Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-19T11:01:46.568Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Examining the unique relationships between problematic use of the internet and impulsive and compulsive tendencies: network approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 May 2024

Chang Liu*
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Kristian Rotaru
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia; and Monash Business School, Monash University, Australia
Lei Ren
Affiliation:
Military Medical Psychology Section, Logistics University of the People's Armed Police Force, Tianjin, China; and China and Military Mental Health Services and Research Centre, Tianjin, China
Samuel R. Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Southampton, UK; and Southern Gambling Clinic and Specialist Clinic for Impulsive/Compulsive Disorders, Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
Erynn Christensen
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Mary-Ellen Brierley
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia; and Melbourne Centre for Behaviour Change, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Australia
Karyn Richardson
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Rico S. C. Lee
Affiliation:
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Australia
Rebecca Segrave
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Jon E. Grant
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Chicago, USA
Edouard Kayayan
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Sam Hughes
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Leonardo F. Fontenelle
Affiliation:
Obsessive, Compulsive, and Anxiety Spectrum Research Program, Institute of Psychiatry, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil; and D'Or Institute for Research and Education, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
Amelia Lowe
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Chao Suo
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
René Freichel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Reinout W. Wiers
Affiliation:
Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT) Laboratory, Department of Psychology, and Centre for Urban Mental Health, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands
Murat Yücel
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
Lucy Albertella
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Australia
*
Correspondence: Chang Liu. Email: chang.liu5@monash.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Both impulsivity and compulsivity have been identified as risk factors for problematic use of the internet (PUI). Yet little is known about the relationship between impulsivity, compulsivity and individual PUI symptoms, limiting a more precise understanding of mechanisms underlying PUI.

Aims

The current study is the first to use network analysis to (a) examine the unique association among impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptoms, and (b) identify the most influential drivers in relation to the PUI symptom community.

Method

We estimated a Gaussian graphical model consisting of five facets of impulsivity, compulsivity and individual PUI symptoms among 370 Australian adults (51.1% female, mean age = 29.8, s.d. = 11.1). Network structure and bridge expected influence were examined to elucidate differential associations among impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptoms, as well as identify influential nodes bridging impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptoms.

Results

Results revealed that four facets of impulsivity (i.e. negative urgency, positive urgency, lack of premeditation and lack of perseverance) and compulsivity were related to different PUI symptoms. Further, compulsivity and negative urgency were the most influential nodes in relation to the PUI symptom community due to their highest bridge expected influence.

Conclusions

The current findings delineate distinct relationships across impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI, which offer insights into potential mechanistic pathways and targets for future interventions in this space. To realise this potential, future studies are needed to replicate the identified network structure in different populations and determine the directionality of the relationships among impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptoms.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Problematic use of the internet (PUI), which involves excessive and/or otherwise problematic online behaviours, including excessive online gaming, social networking, shopping and pornography watching,Reference Ioannidis, Treder, Chamberlain, Kiraly, Redden and Stein1 poses a growing mental health research challenge due to its associated public health and societal costs.Reference Ioannidis, Treder, Chamberlain, Kiraly, Redden and Stein1,Reference Fineberg, Demetrovics, Stein, Ioannidis, Potenza and Grünblatt2 The weighted average prevalence of PUI is estimated to be 7.02% among the global population, often associated with decreased mental and physical health, impaired social functioning and productivity loss.Reference Rumpf, Effertz and Montag3 Given the prevalence and potential negative consequences of PUI, it is important to understand its underlying mechanisms; this would allow for the development of targeted interventions.

Various theoretical frameworks have proposed that personality traits may play an important role in explaining individual differences in PUI. For instance, the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model states that certain personality traits (e.g. impulsivity) may predispose individuals to develop PUI.Reference Brand, Young, Laier, Wölfling and Potenza4 Meanwhile, compulsivity has been nominated as a primary construct for understanding transdiagnostic addictive behaviours (e.g. PUI) by expert consensus.Reference Yücel, Oldenhof, Ahmed, Belin, Billieux and Bowden-Jones5 Taken together, the European Cooperation in Science and Technology Action Programme proposed that both impulsivity and compulsivity should be considered as candidate constructs in understanding PUI.Reference Fineberg, Demetrovics, Stein, Ioannidis, Potenza and Grünblatt2

Impulsivity is broadly defined as the predisposition to act rashly when facing internal/external stimuli without thinking of consequences,Reference Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz and Swann6 and has been viewed as a hallmark feature of problematic behaviours, including PUI.Reference Ioannidis, Chamberlain, Treder, Kiraly, Leppink and Redden7 As a multidimensional construct,Reference Whiteside and Lynam8 impulsivity includes five interrelated facets, namely, negative urgency (the tendency to act rashly under strong negative emotions), positive urgency (the tendency to act rashly under strong positive emotions), lack of premeditation (the tendency to act without forethought), lack of perseverance (inability to stay focused on tasks) and sensation seeking (the tendency to seek novel, exciting experience). When these different facets of impulsivity were examined individually (as opposed to being merged into an overall impulsivity score), existing research found that these facets were not equally important to PUI.Reference Burnay, Billieux, Blairy and Larøi9 These findings demonstrated the internal heterogeneity of impulsivity, indicating that each facet of impulsivity should be examined separately in relation to PUI.

Compulsivity is defined as the tendency towards undertaking repetitive, habitual actions, whereby the original goal of the act has been lost.Reference Dalley, Everitt and Robbins10,Reference Luigjes, Lorenzetti, de Haan, Youssef, Murawski and Sjoerds11 Core features of compulsivity include perfectionism, reward drive/cognitive rigidity and intolerance of uncertainty.Reference Tiego, Oostermeijer, Prochazkova, Parkes, Dawson and Youssef12 Research on the association between compulsivity and PUI is still evolving. Several studies found that compulsivity is associated with increased PUI severity.Reference Albertella, Chamberlain, Le Pelley, Greenwood, Lee and Den Ouden13 It has been proposed that compulsivity may serve to maintain PUI via rigid coping responses when facing distress.Reference Liu, Rotaru, Chamberlain, Ren, Fontenelle and Lee14

While previous research has demonstrated that both impulsivity and compulsivity may be associated with PUI, there is limited understanding of how these constructs may be related to individual PUI symptoms. This drawback may be problematic in light of research showing that PUI may be composed of heterogeneous symptoms and that each of these symptoms may have unique relationships with risk factors.Reference Zhao, Qu, Chen and Chi15 For instance, PUI symptoms characterised by interpersonal conflict (e.g. yelling when being bothered during internet use) may be particularly relevant to negative urgency, as high negative urgency may increase individuals’ propensity towards rash reactions when irritated. Thus, by looking specifically at the nuanced associations between risk-related traits and individual PUI symptoms, researchers may gain insights into the specific mechanisms that give risk to different PUI symptom profiles and inform more precise profile-targeted interventions for PUI.

One way of understanding how specific impulsive and compulsive traits may be related to individual PUI symptoms is through network analysis. As a graphic-based approach, network analysis enables researchers to estimate and visualise in an insightful way the complex interrelationships between predisposing variables and individual psychological symptoms.Reference Bringmann and Eronen16 Within a network, impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptoms are depicted as nodes, which may directly connect to each other through edges between them.Reference Borsboom and Cramer17 By inspecting the network structure, researchers may gain a direct understanding of which PUI symptoms are most closely related to a given predisposing variable and edges linking predisposing variables to individual PUI symptoms. Further, network analysis employs a concept known as ‘bridge centrality indices’ to statistically gauge the extent to which a specific node surpasses its originating psychological constructs and forms connections with theoretically independent constructs within the network.Reference Jones, Ma and McNally18Reference Liu, Ren, Rotaru, Liu, Li and Yang20 The bridge centrality index quantifies the extent to which a specific node within one subnetwork is connected to all other nodes in another subnetwork within the overarching network. This index is used to pinpoint nodes that are crucial for bridging different psychopathological constructs within the network. In the context of the current study, nodes with higher bridge centrality play a more pivotal role in connecting predisposing variables (e.g. impulsivity and compulsivity) and PUI.Reference Jones, Ma and McNally18

Study aims

The current study represents the first application of network analysis to reveal the interrelations among impulsivity, compulsivity and individual PUI symptoms. By examining the network structure and bridge centrality, we aimed to (a) ascertain the specific edges among impulsivity, compulsivity and individual PUI symptoms, and (b) quantify the extent to which each predisposing variable is linked to the PUI symptom community (subnetwork) and identify the most influential bridge nodes in the network.

Method

Participants

The study engaged individuals who reside in Australia, recruited from two sources. The first group consisted of community members sourced through social media advertisement outreach, while the second comprised online users recruited via the Prolific crowdsourcing platform (www.prolific.com).

To be considered for this study, participants needed to be adults, 18 years or older, who had given their informed consent. Out of the eligible participants (n = 878), 397 completed measures assessing traits of impulsivity and compulsivity. However, from this subset, only 370 participants reported excessive internet use within the past three months (by responding ‘yes’ to the question ‘Have you used the internet excessively in the past three months?’) and, as a result, completed the PUI measure. Therefore, the present analyses incorporated data from these 370 individuals. The sample size exceeds the minimum sample size required for an 18-node network.Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried21 Notably, 50.3% of these participants demonstrated PUI as determined by the established cut-off score (IAT-12 > 30).Reference Pawlikowski, Altstötter-Gleich and Brand22

Participants from the community were offered the opportunity to enter a draw for one of 50 JB HiFi vouchers, each worth AU$100, as compensation upon completion of the study. Meanwhile, participants recruited through Prolific received an hourly reimbursement of £7.50. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Ethics Committee of Monash University (Project ID: 24401). Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Measures

Short UPPS-P impulsivity scale (S-UPPS-PReference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi23)

This instrument consists of 20 items designed to measure impulsivity. The scale is divided into five distinct subscales, namely negative urgency (example item: ‘When I feel bad, I will often do things I later regret in order to make myself feel better now’), positive urgency (example item: ‘I tend to lose control when I am in a great mood’), lack of premeditation (example item: ‘I like to stop and think things over before I do them’), lack of perseverance (example item: ‘I finish what I start’) and sensation seeking (sample item: ‘I quite enjoy taking risks’). Participants are asked to rate their agreement with each statement on a scale from ‘strongly agree’ (1) to ‘strongly disagree’ (4). Scores from negative urgency, sensation seeking and positive urgency subscales were reverse coded, and all five subscale scores were utilised in the data analysis. The internal consistency (McDonald's ω) of each subscale in the current study was as follows: negative urgency (0.76), positive urgency (0.82), lack of premeditation (0.76), lack of perseverance (0.63) and sensation seeking (0.72).

The Cambridge-Chicago compulsivity trait scale (CHITReference Chamberlain and Grant24)

This 15-item self-report measure covers broad aspects of compulsivity, including perfectionism or need for completion, habitual behaviour, reward-seeking, desire for high standards and avoidance of difficult-to-control situations.Reference Chamberlain and Grant24,Reference Hampshire, Hellyer, Soreq, Mehta, Ioannidis and Trender25 In the version applied in this study, responses ranged from ‘strongly disagree’ (0) to ‘strongly agree’ (3). The total score was employed in the data analysis, and the scale demonstrated acceptable internal consistency in the current study (McDonald's ω = 0.71).

Young's internet addiction test (IAT), short version (IAT-12Reference Pawlikowski, Altstötter-Gleich and Brand22)

This is a 12-item measure of PUI. Participants who had indicated excessive internet use over the past three months were invited to complete the IAT. An example item is ‘How often do you lose sleep due to being online late at night?’ Response options range from ‘never’ (1) to ‘very often’ (5). Individual item scores were used in the data analysis. The scale exhibited good internal consistency in the current study (McDonald's ω = 0.87).

Data analysis

The network was estimated using the Gaussian graphical model (GGM), an undirected network where edges reflect partial correlations between nodes after controlling for all other nodes in the network. In our study, GGM was estimated based on Spearman's partial correlation, which calculates the pairwise relationships between nodes while adjusting for the effects of all other nodes within the network. We preferred Spearman's partial correlation over Pearson's, due to the former's resilience to skewed data, making it suitable for non-normally distributed data.Reference Isvoranu and Epskamp26

We used R, version 3.3.3 for Mac OS (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) to perform the network analysis. For regularisation, we used the Extended Bayesian Information Criterion Graphical Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (EBICglasso) procedure. This regularisation approach minimises trivial and minor coefficients to zero, reducing false-positive edges and generating a sparse network composed of the most robust edges.Reference Epskamp and Fried27 To strike a balance between sensitivity and specificity, we set the regularisation penalty term to 0.5.Reference Epskamp and Fried27 The Fruchterman-Reingold algorithmReference Fruchterman and Reingold28 was utilised for network visualisation. Within these visualised networks, correlation magnitude was represented by edge thickness, with thicker edges indicating stronger correlations. Positive correlations were designated by blue edges and negative correlations by red edges, and nodes with stronger connections were situated closer together. The R package qgraph (version 1.9.2)Reference Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Borsboom29 was utilised for network estimation and visualisation.

The nodes in the displayed networks were pre-grouped into two communities, specifically the trait community (subscale scores of the S-UPPS-P scale and CHIT sum score) and the symptom community (individual items from the IAT scale). Bridge expected influence was employed to quantify how much each trait might connect to the PUI symptom community and identify influential bridge nodes. The concept of bridge expected influence tallies the total connectivity (i.e. sum of edge weights) from a specific node within one community to all nodes in a separate community,Reference Jones, Ma and McNally18 application of which is advised when the network encapsulates both positive and negative edges.Reference Jones, Ma and McNally18 Theoretically, nodes with high positive bridge expected influence values hold a higher probability of disseminating influence and prompting activation within the connected community.Reference Jones, Ma and McNally18

We ascertained edge accuracy by plotting the 95% CI (using 2000 bootstrap samples) of the edge weights and computed bootstrapped difference tests for edge weights. The Correlation-Stability coefficient was calculated to estimate the stability of the bridge expected influence centrality measure using a case-dropping bootstrap approach (with 2000 bootstrap samples). Bootstrapped difference tests for node bridge centrality were also calculated. The minimum acceptable Correlation-Stability-coefficient is 0.25, though preferably above 0.5.Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried21 These procedures were carried out using the R package bootnet (version 1.5.3).Reference Epskamp and Fried30

Results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics of the examined variables. The sample was composed of 370 participants (51.1% female) with an average age of 29.8 (s.d. = 11.1). The majority of the participants were currently employed (n = 303, 81.9%), and 236 (63.7%) reported that they had attained a bachelor's degree or higher. Additionally, 72 participants (19.5%) disclosed receiving income support payments from the government.

Table 1 Descriptive information of demographic and study variables

M, mean; IAT, internet addiction test.

Network estimation

The estimated network is depicted in Fig. 1(a). CHIT demonstrated a positive correlation with four PUI symptoms (IAT 2, IAT 6, IAT 7, IAT 11), with weights ranging from 0.02 to 0.08. The strongest edge emerged between compulsivity and IAT 7 (‘How often do you feel preoccupied with the internet when offline or fantasise about being online?’), yielding an edge weight of 0.08. Similarly, negative urgency was positively correlated with four PUI symptoms (IAT 3, IAT 4, IAT 5, IAT 10), with weights varying between 0.02 and 0.07. The strongest edge materialised between negative urgency and IAT 3 (‘How often do your grades or schoolwork suffer because of the amount of time you spend online?’), presenting an edge weight of 0.07. Positive urgency showed a positive correlation with four PUI symptoms (IAT 3, IAT 4, IAT 11, IAT 12) with weight ranging from <0.01 to 0.03. The most significant connection was observed between positive urgency and IAT 12 (‘How often do you feel depressed, moody or nervous when you are offline, which goes away once you are back online?’), giving an edge weight of 0.03. Lack of premeditation revealed a negative correlation with one PUI symptom, IAT 11 (‘How often do you choose to spend more time online over going out with others?’), presenting an edge weight of -0.02. Lack of perseverance was positively correlated with one PUI symptom, IAT 2 (‘How often do you neglect household chores to spend more time online?’), giving an edge weight of 0.07. Sensation seeking exhibited no association with any PUI symptoms. Bootstrapped CIs of each node (Supplementary Figure 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2024.59) and bootstrapped edge weight difference test (Supplementary Figure 2) are provided in the Supplementary Materials.

Fig. 1 (a) Network structure of the estimated network. Solid edges represent positive correlations and dotted edges represent negative correlations. The thickness of the edge reflects the magnitude of the correlation. Cut value = 0.03. The text of problematic use of the internet symptoms can be seen in Table 1. (b) Bridge centrality plot. CHIT, compulsivity; NU, negative urgency; IAT, internet addiction test; LoPM, lack of premeditation; LoPers, lack of perseverance; SS, sensation seeking; PU, positive urgency.

Bridge centrality

Raw bridge expected influence values are illustrated in Fig. 1(b). Two nodes displaying the highest bridge expected influence were identified – CHIT and negative urgency, followed by lack of perseverance, positive urgency, sensation seeking and lack of premeditation (in descending order of bridge centrality). The Correlation-Stability-coefficient for bridge expected influence is 0.28, surpassing the recommended cut-off value (i.e. 0.25). Results from bootstrapped stability tests (Supplementary Figure 3) and bootstrapped difference tests (Supplementary Figure 4) are presented in the Supplementary Materials.

Discussion

This investigation stands as the first to scrutinise the unique relationships among impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptoms. One significant advancement facilitated by the current study lies in exposing the distinct relationships between well established predisposing factors (i.e. impulsivity and compulsivity) and PUI, while controlling for shared variances. Regarding our first aim, we discerned several distinct relationships among impulsivity traits, trait compulsivity and PUI symptoms (e.g. negative urgency-interpersonal conflict and positive urgency-withdrawal), with the sole negative relation appearing between lack of premeditation and neglect of social activities. Regarding our second aim, we discovered that trait compulsivity and negative urgency were the most influential bridge nodes in the network, thus affirming our hypothesis.

The European Cooperation in Science and Technology Action Programme called for research into elucidating the potential role of compulsivity in PUI.Reference Fineberg, Demetrovics, Stein, Ioannidis, Potenza and Grünblatt2 In response to this call, we investigated how trait compulsivity might uniquely relate to individual PUI symptoms. We found that trait compulsivity was closely tied to PUI symptoms characterised by negative consequences (e.g. sleep loss, neglect of household chores and neglect of social activities). This can be attributed to cognitive inflexibility, a hallmark of compulsivity.Reference Chamberlain, Solly, Hook, Vaghi, Robbins, NA and TW31 Specifically, inflexible individuals are more likely to struggle with adjusting their behavioural patterns,Reference Albertella, Le Pelley, Chamberlain, Westbrook, Lee and Fontenelle32 and hence are more likely to persistently engage in internet use despite experiencing aversive consequences such as sleep loss and failure to fulfil role obligations at home.

By pinpointing specific trait-symptom relationships, our results contribute to the ongoing debate over whether positive urgency and negative urgency should be considered as two distinct constructs (e.g. Reference Billieux, Heeren, Rochat, Maurage, Bayard and Bet33,Reference Cyders, Smith, Spillane, Fischer, Annus and Peterson34 ). Cyders et alReference Cyders, Smith, Spillane, Fischer, Annus and Peterson34 argued that positive urgency is distinct from negative urgency as it explains unique variance in problematic behaviours that is not explained by negative urgency. Conversely, a meta-analysis contended that both traits demonstrated a relatively similar pattern of correlations across different mental disorders including substance-related addictions.Reference Berg, Latzman, Bliwise and Lilienfeld35 Nevertheless, most empirical studies examining the roles of positive and negative urgency in psychopathology were based on the sum-score approach, which considered mental disorders as unitary constructs (indexed by symptom sum scores). As previously mentioned, this approach might conceal symptom heterogeneity and might potentially overlook different association patterns between predisposing variables and symptoms. Supporting this viewpoint, we found some unique relationships that might distinguish positive from negative urgency. For instance, positive urgency has a strong positive relationship with withdrawal (IAT 12), which is not observable for negative urgency. Moreover, no association was found between positive urgency and interpersonal conflict (IAT 5), which is pronounced for negative urgency only. These results suggest that, when controlling for the shared variance, positive and negative urgency differ in their co-occurring symptoms and support the notion that positive urgency and negative urgency may be considered as two distinct constructs.

Our results also help clarify the role of lack of perseverance in PUI. We found that lack of perseverance was uniquely related to neglect of household chores (IAT 2). One theory posits that the association between lack of perseverance and PUI may be explained by intrusive thoughts in relation to the internet, as such thoughts may trigger craving, leading to excessive internet use.Reference Burnay, Billieux, Blairy and Larøi9 However, we did not find any association between lack of perseverance and fantasising about being online (IAT 7). The unique association between lack of perseverance and neglect of household chores may suggest a procrastinatory use of the internet,Reference Breems and Basden36,Reference Sümer and Büttner37 with individuals high on lack of perseverance using the internet to procrastinate about intended but dull tasks (e.g. doing household chores).

Interestingly, we found a distinct negative relationship between lack of premeditation and neglect of social activities (IAT 11). A possible explanation for this association may be that people characterised by lack of premeditation tend to be less organised and may rush into things without forethought. Thus, instead of purposefully choosing between spending more time online and going out with others, these individuals may randomly allocate their time to either of these activities.

Our study aligns with earlier research,Reference Burnay, Billieux, Blairy and Larøi9,Reference Liu, Lan, Wu and Yan38 failing to find connections between sensation seeking and PUI. This may be attributed to IAT 12 focusing solely on addictive PUI. Sensation seeking could be more applicable to dangerous and antisocial PUI types, not addictive PUI.Reference Billieux, Maurage, Lopez-Fernandez, Kuss and Griffiths39,Reference Canale, Moretta, Pancani, Buodo, Vieno and Dalmaso40 Future research should explore differences in network connectivity between sensation seeking and various internet usage types.

Our network's node bridge centrality offers insights into the relative importance of impulsivity and compulsivity in connection to the PUI symptom community. As hypothesised, compulsivity and negative urgency emerged as bridge nodes within the network. The significant role of negative urgency aligns with previous research involving Chinese university students that found negative urgency to have the most significant impact on PUI (among the five UPPS-P facets).Reference Liu, Lan, Wu and Yan38 Crucially, our results underscore the primary role of compulsivity in PUI, indicating it may characterise a behavioural phenotype of PUI.

In theory, addressing nodes with high bridge centrality could deactivate the symptom community. Both compulsivity and negative urgency might be associated with impaired cognitive functioning (i.e. cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control). Consequently, cognitive training focusing on flexibility and inhibitory control may effectively reduce these traits. Further, digital personality change interventions have shown promising results in reducing unwanted traits (e.g. neuroticism).Reference Stieger, Flückiger, Rüegger, Kowatsch, Roberts and Allemand41 Future research should examine the applicability of such interventions in reducing compulsivity and negative urgency.

Despite the promise of our findings, there are several limitations that merit consideration. First, given the cross-sectional design, causal relationships cannot be definitively established among the studied variables. Future research should strive to confirm these findings with longitudinal data. Second, the variables in this study were examined through self-report measures, inducing potential reporting errors and shared method variance. However, these measures capture in a concise and convenient manner a wealth of information about traits. Third, the current results were generated from a community sample; thus, there are limitations regarding the extent to which these findings would apply in the clinical world. Future studies should aim to replicate our findings in clinical contexts, such as with individuals exhibiting severe levels of PUI or those currently undergoing PUI treatment. Fourth, despite our study meeting the minimum sample size requirement (153 individuals for an 18-node network),Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried21 the network stability was acceptable but not optimal. It would be beneficial if future studies attempted to replicate current findings under conditions of optimal stability. Last, bridge nodes theoretically have the potential to activate the symptom community,Reference Jones, Ma and McNally18 and this assumption needs to be empirically tested.

Future directions

In our study, we recognised compulsivity as one of the influential nodes in relation to the PUI symptom community. Despite its multidimensional nature, there is no consensus on the specific dimensions included in the compulsivity constructs. Future research should aim to (a) determine compulsivity's constituent dimensions, and (b) examine the relationships between different compulsivity dimensions and PUI symptoms. This information may help identify the critical compulsivity dimension related to PUI symptoms, informing more precise prevention and interventions.

The current network was estimated on cross-sectional, between-subject data. Given the mixed evidence on the validity of using centrality metrics derived from cross-sectional data to predict symptom changes over time, and concerns over whether results from between-subject data may predict personalised dynamic processes,Reference Fried and Cramer42 it is crucial for future studies to evaluate our findings using time series data with dynamical systems approaches.

Conclusion

Our study is the first exploratory endeavour to apply network analysis to model the intricate relationships between impulsivity, compulsivity and PUI symptomatology. Our findings began to illuminate the specific and distinct relationships between impulsivity, compulsivity and individual PUI symptoms, and pinpointed negative urgency and compulsivity as influential bridging nodes. To enhance the robustness and applicability of our findings, it is essential to verify the identified network structure in independent data-sets and determine the directions of relationships using longitudinal data. Conducting these replication and extension studies across both subclinical and clinical populations will establish a solid base for translating the findings into prevention and intervention strategies.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2024.59.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, C.L., on reasonable request.

Acknowledgement

We are extremely grateful to the Wilson Foundation and David Winston Turner Endowment Fund whose generous philanthropic investment in the BrainPark research team and facility made this research possible. K.R. and S.H. were supported by the Wilson Foundation.

Author contributions

C.L., M.Y. and L.A. developed the study idea and design. C.L. wrote the original draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to revising subsequent versions of the paper.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency or commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

J.E.G. has received research grants from Biohaven, Promentis and Otsuka Pharmaceuticals. He also receives yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Norton Press and McGraw Hill. L.A. receives a stipend for her work as Associate Editor at Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, and at Comprehensive Psychiatry. M.Y. receives funding from government funding bodies such as the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia, Australian Research Council (ARC), Australian Defence Science and Technology (DST), the Department of Industry, Innovation and Science (DIIS) and the National Institutes of Health (NIH, USA); philanthropic donations from the David Winston Turner Endowment Fund and Wilson Foundation; and sponsored Investigator-Initiated trials (and associated licencing fees) from Incannex Healthcare Ltd. These funding sources had no role in the data analysis, presentation or interpretation and write-up of the data. M.Y. also sits on the Advisory Boards of: Centre of The Urban Mental Health, University of Amsterdam and Enosis Therapeutics. R.S.C.L. was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council Investigator Grant funded by the Medical Research Future Fund (APP1193946). S.R.C. receives a stipend for his work as Associate Editor at Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews and at Comprehensive Psychiatry. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ioannidis, K, Treder, MS, Chamberlain, SR, Kiraly, F, Redden, SA, Stein, DJ, et al. Problematic internet use as an age-related multifaceted problem: evidence from a two-site survey. Addict Behav 2018; 81: 157–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fineberg, NA, Demetrovics, Z, Stein, DJ, Ioannidis, K, Potenza, MN, Grünblatt, E, et al. Manifesto for a European research network into problematic usage of the internet. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2018; 28(11): 1232–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumpf, H-J, Effertz, T, Montag, C. The cost burden of problematic internet usage. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2022; 44: 101107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brand, M, Young, KS, Laier, C, Wölfling, K, Potenza, MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: an interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016; 71: 252–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yücel, M, Oldenhof, E, Ahmed, SH, Belin, D, Billieux, J, Bowden-Jones, H, et al. A transdiagnostic dimensional approach towards a neuropsychological assessment for addiction: an international Delphi consensus study. Addict 2019; 114(6): 1095–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, FG, Barratt, ES, Dougherty, DM, Schmitz, JM, Swann, AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158(11): 1783–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, K, Chamberlain, SR, Treder, MS, Kiraly, F, Leppink, EW, Redden, SA, et al. Problematic internet use (PIU): associations with the impulsive-compulsive spectrum. An application of machine learning in psychiatry. J Psychiatr Res 2016; 83: 94102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiteside, SP, Lynam, DR. The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers Individ Diff 2001; 30(4): 669–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnay, J, Billieux, J, Blairy, S, Larøi, F. Which psychological factors influence internet addiction? Evidence through an integrative model. Comput Hum Behav Rep 2015; 43: 2834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalley, JW, Everitt, BJ, Robbins, TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 2011; 69(4): 680–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luigjes, J, Lorenzetti, V, de Haan, S, Youssef, GJ, Murawski, C, Sjoerds, Z, et al. Defining compulsive behavior. Neuropsychol Rev 2019; 29(1): 413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiego, J, Oostermeijer, S, Prochazkova, L, Parkes, L, Dawson, A, Youssef, G, et al. Overlapping dimensional phenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity explain co-occurrence of addictive and related behaviors. CNS Spectr 2019; 24(4): 426–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Chamberlain, SR, Le Pelley, ME, Greenwood, L-M, Lee, RS, Den Ouden, L, et al. Compulsivity is measurable across distinct psychiatric symptom domains and is associated with familial risk and reward-related attentional capture. CNS Spectr 2020; 25(4): 519–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C, Rotaru, K, Chamberlain, SR, Ren, L, Fontenelle, LF, Lee, RS, et al. The moderating role of psychological flexibility on the association between distress-driven impulsivity and problematic internet use. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022; 19(15): 9592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Y, Qu, D, Chen, S, Chi, X. Network analysis of internet addiction and depression among Chinese college students during the COVID-19 pandemic: a longitudinal study. Comput Hum Behav 2023; 138: 107424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bringmann, LF, Eronen, MI. Don't blame the model: reconsidering the network approach to psychopathology. Psychol Rev 2018; 125(4): 606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsboom, D, Cramer, AO. Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Ann Rev Clin Psychol 2013; 9(1): 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, PJ, Ma, R, McNally, RJ. Bridge centrality: a network approach to understanding comorbidity. Multivar Behav Res 2021; 56(2): 353–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, S, Liu, C, Rotaru, K, Li, K, Wei, X, Yuan, S, et al. The relations between emotion regulation, depression and anxiety among medical staff during the late stage of COVID-19 pandemic: a network analysis. Psychiatry Res 2022; 317: 114863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C, Ren, L, Rotaru, K, Liu, X, Li, K, Yang, W, et al. Bridging the links between Big Five personality traits and problematic smartphone use: A network analysis. J Behav Addict 2023; 12(1): 128–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S, Borsboom, D, Fried, EI. Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behav Res Methods 2018; 50(1): 195212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pawlikowski, M, Altstötter-Gleich, C, Brand, M. Validation and psychometric properties of a short version of Young's internet addiction test. Comput Hum Behav 2013; 29(3): 1212–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cyders, MA, Littlefield, AK, Coffey, S, Karyadi, KA. Examination of a short English version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Addict Behav 2014; 39(9): 1372–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Grant, JE. Initial validation of a transdiagnostic compulsivity questionnaire: the Cambridge-Chicago compulsivity trait scale. CNS Spectr 2018; 23(5): 340–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampshire, A, Hellyer, PJ, Soreq, E, Mehta, MA, Ioannidis, K, Trender, W, et al. Associations between dimensions of behaviour, personality traits, and mental-health during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Kingdom. Nat Commun 2021; 12(1): 115.Google ScholarPubMed
Isvoranu, A-M, Epskamp, S. Which estimation method to choose in network psychometrics? Deriving guidelines for applied researchers. Psychol Methods 2023: 28(4): 925–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S, Fried, EI. A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol Methods 2018; 23(4): 617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fruchterman, TM, Reingold, EM. Graph drawing by force-directed placement. Softw: Pract Exper 1991; 21(11): 1129–64.Google Scholar
Epskamp, S, Cramer, AO, Waldorp, LJ, Schmittmann, VD, Borsboom, D. Qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Softw 2012; 48: 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S, Fried, E. Package ‘bootnet'. R Package Version 1.5.3. 2020.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Solly, JE, Hook, RW, Vaghi, MM, Robbins, TW. Cognitive inflexibility in OCD and related disorders. In The Neurobiology and Treatment of OCD: Accelerating Progress (eds NA, Fineberg, TW, Robbins): 125–45. Springer, 2021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albertella, L, Le Pelley, ME, Chamberlain, SR, Westbrook, F, Lee, RS, Fontenelle, LF, et al. Reward-related attentional capture and cognitive inflexibility interact to determine greater severity of compulsivity-related problems. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2020; 69: 101580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billieux, J, Heeren, A, Rochat, L, Maurage, P, Bayard, S, Bet, R, et al. Positive and negative urgency as a single coherent construct: evidence from a large-scale network analysis in clinical and non-clinical samples. J Pers 2021; 89(6): 1252–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyders, MA, Smith, GT, Spillane, NS, Fischer, S, Annus, AM, Peterson, C. Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychol Assess 2007; 19(1): 107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, JM, Latzman, RD, Bliwise, NG, Lilienfeld, SO. Parsing the heterogeneity of impulsivity: a meta-analytic review of the behavioral implications of the UPPS for psychopathology. Psychol Assess 2015; 27(4): 1129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breems, N, Basden, A. Understanding of computers and procrastination: a philosophical approach. Comput Hum Behav 2014; 31: 211–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sümer, C, Büttner, OB. I'll do it - after one more scroll: the effects of boredom proneness, self-control, and impulsivity on online procrastination. Front Psychol 2022; 13: 918306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, S-J, Lan, Y, Wu, L, Yan, W-S. Profiles of impulsivity in problematic internet users and cigarette smokers. Front Psychol 2019; 10: 772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billieux, J, Maurage, P, Lopez-Fernandez, O, Kuss, DJ, Griffiths, MD. Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Curr Addict Rep 2015; 2(2): 156–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canale, N, Moretta, T, Pancani, L, Buodo, G, Vieno, A, Dalmaso, M, et al. A test of the pathway model of problematic smartphone use. J Behav Addict 2021; 10(1): 181–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stieger, M, Flückiger, C, Rüegger, D, Kowatsch, T, Roberts, BW, Allemand, M. Changing personality traits with the help of a digital personality change intervention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2021; 118(8): e2017548118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, EI, Cramer, AO. Moving forward: challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect Psychol Sci 2017; 12(6): 9991020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive information of demographic and study variables

Figure 1

Fig. 1 (a) Network structure of the estimated network. Solid edges represent positive correlations and dotted edges represent negative correlations. The thickness of the edge reflects the magnitude of the correlation. Cut value = 0.03. The text of problematic use of the internet symptoms can be seen in Table 1. (b) Bridge centrality plot. CHIT, compulsivity; NU, negative urgency; IAT, internet addiction test; LoPM, lack of premeditation; LoPers, lack of perseverance; SS, sensation seeking; PU, positive urgency.

Supplementary material: File

Liu et al. supplementary material

Liu et al. supplementary material
Download Liu et al. supplementary material(File)
File 489.9 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.