or conflicting interpretations of a situation (Daft, Lengel & Trevino Reference Daft, Lengel and Trevino1987). However, before an individual can act on any such manifest concepts, they must perceive and process them with respect to their own understanding of the situation (Tversky & Kahneman Reference Tversky and Kahneman1974; Evans & Stanovich Reference Evans and Stanovich2013), thus they are transformed into uncertainty perception. Manifest concepts such as extant uncertainty or lack of knowledge only affect activity if they are perceived by the designer via this metacognitive awareness (Schraw & Dennison Reference Schraw and Dennison1994; Ball & Christensen Reference Ball and Christensen2009). Uncertainty perception describes the mental state of an individual, such as a designer, who faces uncertainty and may thus feel unconfident in their activity (Kreye Reference Kreye2016). Uncertainty perception thus provides a bridge from the literature that generally highlights extant uncertainty (or other manifest concepts) as an important feature of design work (Tracey & Hutchinson Reference Tracey and Hutchinson2016) to an explicit causal mechanism linking behaviour and cognition in relation to design activity. This understanding of uncertainty perception is adopted throughout this work.

Uncertainty perception reflects how the designer understands a situation with respect to their personality, experience, and other personal characteristics (Kreye Reference Kreye2016; Tracey & Hutchinson Reference Tracey and Hutchinson2016) and does not necessarily accurately reflect the level of extant uncertainty. Uncertainty perception thus captures an individual’s understanding translated into an antecedent through perception, based on their own experience (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ; Tracey & Hutchinson Reference Tracey and Hutchinson2016). This has been captured in the design domain where design activity is described as a chain of decisions made under uncertainty where knowledge develops (Beheshti Reference Beheshti1993; Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001; Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). Similarly, uncertainty perception has been highlighted in the wider product development literature as an important driver of activity (Daft & Lengel Reference Daft and Lengel1986; O’Connor & Rice Reference O’Connor and Rice2013; Kreye Reference Kreye2016). Uncertainty perception thus forms a unifying mechanism with respect to cognition and the core actions (Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011; Deken et al. Reference Deken2012; Gerber & Carroll Reference Gerber and Carroll2012).

In the design literature a number of studies have connected the progression of specific action sub-types to uncertainty perception (Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). Bringing the literature together, uncertainty perception has been separately described as a driver of specific information (Borlund Reference Borlund2003; Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011), knowledge-sharing (Deken et al. Reference Deken2012), and representation actions (Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). In addition, a small number of works have pointed to the role of uncertainty perception in directing the progression of specific action sub-types, such as stimulating recall within sketching (Scrivener et al. Reference Scrivener, Ball and Tseng2000). Descriptions of uncertainty perception in the design literature have two main limitations. First, the effect of uncertainty perception as a driver of activity has only been studied with respect to the progression of specific action sub-types (e.g. sketching). This is in contrast to formalisms where wider activity progression and connection across the core actions has been highlighted (Daft & Lengel Reference Daft and Lengel1986; Hult et al. Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004). Second, in design, uncertainty perception has been characterised predominantly as a unitary whole with no subdivision and using a binary existence scale. For example, Ball & Christensen (Reference Ball and Christensen2009) treat uncertainty perception via binary schema i.e. either present/not present (Ball & Christensen Reference Ball and Christensen2009; Ball, Onarheim & Christensen Reference Ball, Onarheim and Christensen2010) or low/high (Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013; Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ). This is in contrast to the multi-faceted, scalar conceptualisation found in the management literature (Hurley, Kosenko & Brashers Reference Hurley, Kosenko and Brashers2011; O’Connor & Rice Reference O’Connor and Rice2013) and necessary for understanding the multiple factors influencing the core actions. Thus, important insights can, and need to, be drawn from other fields such as psychology and management.

Given these considerations the impact of uncertainty perception on action varies in two main dimensions: level and nature. The level of uncertainty perception describes the overall amount of the perceived lack of understanding (Kreye et al. Reference Kreye2012). Here, an individual can perceive uncertainty anywhere from ignorance (where design factors are unknown or unknowable) to certainty (where the designer perceives no uncertainty regarding the design task). The level of uncertainty perception should reduce over the duration of the design activity (Wynn, Grebici & Clarkson Reference Wynn, Grebici and Clarkson2011; Yu et al. Reference Yu2016) because the designer’s understanding of the development task gradually increases as interdependencies are clarified (Yu et al. Reference Yu2016). As suggested by the recent work of Christensen & Ball (Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ) the dynamic interplay between changing uncertainty perception and action drive the overall progression of design activity.

The nature of uncertainty perception arises from its heterogeneous character in design (Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008; O’Connor & Rice Reference O’Connor and Rice2013). This describes the perception of different uncertainty types as drivers of activity, by shaping action selection and sequential progression. For example, uncertainty can arise from the external environment (Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008) or the level of technical innovation in the design task (O’Connor & Rice Reference O’Connor and Rice2013). Numerous works have illustrated how different uncertainty types, or combinations of types, influence activity in distinct ways (Bstieler Reference Bstieler2005; Heavey & Simsek Reference Heavey and Simsek2013). In design, all types can be expected to shape activity (Garcia Reference Garcia2005), particularly as designers often face multi-faceted problems where they must deal with team, organisation, and process issues in addition to pure product considerations (Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008). Specifically, Bstieler (Reference Bstieler2005) and Biazzo (Reference Biazzo2009) highlight how activity is typically driven by one or two types in any given situation. Thus the composite perception of these uncertainty types i.e. the nature of uncertainty perception, is an important determinant of design activity progression.

Finally, although uncertainty perception provides a robust and theoretically consistent link between the core actions underpinning design activity, it is not necessarily the only possible unifying element. However, due to the extent of the empirical and theoretical support for uncertainty perception, it provides an ideal unifying antecedent/consequence mechanism for the proposed UDA model.

5 The Uncertainty Driven Action (UDA) Model

This section describes the proposed UDA model. We first synthesise the three core actions together with uncertainty perception before describing the progression through the UDA model.

5.1 UDA Elements

The UDA model takes its starting point in the designer’s uncertainty perception . Uncertainty perception forms the antecedent (i.e. causal mechanism) for activity in the UDA model because it motivates the three core actions described below. It connects activity to cognition via the core actions, with respect to memory and experience (Oxman Reference Oxman1990). It forms a metacognitive process that deals with an individuals’ knowledge about their own cognition as well as regulation of cognition itself (Schraw & Dennison Reference Schraw and Dennison1994). It is important to note that although current literature typically highlights the drive to reduce uncertainty perception over time (Ball & Christensen Reference Ball and Christensen2009; Yu et al. Reference Yu2016), UDA uses the more neutral modify. This distinction is made to highlight the fact that actions can both increase or decrease uncertainty perception (Nagai & Gero Reference Nagai and Gero2012). However, overall reduction is to be expected over the whole duration of the design process (Hult et al. Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004). Uncertainty perception can be varied and complex because it includes level and nature, and captures uncertainty perception about the design itself as well as wider organisational issues (Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008). In combination, the level and nature of uncertainty perception can determine action selection and progression in terms of information, knowledge sharing, and representation. Further, changes in any of these dimensions (level, nature or both) form the trigger for shifting between elements within the UDA model. Thus the UDA model allows for the dynamic interaction between changing uncertainty perception and action progression, providing a unifying foundation for describing aggregate activity.

In information action a designer seeks to modify their uncertainty perception by working with data parts and their manipulation. This can be through e.g. collection, recording, reviewing, filing, archiving, seeking, and requesting (Blandin & Brown Reference Blandin and Brown1977; Robinson Reference Robinson2010b ). Individual designers process this via reasoning or learning resulting in a new uncertainty perception state (Daft & Lengel Reference Daft and Lengel1986; Aurisicchio et al. Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010). The dominant cognitive process is information processing (Song et al. Reference Song, Van Der Bij and Weggeman2005). Examples of information action include, searching for data online (Aurisicchio et al. Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010), or providing/asking for specific data (Robinson Reference Robinson2010a ).

In knowledge-sharing action a designer seeks to modify their uncertainty perception by exchanging knowledge expressed with respect to their understanding and beliefs to e.g. develop a shared understanding with the design team (Daft & Lengel Reference Daft and Lengel1986; Dong Reference Dong2005; Kleinsmann & Valkenburg Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008). Individual designers process this exchange resulting in a new uncertainty perception state. The dominant cognitive process is social cognition (Chiu et al. Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006). Examples of knowledge-sharing action include, expressing belief modified knowledge in conversation (Dong Reference Dong2005), or creative exploration (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ).

In representation action a designer seeks to modify their uncertainty perception by the manipulation of external information representations through, for example, computational simulation (Lamarra & Dunphy Reference Lamarra and Dunphy1998). Individual designers process this by reflecting on the external representation in relation to their internal simulations (Wilson Reference Wilson2002; Evans Reference Evans2008). The dominant cognitive process is embodied cognition (Wilson Reference Wilson2002). Examples of representation action include, sketching (Schön & Wiggins Reference Schön and Wiggins1992), prototyping (Gerber & Carroll Reference Gerber and Carroll2012), or gesturing (Cash & Maier Reference Cash and Maier2016).

In order to describe activity fully, behaviour and cognition must be considered in unity. Thus each of the core actions are connected to cognitive processing (Evans Reference Evans2008), which allows for reflective practice (Schön & Wiggins Reference Schön and Wiggins1992), reasoning, sense-making (Aurisicchio et al. Reference Aurisicchio, Bracewell and Wallace2010), creative imagination (Hatchuel & Weil Reference Hatchuel and Weil2002), and mental simulation (Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013). Cognitive processing includes both fast and un-deliberate, as well as slow and deliberate processes (Evans & Stanovich Reference Evans and Stanovich2013), however, such decomposition is not necessary for the purposes of the proposed model. In particular, there is a relative lack of unanimity across formalisms of human processing in the psychology literature (Francis et al. Reference Francis2009), as well as their application in the design domain. Thus by combining these elements the UDA model captures the whole cycle from uncertainty perception through behaviour to cognition, as a single unity of activity.

Figure 2. The UDA model linking internal metacognitive uncertainty perception (red) and cognitive processing (grey), and externally enacted information, knowledge-sharing, and representation actions (black). As a whole, the UDA model composes one ‘building block’ denoted by Note 2 in Figure 1.

The UDA model is illustrated in Figure 2. Here the internal world of the designer (including uncertainty perception, cognitive processing, and the reflective link between them) and the external world where action can be observed (including information, knowledge-sharing, and representation actions) are explicitly connected via causal links to produce an overall unity between behaviour and cognition. As such, the model cannot be decomposed to only action or cognition if an overall understanding of activity is to be achieved. Thus, the model must be considered as a whole system. Actions are denoted in black – these include the three core actions as the common generic behaviours from which higher-level tasks and activities are composed; cognitive processing is denoted in grey – this comprises the cognitive processes that underpin the core actions, as well as the other systems making up an individual’s cognitive framework; finally, uncertainty perception is denoted in red – this represents the metacognitive antecedence/consequence mechanism linked to understanding, that connects action and cognition. These elements together make up the unitary action/cognition building blocks at the foundation of activity (Section 2).

All of these elements together make up the foundation of activity, and are couched within the wider context of the situation and the designer (Briggs Reference Briggs2006). A full deconstruction of the different contextual factors that influence activity is beyond the scope of this work and forms a significant body of study in its own right (Bedny & Harris Reference Bedny and Harris2005). The context can include the wider social context (Dong Reference Dong2005), discipline (Yilmaz et al. Reference Yilmaz2015), and personal experience (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ). Furthermore, the individual designer links to the wider group (design team and organisation) (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ; Kreye Reference Kreye2016). However, all actions in the UDA model are undertaken by an individual (and can all be carried out in any setting, in a group or alone), building on the core understanding of design activity as the unity of individual behaviour and cognition. Group dynamics can be understood by, for example, modelling the actions of each individual and then examining their connection and interaction (Garcia Reference Garcia2005). In this regard it is possible to imagine each individual as semi-autonomous within a network of connected individuals that act independently but influence each other through their actions (McCarthy et al. Reference McCarthy2006). Alternatively, assessment of actions or uncertainty perception can be aggregated across a population in order to identify overall trends or quantitative relationships (Hult et al. Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004). Thus the individual can be connected to the wider group or system by building on the UDA model. However, this interaction is not currently well developed and is substantially beyond the scope of this paper. Finally, deconstructing each element into its sub-constituents e.g. internal processes underpinning cognition, or physical processes and specific instantiations making up the core actions, is also beyond the scope of this work. This is because current formalisms of design activity do not offer consistently detailed and accepted deconstructions of all elements.

5.2 UDA progression

The UDA elements are connected in a unitary model describing the synthesis of behaviour and cognition fundamental to understanding design activity (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004), and cyclical action progression (Engeström Reference Engeström2000). Each cycle starts with an antecedent change in the designer’s uncertainty perception. Change in uncertainty perception is used here as a driver for action (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ) and can be evaluated in absolute or relative terms (Chan, Paletz & Schunn Reference Chan, Paletz and Schunn2012; Paletz, Chan & Schunn Reference Paletz, Chan and Schunn2017). In reaction to this change in uncertainty perception the designer enters one of the three action cycles. These form the behaviour element of the model. The outcomes from this behaviour then feed into the designer’s cognitive processing system (Bilda & Gero Reference Bilda and Gero2007; Evans Reference Evans2008) where it drives a subsequent change of state in the designer’s uncertainty perception. Cognitive processing (Evans Reference Evans2008) and the change in uncertainty perception form the consequence element in the model and close the cycle. UDA thus captures the full cycle of action from antecedent to consequence, connecting behaviour and cognition (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004), to form the foundation for understanding design activity. In addition to the action cycles it is also possible for a designer to simply reflect on their own understanding through a cognitive processing/uncertainty perception loop.

Each cycle forms a generic building block that can be completed multiple times, for example, addressing different information sources (Song et al. Reference Song, Van Der Bij and Weggeman2005) such as searching for specific information on a webpage and then sharing the interpretation of that information with the team, communication modes (Cash & Maier Reference Cash and Maier2016), or representation media (Sanders & Stappers Reference Sanders and Stappers2014). Further, actions can be directed towards different design goals e.g. ideation or concept refinement (Yang Reference Yang2009), or aspects of the design artefact (Gero Reference Gero1990). The core actions and uncertainty perception dynamically interact and co-vary to provide the foundation for describing aggregate activity progression, i.e. the building up of an overall activity by the sequential connection of multiple unitary action cycles each encapsulating behaviour and cognition. Thus UDA is able to describe iterative, dynamic activity via the sequential completion of multiple action cycles.

Figure 3. Possible progressions allowed by the UDA model, and an example sequence of actions linked by evolving uncertainty perception (UP $_{\text{n}}$ ). Here each action cycle in the sequence is a single iteration of the UDA model and together they form the foundation for activity as explained in Figure 1.

The sequential progression through the UDA model is illustrated in Figure 3. This highlights the different possible progressions allowed by the model, as well as examples of the features of design work that these describe. Each cycle is initiated by the current uncertainty perception state (UP $_{1}$ ) and ends with a new uncertainty perception state (UP $_{2}$ ). Finally, a simplified abstract illustration is used to show the dynamic interaction between changing uncertainty perception and action progression.

Together the proposed elements and cyclical progression allow for a model that offers an integrative understanding of design activity built on a foundation of the three core actions connected to cognition. This is consistent with current design activity formalisms. Further, by integrating these elements the UDA model is able to describe the major features of design activity found in the empirical literature.

6 Discussion

Based on the UDA model, suggestions for future research directions in the form of ‘propositions’ can be derived. This section discusses the proposed UDA model with regard to its link to the design literature and subsequently suggests three propositions for future work.

6.1 Link to the design literature

The proposed UDA model extends the design literature by unifying previously disparate research. In particular, it unifies the varied descriptions of the core actions underpinning design activity. More generally it explains the gradual resolution of uncertainty perception related to the creation of a design artefact (Hult et al. Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004). Further, it extends prior descriptions of uncertainty perception as a driver of specific action sub-types and connects these in a generic model of design activity progression built on the sequential selection and combination of the three core actions. The UDA model hence formalises descriptions of design activity, consistent with Activity Theory as well as extant empirical studies of designing (Eris et al. Reference Eris, Martelaro and Badke-Schaub2014; Crilly Reference Crilly2015). The model’s applicability is further supported by its consistency with prior empirical and theoretical work in the design literature. This illustrates the UDA model’s robustness in linking behaviour and cognition, and its ability to integrate the three core actions underpinning design activity. Finally, the dynamic interaction between changing uncertainty perception and activity progression, driven by the action cycles, highlights the potential for explaining design activity from antecedent to consequence.

Each element included in the UDA model constitutes a distinct area of research in design. Although further decomposition is possible, current descriptions are not consistent across all elements. This bounds the scope of the proposed model without prescribing sub-categorisations. However, the proposed model does define the overall nature of each core action, based on its dominant cognitive process (Bedny & Karwowski Reference Bedny and Karwowski2004), and thus guides where such sub-categorisations could be included. For example, Eris’s (Reference Eris2002) breakdown of questioning types offers a specific decomposition of one aspect of knowledge-sharing action. Other examples of knowledge-sharing sub-categorisations are Wasiak et al.’s (Reference Wasiak2010) breakdown of sharing action or Storga et al.’s (Reference Storga2010) ontology. It is important to note that the UDA model does not point to any specific sub-categorisations and thus does not preclude or favour one over the other.

Uncertainty perception captures both design specific (e.g. problem/solution) and wider factors (Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011; Kreye et al. Reference Kreye, Goh and Newnes2011) characterised in terms of level and nature. Although uncertainty perception as a unitary whole has been studied in the design domain (Wiltschnig et al. Reference Wiltschnig, Christensen and Ball2013) no taxonomy or decomposition of design-related uncertainty perception exists. For example, Kleinsmann & Valkenburg (Reference Kleinsmann and Valkenburg2008) highlight organisational sources of uncertainty, while others focus on social issues (Chiu et al. Reference Chiu, Hsu and Wang2006), or specific aspects of design understanding (Storga et al. Reference Storga2010). Further, no cohesive description of uncertainty perception exists in the design literature below the description currently used in the UDA model, which can be influenced by experience (Demirbaş & Demirkan Reference Demirbaş and Demirkan2003), situational factors (Kreye et al. Reference Kreye2012) and personality (Kreye Reference Kreye2016). In contrast, the management literature offers taxonomies of uncertainty as described, for example, by O’Connor & Rice (Reference O’Connor and Rice2013). Further work is needed to integrate these taxonomies in the design literature and determine the causal relationship with action selection in design. Thus, as with the core actions, the lack of a cohesive model of uncertainty perception means that it is not possible to decompose this element or its impact on design activity beyond what is described in the proposed model.

Together the elements of the UDA model support its ability to answer empirical and theoretical questions raised in the design literature e.g. how the interface between conversation and gesture jointly resolve uncertainty perception (Luck Reference Luck2013), how designers progress through a design task by combining distinct units of action in a directed sequence (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ; Cash & Gonçalves Reference Cash, Gonçalves, Christensen, Ball and Halskov2017), or how co-evolution can be connected to directed action (Lassoet al. Reference Lasso, Cash, Daalhuizen and Kreye2016; Cash & Gonçalves Reference Cash, Gonçalves, Christensen, Ball and Halskov2017). In particular, Cash & Kreye (Reference Cash and Kreye2017) begin to empirically examine the utility of the UDA model in their recent protocol studies. However, significant questions remain as elaborated in the following section.

6.2 Propositions and further work

Based on the UDA model, three propositions can be formulated that provide concrete claims for testing in, for example, subsequent empirical studies (Wacker Reference Wacker1998), and provide directions for future research. First, a central feature of the model is uncertainty perception as the driver of the three core actions: information, knowledge sharing, and representation. The proposed UDA model extends descriptions in the design literature by explicitly stating the causal relationship between uncertainty perception and the core actions. Uncertainty perception can drive action selection and combinatory progression. Specifically, uncertainty perception generally motivates and mediates progression across actions (Calantone & Rubera Reference Calantone and Rubera2012) and is linked to progression within specific instantiations of actions, such as sketching (Scrivener et al. Reference Scrivener, Ball and Tseng2000), and prototyping (Gerber & Carroll Reference Gerber and Carroll2012). Thus we propose uncertainty perception as a major driver for the dynamic interaction between the core actions and higher-level activity progression, encapsulated in Proposition 1.

Proposition 1. Uncertainty perception is a driver and antecedent of design activity, connecting progression across information, knowledge-sharing, and representation actions.

This proposition refines and extends existing descriptions in the design literature in two main ways. First, it captures the varied role of uncertainty perception as a causal driver of design activity in terms of action progression. This connects higher-level research on aggregate activity (Hult et al. Reference Hult, Ketchen and Slater2004) to studies at the action level (Christensen & Ball Reference Christensen and Ball2016a ,Reference Christensen and Ball b ). It thus formally integrates fragmented descriptions of the effect of uncertainty perception as a cause of design activity progression. Second, it points to the need for further research exploring the role of uncertainty perception in connecting the multiple levels associated with design activity (Section 2). For example, this proposition points to the connection between activity level progression and fundamental characterisations of uncertainty perception and action level response. The UDA model thus provides a key bridge between the varied formalisms of design activity and wider activity related literature in the management and innovation research domains, highlighted as a key challenge for design research by the recent works of Luo (Reference Luo2015) and Papalambros (Reference Papalambros2015).

Second, the effect of uncertainty perception on design activity can be conceptualised based on its level and nature. As described in Section 4, level of uncertainty perception describes the overall amount of the perceived lack of understanding, while nature details the perception of different uncertainty types. Together, the level and nature of uncertainty perception affect action selection and progression with respect to the core actions: information, knowledge sharing, and representation. Changes in level and nature also influence how the core actions are realised in practice (Omta & de Leeuw Reference Omta and de Leeuw1997). Further research is needed to characterise how level and nature of uncertainty perception (and changes thereof) determine action selection and across-action progression. Recent work of Cash & Gonçalves (Reference Cash, Gonçalves, Christensen, Ball and Halskov2017) shows that such examination is possible at the unitary action/cognition level conceptualised in the UDA model. It can be expected that a change in the level, nature or both of uncertainty perception may cause a designer to move from, for example, information action to knowledge-sharing action. For example, one combination of different uncertainty types may favour information action over the other two core actions captured in the UDA model. However, the exact impact of these differing changes is not clear. This is encapsulated in Proposition 2.

Proposition 2. Change in uncertainty perception, characterised by level and nature, determines activity progression, in terms of action selection and combination across information, knowledge-sharing, and representation actions.

This proposition contributes to the design literature by decomposing the effect of uncertainty perception beyond current characterisations. This allows for more fine grained understanding of designers’ reactions depending on their perception of the situation (Daalhuizen & Badke-Schaub Reference Daalhuizen and Badke-Schaub2011), and organisational setting (Kreye Reference Kreye2016). It thus allows design researchers to determine the specific effect of uncertainty perception on design activity in terms of action selection and combinatory progression.

Finally, the UDA model suggests that it is the combined effect of action selection and combinatory progression that determines overall design activity progression, and thus design outcome. This follows prior conceptualisations of design that place activity at the heart of performance, but extends them by integrating the three core actions in a single model. Specifically, prior research has highlighted the individual importance of information (Wasiak et al. Reference Wasiak2010), knowledge sharing (Kleinsmann et al. Reference Kleinsmann2012), and representation (Bilda & Gero Reference Bilda and Gero2007) on design outcome, with little or no integration between them. The UDA model offers a means of bringing together descriptions of design activity and its impact on outcome. The connection between uncertainty perception and design outcome is encapsulated in Proposition 3.

Proposition 3. The progression of uncertainty perception (level and nature) and activity determines the design outcome.

This proposition contributes to the design literature by connecting the effect of uncertainty perception and subsequent action selection/progression to design outcome. This integrates antecedent, behaviour, and consequence elements in a single model in the design context (Oliver Reference Oliver1980; Miltenberger Reference Miltenberger2011). This complements and extends prior works that have focused on the antecedent/behaviour relationship between uncertainty perception and specific instantiations of the different core actions (Scrivener et al. Reference Scrivener, Ball and Tseng2000). Further, the focused nature of prior studies of uncertainty perception and action in the design context mean that direct links between overall activity progression and overall outcome have been difficult to characterise. Thus the UDA model enables researchers to understand complex motivation driven activities in terms of generic units, the combination of which underpin overall activity progression. In this way researchers are better able to target support for these core actions or decompose overall activity progression patterns in order to better understand where and when design support is needed.

7 Conclusion

This paper aimed to propose a cohesive model of design activity as a basis for unifying the design activity literature and as a foundation for future theory building in this area. This connects the diverse empirical and theoretical works in the design domain already exploring many aspects of design activity; and seeks to formalise a description of design activity as a goal directed system where cognition, behaviour, and motivation are integrated, with respect to the ‘bringing-into-being’ of a design artefact. Based on an analytical conceptual approach the Uncertainty Driven Action (UDA) model was proposed. This combines three core actions (information action, knowledge-sharing action, and representation action), and their associated cognitive processes via the designers’ uncertainty perception, to explain the progression of overall design activity. The theoretical basis of the model was described and its practical relevance discussed with respect to the wider design literature. The model brings together a wide range of prior descriptions of design activity via the core actions, and connects these into a dynamic system via cognition and uncertainty perception. This provides a cohesive understanding of design activity as built on the sequential combination of fundamental actions.

The UDA model delivers three main contributions to design research. First, it offers a unifying description of design activity synthesising the core actions and uncertainty perception in a single model, with a consistent link to cognition in line with underlying Activity Theory. The three core actions further provide a platform for developing greater commonality in research on design activity, comparing insights from existing formalisms, and directing future typologies on the sub-types defining each of the core actions. Second, the proposed UDA model offers a causal explanation of design activity progression in terms of action selection and sequential combination, with respect to the common mechanism of uncertainty perception. This completes the antecedent–behaviour–consequence requirements lacking in prior formalisms and describes a ‘complete’ framework of design activity. Third, this research proposes important guidelines and directions for future research that will further theory development in the design literature.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the reviewers for providing detailed and constructive input helping to develop the final version of this article.

References

Ahmed, S. & Storga, M. 2009 Merged ontology for engineering design: contrasting empirical and theoretical approaches to develop engineering ontologies. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 23 (4), 391407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, M. M., Thorp Hansen, C. & Cash, P. 2015 Conceptual Design: Interpretations, Mindset, and Models. Springer.Google Scholar
Aurisicchio, M., Bracewell, R. & Wallace, K. 2006 Evaluation of DRed a way of capturing and structuring engineering design processes. In NordDesign, The Design Society, Reykjavik, Iceland.Google Scholar
Aurisicchio, M., Bracewell, R. & Wallace, K. 2010 Understanding how the information requests of aerospace engineering designers influence information-seeking behaviour. Journal of Engineering Design 21 (6), 707730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aurisicchio, M., Bracewell, R. & Wallace, K. M. 2013 Characterising the information requests of aerospace engineering designers. Research in Engineering Design 24 (1), 4363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ball, L. J. et al. 1997 Problem-solving strategies and expertise in engineering design. Thinking and Reasoning 3 (4), 247270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ball, L. J. & Christensen, B. T. 2009 Analogical reasoning and mental simulation in design: two strategies linked to uncertainty resolution. Design Studies 30 (2), 169186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ball, L. J., Onarheim, B. & Christensen, B. T. 2010 Design requirements, epistemic uncertainty and solution development strategies in software design. Design Studies 31 (6), 567589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K. & Li, N. 2013 The theory of purposeful work behavior: the role of personality, job characteristics, and experienced meaningfulness. Academy of Management Review 38 (1), 132153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedny, G. Z. & Harris, S. R. 2005 The systemic-structural theory of activity: applications to the study of human work. Mind, Culture, and Activity 12 (2), 128147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedny, G. Z. & Karwowski, W. 2004 Activity theory as a basis for the study of work. Ergonomics 47 (2), 134153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beheshti, R. 1993 Design decisions and uncertainty. Design Studies 14 (1), 8595.Google Scholar
Belkin, N. J., Oddy, R. N. & Brooks, H. M. 1982 ASK for information retrieval: Part I. Background and theory. Journal of Documentation 38 (2), 6171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beylier, C. et al. 2009 A collaboration-centred approach to manage engineering knowledge: a case study of an engineering SME. Journal of Engineering Design 20 (6), 523542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biazzo, S. 2009 Flexibility, structuration, and simultaneity in new product development. Journal of Product Innovation Management 26 (3), 336353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilda, Z. & Gero, J. S. 2007 The impact of working memory limitations on the design process during conceptualization. Design Studies 28 (4), 343367.Google Scholar
Blandin, J. S. & Brown, W. B. 1977 Uncertainty and management’s search for information. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management 24 (4), 114119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borlund, P. 2003 The IIR evaluation model: a framework for evaluation of interactive information retrieval systems. Information Research 8 (3), 811.Google Scholar
Briggs, R. O. 2006 On theory-driven design and deployment of collaboration systems. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 64 (7), 573582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brun, J., Le Masson, P. & Weil, B. 2016 Designing with sketches: the generative effects of knowledge preordering. Design Science 2(Goldschmidt 1991), e13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bstieler, L. 2005 The moderating effect of environmental uncertainty on new product development and time efficiency*. Journal of Product Innovation Management 22 (3), 267284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busby, J. S. 2001 Error and distributed cognition in design. Design Studies 22 (3), 233254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calantone, R. & Rubera, G. 2012 When should RD&E and marketing collaborate? the moderating role of exploration-exploitation and environmental uncertainty. Journal of Product Innovation Management 29 (1), 144157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cardella, M. E., Atman, C. J. & Adams, R. S. 2006 Mapping between design activities and external representations for engineering student designers. Design Studies 27 (1), 524.Google Scholar
Cash, P. & Gonçalves, M. 2017 Information-triggered co-evolution: a combined process perspective. In Analysing Design Thinking: Studies of Cross-Cultural Co-Creation (ed. Christensen, B. T., Ball, L. J. & Halskov, K.), pp. 501520. CRC Press.Google Scholar
Cash, P., Hicks, B. & Culley, S. 2015 Activity Theory as a means for multi-scale analysis of the engineering design process: a protocol study of design in practice. Design Studies 38 (May), 132.Google Scholar
Cash, P., Hicks, B. & Culley, S. et al. 2015 A foundational observation method for studying design situations. Journal of Engineering Design 26 (7–9), 187219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cash, P. & Kreye, M. E. 2017 Exploring uncertainty perception as a driver of design activity. Design Studies (in press).Google Scholar
Cash, P. & Maier, A. 2016 Prototyping with your hands: the many roles of gesture in the communication of design concepts. Journal of Engineering Design 27 (1–3), 118145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cash, P. J., Hicks, B. J. & Culley, S. J. 2013 A comparison of designer activity using core design situations in the laboratory and practice. Design Studies 34 (5), 575611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, J., Paletz, S. B. F. & Schunn, C. D. 2012 Analogy as a strategy for supporting complex problem solving under uncertainty. Memory and Cognition 40, 13521365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cave, P. R. & Noble, C. E. I. 1986 Engineering design data management. In 1st International Conference on Engineering Management, Theory and Applications, Swansea, UK.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-M., Hsu, M.-H. & Wang, E. T. G. 2006 Understanding knowledge sharing in virtual communities: an integration of social capital and social cognitive theories. Decision Support Systems 42 (3), 18721888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, B. T. & Ball, L. J. 2016a Creative analogy use in a heterogeneous design team: the pervasive role of background domain knowledge. Design Studies 46, 3858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, B. T. & Ball, L. J. 2016b Fluctuating epistemic uncertainty in a design team as a metacognitive driver for creative cognitive processes. In DTRS11: Design Thinking Research Symposium 2016, pp. 117. CRC Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Brennan, S. E. 1991 Grounding in communication. In Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition, pp. 127149. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Court, A. W. 1997 The relationship between information and personal knowledge in new product development. International Journal of Information Management 17 (2), 123138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crilly, N. 2015 Fixation and creativity in concept development: the attitudes and practices of expert designers. Design Studies 38 (C), 5491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, N. 2007 Forty years of design research. Design Studies 28 (1), 14.Google Scholar
Daalhuizen, J. & Badke-Schaub, P. 2011 The use of methods by advanced beginner and expert industrial designers in non-routine situations: a quasi-experiment. International Journal of Product Development 15 (1/2/3), 54.Google Scholar
Daft, R. L. & Lengel, R. H. 1986 Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science 32 (5), 554571.Google Scholar
Daft, R. L., Lengel, R. H. & Trevino, L. K. 1987 Message equivocality, media selection, and manager performance: implications for information systems. MIS Quarterly 11 (3), 355366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daley, J. 1982 Design creativity and the understanding of objects. Design Studies 3 (3), 133137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, G. B. 2006 Rethinking risk attitude: aspiration as pure risk. Theory and Decision 61 (2), 159190.Google Scholar
Deckers, E. et al. 2012 Designing for perceptual crossing: applying and evaluating design notions. International Journal of Design 6 (3), 4155.Google Scholar
Deken, F. et al. 2012 Tapping into past design experiences: knowledge sharing and creation during novice-expert design consultations. Research in Engineering Design 23 (3), 203218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demirbaş, O. O. & Demirkan, H. 2003 Focus on architectural design process through learning styles. Design Studies 24 (5), 437456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dong, A. 2005 The latent semantic approach to studying design team communication. Design Studies 26 (5), 445461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dong, A., Kleinsmann, M. S. & Deken, F. 2013 Investigating design cognition in the construction and enactment of team mental models. Design Studies 34 (1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. & Cross, N. 2001 Creativity in the design process: co-evolution of problem-solution. Design Studies 22 (5), 425437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. & Dijkhuis, J. 1995 Comparing paradigms for describing design activity. Design Studies 16 (2), 261274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. & Vermaas, P. E. 2005 John Gero’s function-behaviour-structure model of designing: a critical analysis. Research in Engineering Design 16 (1–2), 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsberg, D. 2001 Risk, Ambiguity and Decision Studies in Psychology. Routledge.Google Scholar
Engeström, Y. 2000 Activity theory as a framework for analyzing and redesigning work. Ergonomics 43 (7), 960974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eris, O. 2002 Perceiving, Comprehending and Measuring Design Activity Through the Questions Asked While Designing. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Eris, O., Martelaro, N. & Badke-Schaub, P. 2014 A comparative analysis of multimodal communication during design sketching in co-located and distributed environments. Design Studies 35 (6), 559592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. T. 2008 Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59 (1), 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. & Stanovich, K. E. 2013 Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8 (3), 223241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairlie-Clarke, T. & Muller, M. 2003 An activity model of the product development process. Journal of Engineering Design 14 (3), 247272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fidel, R. & Green, M. 2004 The many faces of accessibility: engineers’ perception of information sources. Information Processing and Management 40 (3), 563581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B. 1981 Design-based studies: an action-based “form of knowledge” for thinking, reasoning and operating. Design Studies 2 (1), 3340.Google Scholar
Francis, J. J. et al. 2009 Evidence-based selection of theories for designing behaviour change interventions: using methods based on theoretical construct domains to understand clinicians’ blood transfusion behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology 14 (4), 625646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, R. 2005 Uses of agent-based modeling in innovation/new product development research. Journal of Product Innovation Management 22 (5), 380398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ge, C. P. & Wang, B. 2007 An activity-based modelling approach for assessing the key stakeholders’ corporation in the eco-conscious design of electronic products. Journal of Engineering Design 18 (1), 5571.Google Scholar
Gerber, E. & Carroll, M. 2012 The psychological experience of prototyping. Design Studies 33 (1), 6484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J. S. 1990 Design prototypes: a knowledge representation schema for design. AI Magazine 11, 26.Google Scholar
Gero, J. S. & Kannengiesser, U. 2004 The situated function–behaviour–structure framework. Design Studies 25 (4), 373391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonçalves, M., Cardoso, C. & Badke-Schaub, P. 2016 Inspiration choices that matter: the selection of external stimuli during ideation. Design Science 2 (November), e10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonnet, S., Henning, G. & Leone, H. 2007 A model for capturing and representing the engineering design process. Expert Systems with Applications 33 (4), 881902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gursoy, B. & Ozkar, M. 2015 Visualizing making: shapes, materials, and actions. Design Studies 41, 2950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatchuel, A. & Weil, B. 2002 CK theory. In Proceedings of the Herbert Simon International conference on Design Sciences. Lyon, France, pp. 122. Citeseer.Google Scholar
Hatchuel, A. & Weil, B. 2003 A new approach of innovative design: an introduction to C-K theory. In Proceedings of ICED 03, the 14th International Conference on Engineering Design, Stockholm. ICED 03 International Conference on Engineering Design. Stockholm, Sweden. The Design Society.Google Scholar
Hazelrigg, G. A. 1998 A framework for decision-based engineering design. Journal of Mechanical Design 120, 5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heavey, C. & Simsek, Z. 2013 Top management compositional effects on corporate entrepreneurship: the moderating role of perceived technological uncertainty. Journal of Product Innovation Management 30 (5), 837855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertz, K. 1992 A coherent description of the process of design. Design Studies 13 (4), 393410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horvath, I. 2004 A treatise on order in engineering design research. Research in Engineering Design 15, 155181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hult, G. T. M., Ketchen, D. J. & Slater, S. F. 2004 Information processing, knowledge development, and strategic supply chain performance. Academy of Management Journal 47 (2), 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humayun, M. & Gang, C. 2013 An empirical study on improving shared understanding of requirements in GSD. International Journal of Software Engineering and Its Applications 7 (1), 7992.Google Scholar
Hurley, R. J., Kosenko, K. A. & Brashers, D. 2011 Uncertain terms: message features of online cancer news. Communication Monographs 78 (3), 370390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, W. 1993 Design as problem-solving? or: here is the solution -what was the problem? Design Studies 14 (2), 157170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kan, J. W. T., Gero, J. S. & Tang, H. H. 2011 Measuring cognitive design activity changes during an industry team brainstorming session. In Design Computing and Cognition ’10 (ed. Gero, J. S.), pp. 621640. Springer.Google Scholar
Kanstrup, A. M. 2014 Design concepts for digital diabetes practice: design to explore, share, and camoufage chronic illness. International Journal of Design 8 (3), 4960.Google Scholar
Kavakli, M. & Gero, J. S. 2001 Sketching as mental imagery processing. Design Studies 22 (4), 347364.Google Scholar
Kim, K. & Lee, K. 2016 Collaborative product design processes of industrial design and engineering design in consumer product companies. Design Studies 46, 226260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinsmann, M. et al. 2012 Development of design collaboration skills. Journal of Engineering Design 23 (7), 485506.Google Scholar
Kleinsmann, M. & Valkenburg, R. 2008 Barriers and enablers for creating shared understanding in co-design projects. Design Studies 29 (4), 369386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. et al. 1984 Individual differences in mental imagery ability: a computational analysis. Cognition 18 (1–3), 195243.Google Scholar
Kreye, M. E. et al. 2012 Approaches of displaying information to assist decisions under uncertainty. Omega - International Journal of Management Science 40 (6), 682692.Google Scholar
Kreye, M. E. 2016 Uncertainty and Behaviour: Perceptions, Decisions and Actions in Business. Gower Publishing, Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreye, M. E., Goh, Y. M. & Newnes, L. B. 2011 Manifestation of uncertainty – a classification. In ICED’11 – International Conference on Engineering Design, Copenhagen, Denmark. The Design Society.Google Scholar
Kulkarni, S., Summers, J. D., Vargas-Hernandez, N. & Shah, J. J. 2000 Evaluation of collaborative sketching (C-Sketch) as an idea generation technique for engineering design. Journal of Creative Behaviour 35 (3), 168198.Google Scholar
Lamarra, N. & Dunphy, J. 1998 Interactive sharable environment for collaborative spacecraft design. In Aerospace Conference, 1998. Proceedings, vol. 2, pp. 487496. IEEE.Google Scholar
Lasso, S., Cash, P., Daalhuizen, J. & Kreye, M. E. 2016 A model of designing as the intersection between uncertainty perception, information processing, and coevolution. In Design 2016, pp. 301310. The Design Society.Google Scholar
Leont’ev, A. N.1981 Problems of the Development of the Mind. Progress.Google Scholar
Liikkanen, L. A. & Perttula, M. 2010 Inspiring design idea generation: insights from a memory-search perspective. Journal of Engineering Design 21 (5), 545560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y. T. 1996 Is designing one search or two? A model of design thinking involving symbolism and connectionism. Design Studies 17 (4), 435449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Love, T. 2000 Philosophy of design: a meta-theoretical structure for design theory. Design Studies 21 (3), 293313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Love, T. 2002 Constructing a coherent cross-disciplinary body of theory about designing and designs: some philosophical issues. Design Studies 23 (3), 345361.Google Scholar
Luck, R. 2013 Articulating (mis)understanding across design discipline interfaces at a design team meeting. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 27 (2), 155166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, J. 2015 The united innovation process: integrating science, design, and entrepreneurship as sub-processes. Design Science 1 (October 2015), e2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macleod, I. A., McGregor, D. R. & Hutton, G. H. 1994 Accessing of information for engineering design. Design Studies 15 (3), 260269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maier, J. F., Eckert, C. M. & Clarkson, P. J. 2017 Model granularity in engineering design – concepts and framework. Design Science 3 (e-1), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, L. M. 2001 Toward a theory of knowledge reuse: types of knowledge reuse situations and factors in reuse success. Journal of Management Information Systems 18 (1), 5793.Google Scholar
Maznevski, M. L. & Chudoba, K. M. 2000 Bridging space over time: global virtual team dynamics and effectiveness. Organization Science 11 (5), 473492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAlpine, H., Cash, P. & Hicks, B. 2017 The role of logbooks as mediators of engineering design work. Design Studies 48 (January), 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, I. P. et al. 2006 New product development as a complex adaptive system of decisions. Journal of Product Innovation Management 23 (5), 437456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonnell, J. 1997 Descriptive models for interpreting design. Design Studies 18 (4), 457473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahon, C., Lowe, A. & Culley, S. 2004 Knowledge management in engineering design: personalization and codification. Journal of Engineering Design 15 (4), 307325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medland, A. J. 1992 Forms of communications observed during the study of design activities in industry. Journal of Engineering Design 3 (3), 243253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miltenberger, R. 2011 Behavior Modification: Principles and Procedures. Wadsworth Publishing.Google Scholar
Moscoso, P. G. 2007 A design-oriented framework for modelling production management systems. Journal of Engineering Design 18 (6), 599616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagai, Y. & Gero, J. S. 2012 Design creativity. Journal of Engineering Design 23 (4), 237239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Connor, G. C. & Rice, M. P. 2013 A comprehensive model of uncertainty associated with radical innovation. Journal of Product Innovation Management 30, 218.Google Scholar
Oh, S., Oh, J. S. & Shah, C. 2009 The use of information sources by internet users in answering questions. Proceedings of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 45 (1), 113.Google Scholar
Oliver, R. L. 1980 A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions. Journal of Marketing Research 17 (4), 460469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omta, S. W. F. & de Leeuw, A. C. J. 1997 Management control, uncertainty, and performance in biomedical research in universities, institutes and companies. Journal of Engineering and Technology Management 14 (3–4), 223257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ostergaard, K. J. & Summers, J. D. 2009 Development of a systematic classification and taxonomy of collaborative design activities. Journal of Engineering Design 20 (1), 5781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oxman, R. 1990 Prior knowledge in design: a dynamic knowledge-based model of design and creativity. Design Studies 11 (1), 1728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oxman, R. 1997 Design by re-representation: a model of visual reasoning in design. Design Studies 18 (4), 329347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pahl, G. & Beitz, W. 1996 Engineering Design: A Systematic Approach. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paletz, S. B. F., Chan, J. & Schunn, C. D. 2017 The dynamics of micro-conflicts and uncertainty in successful and unsuccessful design teams. Design Studies 50, 3969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papalambros, P. Y. 2015 Design science: why, what and how. Design Science 1, e1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pask, G. 1975 Conversation, Cognition and Learning. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Peeters, M. A. G. et al. 2007 The development of a design behaviour questionnaire for multidisciplinary teams. Design Studies 28 (6), 623643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, D. S., Karahanna, E. & Rowe, F. 2006 Development of shared understanding between the Chief Information officer and top management team in U.S. and French Organizations: a cross-cultural comparison. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management 53 (2), 191206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pugh, S. 1989 Knowledge-based systems in the design activity. Design Studies 10 (4), 219227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puttre, M. 1991 Product data management. Mechanical Engineering 113 (10), 8183.Google Scholar
Robinson, M. A. 2010a An empirical analysis of engineers’ information behaviours. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 61 (4), 640658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, M. A. 2010b Work sampling: methodological advances and new applications. Human Factors and Ergonomics in Manufacturing and Service Industries 20 (1), 4260.Google Scholar
Rouibah, K. & Caskey, K. 2003 A workflow system for the management of inter-company collaborative engineering processes. Journal of Engineering Design 14 (3), 273293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanders, E. B. N. & Stappers, P. J. 2014 Probes, toolkits and prototypes: three approaches to making in codesigning. CoDesign 10 (1), 514.Google Scholar
Sauder, J. & Jin, Y. 2016 A qualitative study of collaborative stimulation in group design thinking. Design Science 2 (April), e4.Google Scholar
Scaife, M. & Rogers, Y. 1996 External cognition: how do graphical representations work? International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 45 (2), 185213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schön, D. A. & Wiggins, G. 1992 Kinds of seeing and their functions in designing. Design Studies 13 (2), 135156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schraw, G. & Dennison, R. S. 1994 Assessing metacognitive awareness. Contemporary Educational Psychology 19 (4), 460475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scrivener, S. A. R., Ball, L. J. & Tseng, W. 2000 Uncertainty and sketching behaviour. Design Studies 21 (5), 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shull, F. et al. 2004 Knowledge-sharing issues in experimental software engineering. Empirical Software Engineering 9 (1), 111137.Google Scholar
Sim, S. K. & Duffy, A. H. B. 2003 Towards an ontology of generic engineering design activities. Research in Engineering Design 14 (4), 200223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, V. & Gu, N. 2012 Towards an integrated generative design framework. Design Studies 33 (2), 185207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, M., Van Der Bij, H. & Weggeman, M. 2005 Determinants of the level of knowledge application: a knowledge-based and information-processing perspective. Journal of Product Innovation Management 22 (5), 430444.Google Scholar
Stempfle, J. & Badke-Schaub, P. 2002 Thinking in design teams – an analysis of team communication. Design Studies 23 (5), 473496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storga, M. et al. 2010 The design ontology: foundation for the design knowledge exchange and management. Journal of Engineering Design 21 (4), 427454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suh, N. P. 1998 Axiomatic design theory for systems. Research in Engineering Design 10 (4), 189209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, L. et al. 2014 Designers’ perception during sketching: an examination of creative segment theory using eye movements. Design Studies 35 (6), 593613.Google Scholar
Suss, S. & Thomson, V. 2012 Optimal design processes under uncertainty and reciprocal dependency. Journal of Engineering Design 23 (10–11), 826848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suwa, M., Purcell, T. & Gero, J. S. 1998 Macroscopic analysis of design processes based on a scheme for coding designers’ cognitive actions. Design Studies 19 (4), 455483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taura, T. et al. 2012 Constructive simulation of creative concept generation process in design: a research method for difficult-to-observe design-thinking processes. Journal of Engineering Design 23 (4), 297321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thielman, J. & Ge, P. 2006 Applying axiomatic design theory to the evaluation and optimization of large-scale engineering systems. Journal of Engineering Design 17 (1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracey, M. W. & Hutchinson, A. 2016 Uncertainty, reflection, and designer identity development. Design Studies 42, 86109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. 1974 Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185 (4157), 11241131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vajna, S. et al. 2005 The autogenetic design theory: an evolutionary view of the design process. Journal of Engineering Design 16 (4), 423440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Lugt, R. 2005 How sketching can affect the idea generation process in design group meetings. Design Studies 26 (2), 101112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, W. 2009 Design: one, but in different forms. Design Studies 30 (3), 187223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viswanathan, V. et al. 2014 A study on the role of physical models in the mitigation of design fixation. Journal of Engineering Design 25 (1–3), 2543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wacker, J. G. 1998 A definition of theory: research guidelines for different theory-building research methods in operations management. Journal of Operations Management 16 (4), 361385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, W. et al. 2013 Creation dependencies of evolutionary artefact and design process knowledge. Journal of Engineering Design 24 (9), 681710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wasiak, J. et al. 2010 Understanding engineering email: the development of a taxonomy for identifying and classifying engineering work. Research in Engineering Design 21 (1), 4364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitefield, A. & Warren, C. 1989 A blackboard framework for modelling designers’ behaviour. Design Studies 10 (3), 179187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wild, P. J. et al. 2010 A diary study of information needs and document usage in the engineering domain. Design Studies 31 (1), 4673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. 2002 Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9 (4), 625636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D. 1999 Models in information behaviour research. Journal of Documentation 55 (3), 249270.Google Scholar
Wiltschnig, S., Christensen, B. T. & Ball, L. J. 2013 Collaborative problem-solution co-evolution in creative design. Design Studies 34 (5), 515542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, D. & Clarkson, J. 2005 Models of designing. In Design Process Improvement, pp. 3459. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, D. C., Grebici, K. & Clarkson, P. J. 2011 Modelling the evolution of uncertainty levels during design. International Journal on Interactive Design and Manufacturing 5 (3), 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, M. C. 2009 Observations on concept generation and sketching in engineering design. Research in Engineering Design 20 (1), 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yilmaz, S. et al. How do designers generate new ideas? Design heuristics across two disciplines. Design Science 1 (2015), e4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, B. Y. et al. 2016 Human behavior and domain knowledge in parameter design of complex systems. Design Studies 45, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Human activity composed of multiple aggregate levels built on a generic, unitary action/cognition foundation.

Figure 1

Table 1. Overview of formalisms associated with design activity, categorised with respect to their related core actions

Figure 2

Figure 2. The UDA model linking internal metacognitive uncertainty perception (red) and cognitive processing (grey), and externally enacted information, knowledge-sharing, and representation actions (black). As a whole, the UDA model composes one ‘building block’ denoted by Note 2 in Figure 1.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Possible progressions allowed by the UDA model, and an example sequence of actions linked by evolving uncertainty perception (UP$_{\text{n}}$). Here each action cycle in the sequence is a single iteration of the UDA model and together they form the foundation for activity as explained in Figure 1.