Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-02T05:25:00.035Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reflections on the concept of musical development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 March 2022

Graham F. Welch*
Affiliation:
UCL Institute of Education, University College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author. Email: graham.welch@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

‘The Sequence of Musical Development’ by Swanwick and Tillman was published in the British Journal of Music Education (BJME) in 1986. This year marks its 35th anniversary and provides an occasion to look back on the article’s content and legacy. It is also an opportunity to reflect on the antecedents for the article’s underlying concepts, as well as how our understanding of children and young people’s musical behaviours and development has evolved. Alternate and more nuanced perspectives, both available at the time and since, draw on an expanding, diverse, multidisciplinary research base. These enable us to have a better grasp of the strengths of the original, as well as what continues to need investigation.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

This invited article has been prompted by the 35th anniversary of the ‘The Sequence of Musical Development’, an article by Keith Swanwick and June Tillman that was published in the British Journal of Music Education (BJME) in 1986. At that time, the journal was relatively new, having been first published 2 years earlier in 1984. The Swanwick and Tillman article is considered to be a landmark study, being one of the first major research-based contributions that sought to map the nature of musical behaviour and development in children, a field which has its roots in the pioneering studies of 500 young children by Helmut Moog (Reference MOOG1968, Reference MOOG1976a), and which has expanded considerably since (cf Barrett & Welch Reference BARRETT, ABAD, WELCH, Barrett and Welch2022).

A recent scoping review, for example, of publications in the field of children’s musical behaviour and development over the past two decades identified 1,308 articles in 16 key international journals, of which n = 507 articles covered the age range 0–8 years and n = 901 articles the ages 9–14 years (Barrett, Abad & Welch, Reference BARRETT, ABAD, WELCH, Barrett and Welch2022). Within the younger age phase, n = 231 articles had an explicit research focus. These were roughly evenly split in their methodological biases between quantitative (cf Swanwick & Tillman) and qualitative approaches, with over half of the latter being case studies that explored particular instances of musical behaviour of individuals in greater depth.

This diversity of research methodologies was reflected in the contents of that same BJME 1986 issue in which two of the articles chosen by the editorial team shared a concern with aspects of children’s musical development. These related to examples of children’s invented songs as illustrations of musical language development (Davies, Reference DAVIES1986Footnote 1) and observed patterns in young children’s song singing development (Welch, Reference WELCH1986Footnote 2). Furthermore, Davies (Reference DAVIES1992) suggested that vocal and instrumental music making ‘may reveal different aspects of children’s musical understanding’ (p. 2) and that invented songs are an essential feature of young children’s sense-making of their world (p. 358) – a point that has been echoed subsequently in the extensive case study reports of children’s song-making by Barrett (cf 2019). As such, these two BJME 1986 singing-related reports provide a counterpoint to the landmark ‘sequence’ article on children’s instrumental improvisation.

‘Stages’ of development

One interpretation of Swanwick and Tillman’s article is that it was characteristic of its time – the 60s, 70s and early 80s – when there was a trend in pedagogical and sociocultural theories, often grounded in psychology, that children’s learning could be promoted more effectively if teaching could be matched somehow to the ‘natural’ development of the child (cf Zimmerman, 1971/2011, Reference ZIMMERMAN2003). The concept of ‘readiness’ was prevalent (such as in Goal 1 of the US Six National Education Goals of 1990, Readiness for School, Katz, Reference KATZ1991 and Goal 4 of the United Nations (2015) Sustainable Development GoalsFootnote 3), implying that children are more able to learn if pedagogical content were appropriate to their ‘stage’ of development, a concern that continues today (e.g., see review by Sabol & Planta, Reference SABOL, PLANTA, Votruba-Drzal and Dearing2017; Pan et al., Reference PAN, TRANG, LOVE and TEMPLIN2019). There was also a growing awareness that age and experience were important factors in children’s observed musical behaviour, such as exampled by a study of 3-year-olds who tended to reproduce three target songs in the original learned key, whereas slightly older children (aged 5 and 6 years) were less accurate in terms of their absolute pitch imitation, but more able to abstract the songs’ key features and reproduce these more in terms of contour and interval (Sergeant & Roche, Reference SERGEANT and ROCHE1973).

This notion of discrete phases of development (‘stages’) was a likely outgrowth of the widespread psychological and educational interest arising from the child development studies of the Swiss psychologist, Jean Piaget (d. 1980). The prime focus for Piaget’s research and writing was on how the child makes sense of their world, suggesting that this evolves through a series of developmental ‘stages’ from early childhood through to adulthood (e.g., Piaget, Reference PIAGET and Cook1954). Development-related studies in music also became more evidenced around this time, such as reviewed in the major music psychology monographs authored by Sloboda (Reference SLOBODA1985) and – in particular – Hargreaves (Reference HARGREAVES1986), as well as in the edited collection by Peery, Peery and Draper (Reference PEERY, PEERY and DRAPER1987).

Subsequently, aspects of the UK-based doctoral research that underpinned the Swanwick and Tillman BJME article (Tillman, Reference TILLMAN1987) were replicated in a related doctoral study in Cyprus (Stavrides, Reference STAVRIDES1995). According to Swanwick (Reference SWANWICK1991; Reference SWANWICK, Colwell and Webster2011), the Cypriot data supported the components of the theorised general developmental framework in the original article, albeit with a different methodology and in a different cultural setting. Notwithstanding these related findings, it seems important to recognise that, rather than being ‘The Sequence of Musical Development’ as in the original title’s use of the definite article, this widely cited study is actually ‘A Sequence of Musical Development’, being one of several possible sequences and – in this case – related to a particular form of musicking – composing with simple tuned and untuned percussion instruments by children in a single school, recorded by a single researcher (as noted by Tillman, Reference TILLMAN1987, vol2:57/103). The diversity of musics in the world and their cultural specificity and distinctiveness – such as exampled in the five major distinctive forms of Chinese traditional musicFootnote 4 (Yang, Reference YANG2011) – imply that there may be many different sequences of musical development related to context, task, and whatever counts as local and regional musical behaviour. This was a possibility acknowledged by Tillman in her doctoral thesis (Reference TILLMAN1987, vol. 2: 78–79), which was completed 1 year after the BJME article and reflected in her careful choice of words in the actual thesis title: ‘Towards a Model of the Development…’.

Alternative perspectives on development

Other authors have adopted different conceptual perspectives related to how children make sense of music. According to Hargreaves and Lamont (Reference HARGREAVES and LAMONT2017), such theories can be clustered into six general theoretical approaches, which themselves can be subdivided into 10 distinct groups in the music development literature. These are based on developmental stages (e.g., Swanwick & Tillman, Reference SWANWICK and TILLMAN1986; Hargreaves & Galton, Reference HARGREAVES, GALTON, Reimer and Smith1992), learning and cognition (e.g., Serafine, Reference SERAFINE1988; Gordon, Reference GORDON1979, Reference GORDON1997), symbol systems (e.g., Bamberger, Reference BAMBERGER1991, drawing on the research with Howard Gardner and colleagues in Project Zero – cf Perkins, Reference PERKINS1974), music theory (e.g., see recent summary by Ockelford & Welch, Reference OCKELFORD and WELCH2021), social factors (e.g., Welch, Reference WELCH2007; Welch & Ockelford, Reference WELCH, OCKELFORD, Hallam, Cross and Thaut2016) and neuroscientific studies (e.g., Costa-Giomi, Reference COSTA-GIOMI2015; Ford Thompson & Schlaug, Reference FORD THOMPSON and Schlaug2015). The last of these include applied neuroscientific studies, such as by Verney (Reference VERNEY2013) linking reading, dyslexia and rhythmic perception, and Pocwierz-Marciniak and Harciarek’s (Reference POCWIERZ-MARCINIAK and HARCIAREK2021) literature review concerning early auditory experience and the mother’s voice.

Furthermore, Davies (Reference DAVIES1992) cautions against the impression created by a spiral’s visual portrayal of linear progression, reporting that several young children in her study were demonstrating several different types of musical thinking concurrently in terms of the spiral’s elements – a possibility that had been noted earlier by Tillman (Reference TILLMAN1987, vol. 2: 105). This potential weakness in a spiral conception of development has also been flagged in alternate models of curriculum design because ‘learning is not always linear’ (Ireland & Mouthaan, Reference IRELAND and MOUTHAAN2020: 11). Additionally, with regard to age, Davies noted that her data analyses suggested that young children at pre-school level, despite their age, were able to demonstrate ‘an implicit understanding of music’s structural relationships and a sense of form’ (Reference DAVIES1992: 382). This particular finding is echoed in recent example case study evidence from Australia which reports that young children ‘are likely to arrive at formal schooling [already] with a rich repertoire of music, [and] a capacity to engage in embodied musical experience’ (Barrett & Welch, Reference BARRETT and WELCH2021: 1239).

Geneses for the BJME article?

The geneses for Swanwick and Tillman’s (Reference SWANWICK and TILLMAN1986) article were likely to be various. These included Tillman’s soon-to-be-completed doctoral studies, based on her empirical investigations of children’s instrumental-based music making in her Primary school in South West London (Tillman, Reference TILLMAN1987, vol2). On a personal note, in writing this reflective article, I am reminded that Tilman’s (Reference TILLMAN1987) doctoral research was undertaken just two miles away from the three Primary schools in my own (1983) South West London doctoral fieldwork – albeit unknown to each other at the time – and reported in the same BJME 1986 issue, as mentioned earlier. Our child participants shared a common inner-city background representing a wide variety of ethnic backgrounds and maternal language groups, living and schooled in areas of relative socio-economic disadvantage, but with pockets of relative affluence.

Within her literature review, the thesis referenced a recent book on the nature of aesthetic development in the arts by Ross (Reference ROSS1984), as well as reports of a pioneering large-scale study of young children’s singing by Moog (Reference MOOG1976a; Reference MOOG1976b), which had drawn on Moog’s own 1963 thesis. In addition, Swanwick – Tillman’s doctoral supervisor – had recently returned from a visit to New York where he had been impressed by the conceptualisation and realisation of the Manhattanville Music Curriculum Project (MMCP), which was itself based on a spiral concept of curriculum development as proposed by Bruner (Reference BRUNER1960).

The MMCP had been conceived in the mid-1960s as a major innovative pedagogical project that was focused on the design of an alternate music curriculum for Grades K-12 (Primary to High school) using ‘discovery approaches’ and ‘a meaningful sequence of musical concepts’ (Moon & Humphreys, Reference MOON and HUMPHREYS2010: 76). The MMCP was seen more as a music education process – a way of making music, of acting like a musician – than a prescribed curriculum, designed to draw on music’s building blocks of sound, such as rhythm, pulse, metre, duration, pitch, timbre, volume, form, texture, expression and style (Fisher, Reference FISHER1968). These were subsequently refined in a final publication (Thomas, Reference THOMAS1970) as a music curriculum based on five core elements: dynamics, timbre, form, rhythm and pitch, to be used in combination and not in isolation from one another, and repeatedly revisited by students at ‘increasingly sophisticated levels’ (Moon & Humphreys, Reference MOON and HUMPHREYS2010: 80), often in the form of musical problem-solving. The influence of Bruner’s spiral curriculum concept was explicit, with the process of music education being founded on students’ musical compositions, and with their creativity being enriched through encounters with how sonic materials were used by established composers, including musical idioms from the 20th century.

Prior to the 1986 BJME article which echoes MMCP elements and its emphasis on creativity, Swanwick had earlier written about his ideas on the essential ingredients for music education (Reference SWANWICK1979) which included composition as well as performance. Subsequently, he reported that he was working on understanding the role of play in musical learning by drawing on Piaget’s concepts of mastery, imitation and imaginative play (Piaget, Reference PIAGET1952, Reference PIAGET, Gattengno and Hodgson1962; Swanwick, Reference SWANWICK1991) – elements which are also common to MMCP and which underpin the nature of the empirical data that form the basis for Swanwick and Tillman’s (Reference SWANWICK and TILLMAN1986) conceptualisation of creative musical development. The significance of play also surfaces in Swanwick’s (1994) view of the importance of improvisation in instrumental teaching.

Around that time, I can remember sitting in a seminar at the Institute of Education in London and being particularly intrigued by the notion of a spiral curriculum – also termed a helix (e.g., Swanwick, Reference SWANWICK, Colwell and Webster2011); Swanwick was sharing his impressions of the MMCP, which was shown as a looped continuum in which experiences could be linked horizontally and vertically (ascent/descent) as the child/young person makes sense of new musical encounters by assimilating and accommodating these – in a Piagetian sense – into existing conceptualisations and understandings of their sonic world. This was a time when Piaget’s and Bruner’s theories, based on their grounded observations of young children, were highly influential in suggesting that learning develops in the light of pre-existing perception and cognition (e.g., Piaget, Reference PIAGET, Gattengno and Hodgson1962), and also through social experience with knowledgeable others (e.g., Bruner, Reference BRUNER1985).

This child-centred view of the world was (and is) particularly important as a way of challenging a more overtly teacher-directed, subject-based pedagogy which assumed that there was (is) a ‘canon’ of existing knowledge into which the child needs to be inducted – a perceived weakness of the 1960’s US school music curriculum which the MMCP was designed to address. Whilst music repertoire-related knowledge is important, it can ignore (and be ignorant of) what the learner brings to the activity and so be less effective as the basis for an approach which actually promotes learning by allowing for a sense of agency on the part of the learner. This tension is echoed in the Department for Education’s (DfE) new ‘Model Music Curriculum’ (2021), which contains selected examples from the musical canon – albeit with some examples from the wider world of music for each phase of schooling. Rather more positively – in the sense of making explicit reference to the perception and cognition of music, a new research-based review of music by Ofsted (2021) – the national inspection body for schools in England – also appears to steer an uneasy line between the need for an induction into music’s ‘elements’ (the ‘domain’ of music) and how people learn best. In places, there is a view of the music learner that is biased towards an expert perspective, rather than what an individual might bring to the taskFootnote 5, although there is a recognition elsewhere of the need for explicit music curricular space for creativity and innovation.

The MMCP was important because, at its core, it had a learner-centred (‘child-centred’) approach in which ‘the child is not seen as a spectator in music, but as an active participant’ (Walker, Reference WALKER1984: 27). The cultural and pedagogical antecedents for this can be traced back to those early pioneers of learning through exploration and play, such as Froebel (d. 1852Footnote 6) (cf Liebschner, Reference LIEBSCHNER1992) and Pestalozzi (d. 1827) (cf Silber, Reference SILBER1973), and – later in the 20th century – by Piaget (Reference PIAGET, Gattengno and Hodgson1962), Vygotsky (Reference VYGOTSKY1967), Bronfenbrenner (Reference BRONFENBRENNER1979) and Bruner (Reference BRUNER1986). More recently, others have continued to stress the important of play, both in general development, such as by Pellegrini (Reference PELLEGRINI2009), Fagan (Reference FAGAN, Nathan and Pellegrini2010), Bruce (Reference BRUCE2011) and Zosh et al. (Reference ZOSH, HOPKINS, JENSEN, LIU, NEALE, HIRSH-PAREK, SOLIS and WHITEBREAD2017), and also in the diverse spaces where musical development occurs. These contexts include school and community playgrounds (Marsh, Reference MARSH2008; Hardwood & Marsh, Reference HARWOOD, MARSH, McPherson and Welch2018; Saltari & Welch, Reference SALTARI and WELCHin press), in the home and local community (Wu & Welch, Reference WU, WELCH, Barrett and Welch2022), with online media (Veblen & Kruse, Reference VEBLEN, KRUSE, Waldron, Horsley and Veblen2020), and in sites of transition, trauma and conflict (McFerran, Reference MCFERRAN, Barrett and Welch2022).

However, there is a potential difficulty if we assume that a developmental curriculum is identical to what we might expect in a child’s development. A curriculum that is based on a concept of development presumably needs to take account of possible individual variations if it is to have the widest applicability. And, as pointed out by Anderson (Reference ANDERSON2019), a curriculum that is sequenced in some way is not necessarily well matched to the variations and idiosyncrasies of a particular child’s learning. Consequently, it seems important first to explore the nature of children’s development, both more broadly, as well as in relation to how they make sense of music and develop musically.

Understanding sonic sense-making in context

In a recent book, Mark Reybrouck (Reference REYBROUCK2021) reports on his lifelong interest in what he calls ‘musical sense-making’ (Reference REYBROUCK2021: xvii), being related to the experience of music as sound. Musical experience is seen as a dynamic, active process of ‘sense-making’, which is both essentially subjective and peculiar to the individual, as well as social, being an appreciation that we are social, communal beings and open to influence by others.

A related perspective is offered by Nina Kraus (Reference KRAUS2021) in her new book where she reviews a lifetime of research into understanding the central significance of sound in the human condition. ‘Our sense of hearing is always on’ (p. 1). Kraus (op. cit., p. 96) argues convincingly that the ‘sound mind’ – those areas of the brain engaged in our sonic experience – ‘is vast, engaging our cognitive, motor, reward, and sensory networks’. Within this neurological landscape, Kraus’ emphasis on reward is particularly important in any consideration of children’s musical behaviour and development, because it is a pertinent reminder of the significance of emotion in all our human experience, including music.

Emotion (self-perceived as ‘feelings’ when consciously labelled) is a core feature of our human design. Nearly 50 years ago, the pioneering neuroscientist and pharmacologist, Candace Pert, had discovered the brain’s opiate receptor. Subsequently, Pert drew on this ground-breaking research to propose a concept of the human ‘bodymind’ (Reference PERT1988) as a way of signalling how our three, core internal bodily systems – nervous, endocrine and immune – function in an integrated, holistic manner and communicate with each other. As such, in the same way that we cannot switch off hearing (cf Kraus, Reference KRAUS2021), we cannot switch off emotion (cf Pert, Reference PERT1988).

The research literature on musical development, including my own on children’s singing development, tends to employ simple musical tasks which can be used to make comparisons across large groups of individuals. However, such data gathering also needs to acknowledge the central importance of emotion and engagement, including in musical preferences. For example, a major, externally funded longitudinal study of children’s singing development in the early 1990s was structured around simple children’s songs as target models against which learning and development in singing could be measured (Welch, Sergeant & White, Reference WELCH, SERGEANT and WHITE1996, Reference WELCH, SERGEANT and WHITE1997, Reference WELCH, SERGEANT and WHITE1998). However, our concept of what should count as singing was challenged by an incident at the end of a morning’s fieldwork visit to one of the project’s 10 Primary schools. The young inner-city participant, aged 7 years, had been recorded in each of the two previous school years at ages 5 and 6 years. He dutifully sang that year’s specially chosen project song for our digital database. With lunchtime approaching, and as a way of filling the morning’s visit, the researcher kept the tape running and asked the boy if he would like to sing something else of his own choice. Immediately, he began to sing Michael Jackson’s ‘Heal the World’, demonstrating a level of singing skill and sophisticated performativity which was much in advance of his more childlike rendition of the earlier target research song. He had been able to acquire and reproduce the cultural features of Jackson’s song through listening at home, singing with an appropriate vocal timbre and a sensitivity to the original key and phrasing. This performance of ‘his’ song was of quite a different quality to his performance of the adult chosen ‘research’ song that had been recorded just 5 min earlier.

Thus, we need to take account of the extent to which a child’s sense of agency is allowed for in the nature of the research tasks that we are using to map their musical behaviour and development. This example was a salient reminder of how it is possible to have many ‘musics’ inside us, but that we may only be asked to use one of these when required to participate in a particular research task, and this may or may not be typical of our overall level of musical competency and our musical identity. This young boy’s example resonated with the research team and subsequently informed the fieldwork design in an (ongoing) longitudinal research study of highly skilled female choristers from one of England’s leading cathedrals. Whenever possible, the research team have taken the opportunity to gather different genre examples of these adolescent girls’ singing, including their own songs – the music that they listen to when on their own, or with friends – as well as sung examples from their formal cathedral repertoire (cf Welch & Howard, Reference WELCH and HOWARD2002).

Both Reybrouck and Kraus argue for the centrality of emotion in the experience of sound, including organised sound that is labelled as music. Welch and Preti (Reference WELCH, PRETI, Welch, Howard and Nix2019) applied a similar understanding to children’s singing and vocal development with their focus on singing as communication, being both intra- as well as interpersonal. At a more fundamental level, a new, genetics-based, review by Navarro, Martinón-Torres and Salas (Reference NAVARRO, MARTINÓN-TORRES and Salas2021) echoes the importance of sonic experience, whilst also seeking to understand the role of music in basic biology. The authors’ main premise is that human knowledge begins through sensation and perception. They argue for a new research discipline in what they conceptualise as ‘musical sensogenomics’. This would investigate the mechanisms and potential of sound-based sensory activation on the genome and – in turn – seek to understand how this activation might subsequently impact gene expression. Navarro et al. report on the identification of genes in various recent studies in music, such as related to the perception of rhythm, absolute pitch, aspects of music performance and music listening. The authors suggest that ‘complementary approaches from social sciences, neurosciences, psychophysiology, and genetics have demonstrated the importance of nature and nurture in our characterisation as musical beings’ (Navarro, Martinón-Torres & Salas, Reference NAVARRO, MARTINÓN-TORRES and Salas2021: 14). Musical development is, therefore, shaped by our encounters with sound and our basic human design, both for making sense of our soundworld and also for acting within it.

This emphasis on the prime importance of the senses in perception chimes with research in the field of special education and musical development (Ockelford & Welch, Reference OCKELFORD and WELCH2021), as well as in the early years (Ockelford & Voyajolu, Reference OCKELFORD, VOYAJOLU, Ockelford and Welch2021; Voyajolu & Ockelford, Reference VOYAJOLU and OCKELFORD2016). In these publications, Ockelford argues that an infant’s first experience of music is as a confusion of sound, then gradually through extrapolating features, patterns and structures that are available in their soundworld. This theoretical perspective forms part of the conceptual underpinning of the Sounds of Intent project, a series of applied research studies into the musical behaviour and development of children and young people with complex needs, including profound and multiple learning difficulties (PMLD) and severe learning difficulties (SLD). Grounded in hundreds of observations in real-world contexts in special schools and participant children’s homes, analyses of the research data concluded that musical behaviour can be conceived as occurring in three complementary domains: reactive (responding to sound), proactive (initiating sound) and interactive (making sound with others) (cf Ockelford & Welch, Reference OCKELFORD, WELCH, McPherson and Welch2012; Welch et al., Reference WELCH, OCKELFORD, CARTER, ZIMMERMANN and HIMONIDES2009). Furthermore, extensive empirical data suggest that musical behaviour and development are not even across the three domains (e.g., Ockelford et al., Reference OCKELFORD, WELCH, JEWELL-GORE, CHENG, VOGIATZOGLOU and HIMONIDES2011), but that there does appear to be a hierarchy within each domain towards growing accomplishment, although development for an individual child is not necessarily linear, but rather multifaceted (Ockelford & Welch, Reference OCKELFORD, WELCH, McPherson and Welch2018; see Wilde, Ockelford & Welch, Reference WILDE, OCKELFORD and WELCH2016, for an overview of how the Sounds of Intent framework was developed).

Relatedly, Reybrouck (Reference REYBROUCK2021) echoes Piaget’s theoretical position in believing that sense-making derives from sensorimotor interactions with the environment, allied to a circularity between action and perception. This leads to enactive and embodied cognition – cognition as an active process, shaped by and shaping behaviour, and which is informed by the individual taking the role of an observer in an observed system.

Such sociocultural influences remind us of the important theoretical insights which underpin the writings of Vygotsky (d.1934) and, more recently, Bronfenbrenner (Reference BRONFENBRENNER1979) and Engeström (Reference ENGESTRÖM, Engeström, Miettinen and Punamäki1999). Several authors have sought to apply social and cultural psychology to studies of musical behaviour and development, such as exampled by Barrett (Reference BARRETT and Elliott2005) in terms of young children, and (separately) to composers (Love & Barrett, Reference LOVE and BARRETT2018), and also Perkins (Reference PERKINS and Burnard2013) concerning undergraduate musicians. Moreover, studies are not confined to Western classical-type musics, as there are a growing number of examples of how children’s musical development is situated in the particulars of local contexts elsewhere across the musical spectrum. These include studies by Campbell (Reference CAMPBELL and Bresler2007) into the diversity of children’s musical cultures, as illustrated by Akuno and colleagues (Reference AKUNO, ADDO, ANDANGO’O, EMBERLEY, DAVHULA, MATIURE, Barrett and Welch2022) concerning musical learning communities in four sub-Saharan regions, by Wu and Welch (Reference WU, WELCH, Barrett and Welch2022) related to young children growing up in the Chinese diaspora in London, and in the research by Treloyn and colleagues (Reference TRELOYN, EMBERLY, CHARLES, UMBAGAI, Barrett and Welch2022) on children growing up with the musics of indigenous communities, being exampled in the northwest of Australia. In other examples of musical learning in a specific cultural context, Welch (Reference WELCH2007, Reference WELCH and Barrett2011) and Stewart (Reference STEWART2021) both drew on Engeström’s cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT) to understand the cultural shaping of female chorister development in the previously all-male tradition of English cathedrals and major chapels.

Collectively, these are illustrations of what Barrett (Reference BARRETT2011) and Hargreaves (Reference HARGREAVES, Barrett and Welch2022) see as the power of culture in shaping children’s musical development, both of individuals and groups. Furthermore, another contextual variable needs to be considered, that of technology use. Children and young people have virtual access from modern electronic media to a wide diversity of musical cultures within and beyond the immediate family. In England, for example, the antecedent of this was the advent of the gramophone in the early 20th century which allowed previous generations of children to access a diverse range of musics for the first time, both in the home (biased towards popular musics) and in school (Western classical). This innovation was closely followed by BBC radio broadcasting which was aimed specifically at promoting music and movement for children (pioneered by Ann Driver, Reference DRIVER1936). Children’s radio for schools also included singing, particularly folk songs, as found in the ‘Singing Together’ programme that was broadcast from 1939Footnote 7, and ‘Time and Tune’ for younger children from 1951Footnote 8. In the 21st century, the advent of new technologies has allowed young children to experience music across their everyday lives (cf Vestad, Reference VESTAD2010): in the home, whilst travelling, at school, during family shopping, and elsewhere in their wider communities, through television and film, as well as the web and apps on smartphones and tablets, and often as an integral part in the design of their toys. Research has illustrated how children’s musical development can be facilitated by such technology (Fredrikson et al., Reference FREDRIKSON, LIVARI, TIKKANEN, HEDBERG, Ruokamo, Eriksson, Pekkala and Vuojärvi2011; Gower & McDowall, Reference GOWER and MCDOWALL2012; Howell, Reference HOWELL, Ruthmann and Mantie2017; Myllykoski, Paananen & Saarikallio, Reference MYLLYKOSKI, PAANANEN, SAARIKALLIO, Mota and Aiqing2010), including for deaf and partially hearing children (Welch et al., Reference WELCH, SAUNDERS, EDWARDS, PALMER, HIMONIDES, KNIGHT, MAHON, GRIFFIN and VICKERS2015, Reference WELCH, SAUNDERS, WILDE, MASON, MAYNARD, KNIGHT and Orman2018). Webster Reference WEBSTER and McPherson(2016) offers a recent summary of such technology and provides several examples from real-life situations of children and young people using computer-based technology for musical creation. In the same chapter, he also lists a wide range of exemplar software to aid their musical development. Overall, the plethora of available technology applications adds another significant contextual, as well as cultural, variable into any research quest that seeks to define and make sense of children’s musical development in general, and musical creativity in particular.

Conscious and other-than-conscious musicking in musical development

Part of the challenge in supporting children and young people’s musical development embraces the concept of consciousness and how this might relate to musical behaviour. Reaction time studies inform us that we can consciously react to a stimulus usually in not less than between .2 and .4 of a second, although it can be slightly less in elite athletes and skilled musicians, believed to be because of enhanced neural connectivity derived from repeated practice (Landry & Champoux, Reference LANDRY and CHAMPOUX2017). Much of an infant’s general learning, such as related to controlled movement, use of language, and through their senses such as vision and audition, will involve trial-and-error and play in relation to environmental stimuli and embrace multiple variations in such stimuli, often multi-sensory. These are processed in fractions of a second that are outside conscious awareness (cf Marchman, & Fernald, Reference MARCHMAN and FERNALD2008). Perception embraces some form of concatenation (‘chunking’) of elements, in which an overall impression of the stimulus, or particular salient features, are noted. Perceptual salience may not be the same as intended salience from a pedagogical perspective, as the nature of any such correlation will depend on what the child (as perceiver) brings to the task.

Seth (Reference SETH2021a, Reference SETH2021b) distinguishes between three different types of consciousness: conscious ‘level’ – ‘how conscious you are’, such as related to wakefulness; conscious ‘content’ – ‘what you are conscious of’; and conscious ‘self’ – ‘the experience of being you’, ‘of being me’ (Reference SETH2021b: 46–47). Davies’ (Reference DAVIES1992) case studies of young children’s invented songs demonstrate a form of self-expression and musical creativity which appears to be ‘in-the-moment’ and outside conscious control, seemingly combining elements of Seth’s concepts of content and self as a form of playful expression which draws on, and combines, their experiences – sonic and otherwise – and views of the world. Furthermore, being able to create successful pieces in song form appears to have little formal relationship with the ability to sing standard song repertoire in-tune (op.cit. p. 384). Thus, the implication is that – given the widespread neural bases for musical behaviour, shaped by cultural experiences and networked within and across various modules in the brain (cf Hodges, Reference HODGES2019; Janzen & Thaut, Reference JANZEN and THAUT2019; Särkämö & Sihvonen, Reference SÄRKÄMÖ and SIHVONEN2018) – we should continue to be cautious about seeing one aspect of musical behaviour (e.g., composing) as operating at the same developmental level as another (e.g., singing repertoire, or improvised singing).

In terms of conscious content and singing, for example, there is an extensive literature that, when some young children in Western cultures are asked to learn a new song, they perceive the lyrics as having the greatest salience rather than any of the song’s particular musical features (other than rhythm), such as the constituent pitches and melody (Rutkowski, 1984, 1990, Reference RUTKOWSKI and Brophy2019, Welch, Reference WELCH1986, Reference WELCH and McPherson2016; Welch, Sergeant & White, Reference WELCH, SERGEANT and WHITE1996, Reference WELCH, SERGEANT and WHITE1997). In the past, such children were assessed and labelled as being ‘tone deaf’, in the sense of being unmusical. As I reported in 1979 and 1994, this bipartite view of musical behaviour can become reified in the language of popular culture through the use of such labels as ‘growler’, ‘grunter’, ‘monotone’, ‘tone deaf’ (Welch, Reference WELCH1994), ‘tone idiot’ (=Onchi in Japan, Welch & Murao, Reference WELCH and MURAO1994), rather than seeing children on a potential journey towards greater competency if their environment is sufficiently nurturing.

Subsequent large datasets on children’s singing (e.g., longitudinal data: Welch, Sergeant & White, Reference WELCH, SERGEANT and WHITE1997; comparative data: Welch et al., Reference WELCH, SAUNDERS, PAPAGEORGI, HIMONIDES, Harrison, Welch and Adler2012), involving many thousands of children in total, reveal that the proportions of children who are less than tuneful in their singing commonly decreases with age, such that there are virtually no girls who are classified as ‘out-of-tune’ singers by the age of 11 years, and only a small proportion of boys (Welch, Saunders, Papapegorgi & Himonides, Reference WELCH, SAUNDERS, PAPAGEORGI, HIMONIDES, Harrison, Welch and Adler2012). Furthermore, if the singing task is focused purely on a song’s musical elements and not combined with the lyrics, both sexes are much more pitch accurate vocally and virtually identical in their developing competency (cf Welch, Reference WELCH, Welch, Howard and Nix2019). Thus, our notions of children’s singing behaviour and development and the assessment of these are highly context-related, with key variables of age, sex/gender, musical task, intentionality and assessment methodology and tools. The mapping of musical behaviours is itself socially located.

Gradually, over time, most children will learn to sing ‘in-tune’ related to a cultural stimulus, although – at the level of the individual – differences are still likely be evidenced, depending on the assessment criteria employed. These might embrace being in-tune throughout, or in-tune for the main pitch anchor points – tonic/dominant – but with other pitches being more variable, or related to the evenness in the quality of vocal timbre. Furthermore, this challenge in making sense of the child’s musical behaviour is inherently intertwined with the nature of the rater’s perception, that is, the person responsible for making a judgement of the child’s musical behaviour and its relative development. Judgement embraces various facets, including what counts as salient for the rater. Even when the criteria for assessment appear to be explicit, it is often only through multiple judgements that a more robust picture is likely to emerge, irrespective of whether the assessment is human- or technology-based.

For example, a study by Buckton of 6-year-old singing in New Zealand (Reference BUCKTON1982) revealed that, when asked about the proportion of ‘out-of-tune’ singers in their class, the teachers’ responses varied from ‘none’ – when actually most of the children sang out-of-tune on an assessed criterion song in her class – to ‘over 50%’ – when, in reality, virtually all the children were in-tune in this teacher’s class. The teachers involved were generalists and not specialists in children’s singing. Their craft knowledge approach, based on experience, was often significantly at variance with a more ‘objective’ measure. Furthermore, if behaviour in singing is on the basis of the application of speech science, the commonly based algorithms which underpin acoustic measures have traditionally been derived from adult male vocal function and may not necessarily be valid and reliable in the assessment of children’s singing (Welch, Reference WELCH2003). Consequently, it is clear that reliability of research data is multifaceted, such as being related to the number of participant children involved and the number of raters, as well as the nature of the criteria and the contexts in which data are being gathered.

Conclusion

New initiatives in tracking individual musical behaviour and development, such as in the Sounds of Intent project in the context of complex needs (Welch et al., Reference WELCH, OCKELFORD, CARTER, ZIMMERMANN and HIMONIDES2009) and its recent adaptation as Sounds of Intent in the early years (Voyajolu & Ockelford, Reference VOYAJOLU and OCKELFORD2016), suggest that we should be responsive to and celebrate the internal musical worlds of individual participants as well as their multiverse contexts. This is especially important if we hope that others will be able to understand and apply the trends, nuances and limitations of our data in their quest to underpin music pedagogy with a robust evidence base.

Furthermore, the latest research trends concerning early childhood musical behaviour and development suggest that investigators are becoming increasingly aware of the significance of development in music and also through music. The multi-sited nature of musical behaviours at a neurological level demonstrate an interconnectedness between diverse brain regions (e.g., Schlaug, Reference SCHLAUG2015) and engagement in sound sense-making is likely to involve reciprocal development across different neurological networks. Thus, children’s musical development is likely to be interwoven with their other-than-musical development, as well as their emotional wellbeing. The role of the teacher is to understand these possible/actual relationships in creating successful scaffolded musical activities in which children’s current expertise can be made manifest, celebrated, shared and developed.

It is evident that the Swanwick and Tillman (Reference SWANWICK and TILLMAN1986) conception of one important aspect of musical development (creating) in a spiral form has had a strong historical impact. It continues to have a positive contemporary resonance in reminding us that musical behaviours can – and often do – change with age and experience. Subsequent studies have provided more detailed insights into how such development is shaped by contextual factors, which can be both nurturing and inhibiting, at individual as well as group levels. Perhaps the spiral’s greatest legacy is in its underlying message that the development of musical behaviours is possible rather than being fixed in some way.

Despite advances over the past 35 years, it is clear that we are still in the early stages (phases?) of a journey to understand the complexities of musical behaviour and what counts as development, as well as how this might be nurtured most effectively for all children and not just some. We are musical by design – our pedagogical challenge is to use this knowledge to enable each individual to realise their musical potential.

Footnotes

1 The Davies (Reference DAVIES1986) article was part of a case study dataset for her subsequent DPhil thesis at York, 1992.

2 The Welch (Reference WELCH1986) article drew on his recently completed 1983 London PhD into the impact of feedback on observed levels of singing competency in young children.

3 In September 2015, world leaders gathered at the United Nations to set ambitious goals for the future of the global community. Goal 4 of the Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs) sought to ensure ‘inclusive and equitable quality education and promote lifelong learning opportunities for all’ (UN, Reference VEBLEN, KRUSE, Waldron, Horsley and Veblen2015, see https://sdgs.un.org/goals). Recently, the OECD (Reference PAN, TRANG, LOVE and TEMPLIN2021: 30) reminded us that this goal (SDG4) reaffirms the importance of children’s participation in ‘high quality early childhood development, care and pre-primary education so that they are ready for Primary education’.

4 These are (1) opera, embracing 400 types of local opera; (2) local singing-narrative, with 300 examples reported; (3) folk song, with over 400,000 recorded; (4) Ci song and Qu (songs set to strict tonal and rhythmic patterns) and (5) religious recitatives – such as Buddhist, Taoist, Muslim, and each influenced by regional dialects (Yang, Reference ZIMMERMAN2011: 31). In addition, there will be many diverse forms of Western-type and Chinese popular musics, as well as Western style classical music.

5 For example, in the section on Expressive quality, the statement ‘Musical expression in performance is dependent on the highly developed technical expertise of the performer’ (2021: 17) appears to ignore the evidence that pre-school children are more than capable of performing music on their own terms (e.g., Barrett, Reference BARRETT, Welch, Howard and Nix2019).

6 For example, see the Froebel Trust’s The power of play https://www.froebel.org.uk/about-us/the-power-of-play as well as Jacob Liebschner’s extensive review of the importance of play in Froebel’s educational theory.

References

AKUNO, E. A., ADDO, A. O., ANDANGO’O, E. A., EMBERLEY, A., DAVHULA, M. & MATIURE, P. (2022). In Barrett, M. S. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. [Chapter 26].Google Scholar
ANDERSON, A. D. (2019). Happy Accidents? Music Teacher Perceptions of Curriculum Design at Key Stage 3 in the English Secondary School. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Birmingham City University, Birmingham.Google Scholar
BAMBERGER, J. (1991). The Mind Behind the Musical Ear: How Children Develop Musical Intelligence. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
BARRETT, M. S. (2005). Children’s communities of musical practice: some socio-cultural implications of a systems view of creativity in music education. In Elliott, D. J. (ed.), Praxial Music Education: Reflections and Dialogues (pp. 177195). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
BARRETT, M. S. (ed.) (2011). A Cultural Psychology of Music Education. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
BARRETT, M. S. (2019). Singing and invented song-making in infants’ and young children’s early learning and development: From shared to independent song-making. In Welch, G. F., Howard, D. & Nix, J. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Singing (pp. 471487). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
BARRETT, M. S., ABAD, V. & WELCH, G. F. (2022). Researching music early learning and development: mapping methods and techniques, locations, problems, and theories. In Barrett, M. S. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. [Chapter 7]Google Scholar
BARRETT, M. S. & WELCH, G. F. (2021). Music early learning programs: Enduring outcomes for children and their families. Psychology of Music, 49(5), 12261241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BARRETT, M. S. & WELCH, G. F. (eds.) (2022). The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
BRONFENBRENNER, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
BRUCE, T. (2011). Learning though Play: Babies, Toddlers and Young Children (2nd ed.). London: Hodder Education.Google Scholar
BRUNER, J. (1985). Models of the learner. Educational Researcher, 14(6), 58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BRUNER, J. S. (1960). The Process of Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
BRUNER, J. S. (1986). Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BUCKTON, R. (1982). Sing a Song of Six-Year-Olds. Wellington, New Zealand: Council for Educational Research.Google Scholar
CAMPBELL, P. S. (2007). Musical meaning in children’s cultures. In Bresler, L. (ed.), International Handbook of Research in Arts Education. Springer International Handbook of Research in Arts Education, 16. Dordrecht: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4020-3052-9_60 Google Scholar
COSTA-GIOMI, E. (2015). The long-term effects of childhood music instruction on intelligence and cognitive abilities. Update, 33(2), 2026.Google Scholar
DAVIES, C. (1986). Say it till a song comes (reflections on songs invented by children 3–13). British Journal of Music Education, 3(3), 279293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DAVIES, C. (1992). Young Children as Song-Makers. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of York, New York.Google Scholar
DRIVER, A. (1936). Music and Movement. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
ENGESTRÖM, Y. (1999). Activity theory and individual and social transformations. In Engeström, Y., Miettinen, R. & Punamäki, R. L. (eds.), Perspectives on Activity Theory (pp. 1938). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FAGAN, R. M. (2010). Play and development. In Nathan, P. & Pellegrini, A. D. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play (pp. 83100). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
FISHER, R. B. (1968). Learning music unconventionally. Music Educators Journal, 54, 61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FORD THOMPSON, W. & Schlaug, G. (2015). The healing power of music. Scientific American Mind, March/April, 3341.Google Scholar
FREDRIKSON, M., LIVARI, N., TIKKANEN, R. & HEDBERG, H. (2011). Building a community of musical practice for children in a child-centered way – JamMo singing game for 3–6 aged children. In Ruokamo, H., Eriksson, M., Pekkala, L. & Vuojärvi, H. (eds.), Social Media in the Middle of Nowhere – NBE 2011 Conference (pp. 3947). University of Lapland Publications in Education 25. Rovaniemi: University of Lapland.Google Scholar
GORDON, E. E. (1979). Developmental music aptitude as measured by the primary measures of music audiation. Psychology of Music, 7, 4249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GORDON, E. E. (1997). Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content, and Patterns: a Music Learning Theory; Study Guide. Chicago: GIA Publications Inc.Google Scholar
GOWER, L. & MCDOWALL, J. (2012). Interactive music video games and children’s musical development. British Journal of Music Education, 29(1), 91105. doi: 10.1017/S0265051711000398 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. (1986). The Developmental Psychology of Music. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. (2022). Cultural diversity and the explanation of musical development. In Barrett, M. S. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. [Chapter 12]Google Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. & GALTON, M. (1992). Aesthetic learning: Psychology theory and educational practice. In Reimer, B. & Smith, R. A. (eds.), 1992 N.S.E.E. Yearbook on the Arts in Education (pp. 124150). Chicago: N.S.S.E.Google Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. & LAMONT, A. (2017). The Psychology of Musical Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARWOOD, E. & MARSH, K. (2018). Children’s ways of learning inside and outside the classroom. In McPherson, G. E. & Welch, G. F. (Eds.), Music Learning and Teaching in Infancy, Childhood, and Adolescence: An Oxford Handbook of Music Education (Vol. 2, pp. 102120). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
HODGES, D. A. (2019). Music through the lens of cultural neuroscience. In M. H. Thaut & D. A. Hodges (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music and the Brain (pp. 1842). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
HOWELL, G. (2017). Exploring intersections of technology, play, informality, and innovation. In Ruthmann, A. & Mantie, R. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Technology and Music Education (pp. 249253). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
IRELAND, J. & MOUTHAAN, M. (2020). Perspectives on curriculum design: Comparing the spiral and the network models. Research Matters, 30, 712.Google Scholar
JANZEN, T. B. & THAUT, M. H. (2019). Cerebral organization of music processing. In M. H. Thaut & D. A. Hodges (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music and the Brain (pp. 88121). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
KATZ, L. (1991). Readiness: Children and Schools. Striving for Excellence: The National Education Goals. United States Department of Education, Educational Resources Center [ERIC]. EDO-PS-91-4.Google Scholar
KRAUS, N. (2021). Of Sound Mind – How Our Brain Constructs a Meaningful Sonic World. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LANDRY, S. P. & CHAMPOUX, F. (2017). Musicians react faster and are better multisensory integrators. Brain and Cognition, 111, 156162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LIEBSCHNER, J. (1992). A Child’s Work: Freedom and Play in Froebel’s Educational Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Lutterworth Press.Google Scholar
LOVE, K. & BARRETT, M. S. (2018). Signature pedagogies for musical practice: A case study of creativity development in an Orchestral Composers’ Workshop. Psychology of Music, 47(4), 551567. doi: 10.1177/0305735618765317 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MARCHMAN, V. A. & FERNALD, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9F16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MARSH, K. (2008). The Musical Playground: Global Tradition and Change in Children’s Songs and Games. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MCFERRAN, K. S. (2022). Music with young children in sites of transition, trauma, and conflict. In Barrett, M. S. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. [Chapter 34]Google Scholar
MOOG, H. (1963). Beginn und erste Entwicklung des Musikerlebens im Kindesalter. Eine empirisch-psychologische Untersuchung (Beginning and first development of musical life in childhood. An empirical-psychological investigation). Thesis, Köln.Google Scholar
MOOG, H. (1968). Das Musikleben des vorschulpflichtigen Kindes (The musical life of the pre-school aged child). Mainz: B. Schott’s Söhne.Google Scholar
MOOG, H. (1976a). The development of musical experience of children of pre-school age. Psychology of Music, 4(2), 3845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MOOG, H. (1976b). The Musical Experience of the Pre-school Child. London: Schott & Co.Google Scholar
MOON, K. S. & HUMPHREYS, J. T. (2010). The Manhattanville music curriculum program: 1966–1970. Journal of Historical Research in Music Education, 31(2), 7598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MYLLYKOSKI, M., PAANANEN, P. & SAARIKALLIO, S. (2010). ‘JamMo’ mobile music making software: Composition games for 3–6-year-old children. In Mota, G. & Aiqing, Y. (eds.), Proceedings. ISME 23rd Research Commission Seminar (pp. 102106). Changchun, China: North East Normal University.Google Scholar
NAVARRO, L., MARTINÓN-TORRES, F. & Salas, A. (2021). Sensogenomics and the biological background underlying musical stimuli: Perspectives for a new era of musical research. Genes, 12, 1454. doi: 10.3390/genes12091454 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OCKELFORD, A. & VOYAJOLU, A. (2021). The development of music-structural cognition in the early years – A perspective from the Sounds of Intent model. In Ockelford, A. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), New Approaches in Applied Musicology (pp. 1363). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
OCKELFORD, A. & WELCH, G. F. (2012). Mapping musical development in learners with the most complex needs: The sounds of intent project. In McPherson, G. & Welch, G. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Education (Vol. 2, pp. 1130). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
OCKELFORD, A. & WELCH, G. F. (2018). Mapping musical development in learners with the most complex needs: The sounds of intent project. In McPherson, G. & Welch, G. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Education (Vol. 2, pp. 727). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
OCKELFORD, A. & WELCH, G. F. (2021). Essays in Applied Musicology: A Common Framework for Music Education and Psychology Research. London: Routledge. [with Voyajolu, A., Grundy, R. & Trower, H.]Google Scholar
OCKELFORD, A., WELCH, G. F., JEWELL-GORE, L., CHENG, E., VOGIATZOGLOU, A. & HIMONIDES, E. (2011). Sounds of intent, phase 2: Approaches to the quantification music-developmental data pertaining to children with complex needs. European Journal of Special Education, 26(2), 177199. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2011.563606 Google Scholar
OECD. (2021). Education at a Glance 2021. Paris: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. doi: 10.1787/b35a14e5-en Google Scholar
OFSTED (2021). Research review series: music. London: Office for Standards in Education.Google Scholar
PAN, Q., TRANG, K. T., LOVE, H. R. & TEMPLIN, J. (2019). School readiness profiles and growth in academic achievement. Frontiers in Education, 4, 127. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2019.00127 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PEERY, J. C., PEERY, I. W. & DRAPER, T. W. (eds.) (1987). Music and Child Development. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PELLEGRINI, A. (2009). The Role of Play in Human Development. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PERKINS, D. (1974). Probing artistic process: A progress report from Harvard project zero. The Journal of Aesthetic Education, 8(3), 3357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PERKINS, R. (2013). Learning cultures, creativities and higher music education institutions. In Burnard, P. (ed.), Developing Creativities in Higher Music Education: International Perspectives and Practices (pp. 223233), London: Routledge.Google Scholar
PERT, C. B. (1988). The wisdom of the receptors: Neuropeptides, the emotions, and bodymind. Advances, 8(8), 816.Google Scholar
PIAGET, J. (1952). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. New York: W W Norton & Co.Google Scholar
PIAGET, J. (1954). The Construction of Reality in the Child. (Cook, M., Trans.). Basic Books. doi: 10.1037/11168-000 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PIAGET, J. (1962). Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood (Gattengno, C. & Hodgson, F. M., Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1951).Google Scholar
POCWIERZ-MARCINIAK, I. & HARCIAREK, M. (2021). The effect of musical stimulation and mother’s voice on the early development of musical abilities: A neuropsychological perspective. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18, 8467. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18168467 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
REYBROUCK, M. (2021). Musical Sense-Making – Enaction, Experience, and Computation. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
ROSS, M. (1984). The Aesthetic Impulse. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
RUTKOWSKI, J. (1984). Development of a Rating Scale to Assess Individual Children’s Use of the Vocal Instrument. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY.Google Scholar
RUTKOWSKI, J. (1990). The measurement and evaluation of children’s singing voice development. The Quarterly, 1(1–2), 8195.Google Scholar
RUTKOWSKI, J. (2019). Assessing singing voice development. In Brophy, T. S. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Assessment Policy and Practice in Music Education (Vol. 2, pp. 628652). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
SABOL, T. J. & PLANTA, R. C. (2017). The state of young children in the United States: School readiness. In Votruba-Drzal, E. & Dearing, E. (eds.), The Wiley Handbook of Early Childhood Development Programs, Practices and Policies (pp. 317). Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
SALTARI, R. & WELCH, G. F. (in press). Exploring the culture of Greek children’s musical games in the school playground: An ethnographic study. Research Studies in Music Education.Google Scholar
SÄRKÄMÖ, T. & SIHVONEN, A. J. (2018). Golden oldies and silver brains: Deficits, preservation, learning, and rehabilitation effects of music in ageing-related neurological disorders. Cortex, 109, 104123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SCHLAUG, G. (2015). Musicians and music making as a model for the study of brain plasticity. Progress in Brain Research, 217, 3755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SERAFINE, M. L. (1988). Music as Cognition: The Development of thought in Sound. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
SERGEANT, D. & ROCHE, S. (1973). Perceptual shifts in the auditory information processing of young children. Psychology of Music, 1(2), 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SETH, A. (2021a). Being Y0u: A New Science of Consciousness. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
SETH, A. (2021b). The hard problem of consciousness is already beginning to dissolve. New Scientist, 3350, 4th September 2021. https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg25133501-500-the-hard-problem-of-consciousness-is-already-beginning-to-dissolve/ Google Scholar
SILBER, K. (1973). Pestalozzi: The Man and His Work (3rd ed.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
SLOBODA, J. A. (1985). The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
STAVRIDES, M. (1995). The Interaction of Audience-Listening and Composing: A Study in Cyprus Schools. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of London Institute of Education, London.Google Scholar
STEWART, C. (2021). The Impact of the Introduction of Girl Choristers at SALISBURY and Its Influence on Other British Anglican Cathedral Choirs. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of London Institute of Education, London.Google Scholar
SWANWICK, K. (1979). A Basis for Music Education. Slough: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
SWANWICK, K. (1991). Further research on the musical development sequence. Psychology of Music, 19, 2232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SWANWICK, K. (2011). Musical development. Revisiting a generic theory. In Colwell, R. & Webster, P. R. (eds.), MENC Handbook of Research on Music Learning: Volume 1: Strategies (pp. 140172). New York: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780195386677.003.0004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SWANWICK, K. & TILLMAN, J. (1986). The sequence of musical development: A study of children’s composition. British Journal of Music Education, 3(3), 305339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
THOMAS, R. P. (1970). MMCP Synthesis: A Structure for Music Education. Bardonia, NY: Media Materials Inc.Google Scholar
TILLMAN, J. B. (1987). Towards a Model of the Development of Musical Creativity: A Study of the Compositions of Children Aged 3–11. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of London Institute of Education, London. [2 volumes]Google Scholar
TRELOYN, S., EMBERLY, A., CHARLES, R. G. & UMBAGAI, L. (2022). Early childhood and music in indigenous contexts: Approaches to teaching and learning in an indigenous Australian community. In Barrett, M. S. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. [Chapter 26]Google Scholar
UNITED NATIONS. (2015). Sustainable Development Goals. New York: United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. https://sdgs.un.org/goals Google Scholar
VEBLEN, K. K. & KRUSE, N. B. (2020). Children’s musical play in a digital era. In Waldron, N J. L., Horsley, S. & Veblen, K. K. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Social Media and Musical Learning (pp. 529545). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
VERNEY, J. P. (2013). Rhythmic Perception and Entrainment in 5-Year-Old Children. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Cambridge, Cambridge.Google Scholar
VESTAD, I. L. (2010). To play a soundtrack: How children use recorded music in their everyday lives. Music Education Research, 12(3), 243255. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2010.504811 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VOYAJOLU, A. & OCKELFORD, A. (2016). Sounds of intent in the early years: A proposed framework of young children’s musical development. Research Studies in Music Education, 38(1), 93113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VYGOTSKY, L. (1967). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology, 12, 6276.Google Scholar
WALKER, R. (1984). Education in thoughts too definite for words. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 77, 2030.Google Scholar
WEBSTER, P. (2016). Computer-based technology. In McPherson, G. (ed.), The Child as Musician (2nd ed., pp. 500519), New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (1983). Improvability of Poor Pitch Singing: Experiments in Feedback. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of London Institute of Education, London.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (1986). A developmental view of children’s singing. British Journal of Music Education, 3(3), 295303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (1994). The assessment of singing. Psychology of Music, 22(1), 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (2003). Voice Development, Assessment, Education and Care in Childhood and Adolescence. ESF Standing Committee for the Social Sciences, Exploratory Workshop, End-of-award report. http://www.esf.org/index.php?eID=tx_nawsecuredl&u=0&file=fileadmin/be_user/research_areas/social_sciences/documents/Voice_Development__Assessment__Education_and_Care_in_Childhood_and_Adolescence.pdf&t=1341654241&hash=5df88d13dc5e8db8f714c8ffd3674baeb06dfc31 Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (2007). Addressing the multifaceted nature of music education: An activity theory research perspective. Research Studies in Music Education, 28, 2338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (2011). Culture and gender in a cathedral music context: An activity theory exploration. In Barrett, M. (ed.), A Cultural Psychology of Music Education. (pp. 225258). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (2016). Singing and vocal development. In McPherson, G. (ed.), The Child as Musician: A Handbook of Musical Development (2nd ed., pp. 441461). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F. & HOWARD, D. M. (2002). Gendered voice in the Cathedral Choir. Psychology of Music, 30, 102120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F. & MURAO, T. (eds.). (1994). Onchi and singing development. London: David Fulton.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F. & OCKELFORD, A. (2016). The role of the institution and teachers in supporting learning. In Hallam, S., Cross, I. & Thaut, M. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology, (2nd ed., pp. 509526). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F., OCKELFORD, A., CARTER, F. C., ZIMMERMANN, S. A. & HIMONIDES, E. (2009). ‘Sounds of Intent’: Mapping musical behaviour and development in children and young people with complex needs. Psychology of Music, 37(3), 348370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F. & PRETI, C. (2019). Singing as inter- and intra-personal communication. In: Welch, G., Howard, D. M. & Nix, J. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Singing (pp. 369391). New York: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199660773.013.73 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F., SAUNDERS, J., EDWARDS, S., PALMER, Z., HIMONIDES, E., KNIGHT, J., MAHON, M., GRIFFIN, S. & VICKERS, D. A. (2015). Using singing to nurture children’s hearing? A pilot study. Cochlear Implants International, 16(S3), S63S70. doi: 10.1179/1467010015Z.000000000276 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WELCH, G. F., SAUNDERS, J., PAPAGEORGI, I. & HIMONIDES, E. (2012). Sex, gender and singing development: Making a positive difference to boys’ singing through a national programme in England. In Harrison, S., Welch, G. F. & Adler, A. (eds.), Perspectives on Males and Singing (pp. 3754). London: Springer.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F., SAUNDERS, J., WILDE, E., MASON, K., MAYNARD, L. & KNIGHT, J. (2018). Sounding Out: A research-based evaluation of a specially designed music programme for young Hearing Impaired (HI) adolescents. In Orman, E. K. (ed.), Proceedings, ISME Research Seminar, Dubai, UAE 8–13 July 2018 (pp. 183194). Dubai: Canadian University Dubai. ISBN 978-0-6481219-5-4 (print); 978-0-6481219-6-1 (ebook).Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F., SERGEANT, D. C. & WHITE, P. (1996). The singing competences of five-year-old developing singers. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 127, 155162.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F., SERGEANT, D. C. & WHITE, P. (1997). Age, sex and vocal task as factors in singing ‘in-tune’ during the first years of schooling. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 133, 153160.Google Scholar
WELCH, G. F., SERGEANT, D. C. & WHITE, P. (1998). The role of linguistic dominance in the acquisition of song. Research Studies in Music Education, 10, 6774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WELCH, G. F. (2019). Singing and vocal development. In Welch, G. F., Howard, D. M. & Nix, J. (eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Singing (pp. 509532). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WILDE, E., OCKELFORD, A. & WELCH, G. F. (2016). Muziekonderwijs voor kinderen met speciale onderwijsbehoeften (Education in music for children and young people with special needs). Cultuur+Educatie, 45, 5677.Google Scholar
WU, Y. T. & WELCH, G. F. (2022). Early childhood and musics of the diaspora. In Barrett, M. S. & Welch, G. F. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Early Childhood Music Learning and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. [Chapter 25]Google Scholar
YANG, Y. (2011). The Challenges Inherent in Promoting Traditional Folk Song Performance and Pedagogy in Chinese Higher Education: A Case Study of Hua’er. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of London Institute of Education, London.Google Scholar
ZIMMERMAN, M. P. (2003). Developmental research and the music curriculum. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 155, 5374.Google Scholar
ZIMMERMAN, M. P. (2011). Musical characteristics of children. Visions of Research in Music Education, 17. Originally published (1971) by Music Educators National Conference. Retrieved from ∼http://www--usr.rider.edu/vrme∼/ Google Scholar
ZOSH, J. M., HOPKINS, E. J., JENSEN, H., LIU, C., NEALE, D., HIRSH-PAREK, K., SOLIS, S. L. & WHITEBREAD, D. (2017). Learning through Play: A Review of the Evidence (White Paper). The LEGO Foundation, DK.Google Scholar